Skip to main content
Log in

The Costs of Deception: Evidence from Psychology

  • Published:
Experimental Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recently, it has been argued that the evidence in social science research suggests that deceiving participants in an experiment does not lead to a significant loss of experimental control. Based on this assessment, experimental economists were counseled to lift their de facto prohibition against deception to capture its potential benefits. To the extent that this recommendation is derived from empirical studies, we argue that it draws on a selective sample of the available evidence. Building on a systematic review of relevant research in psychology, we present two major results: First, the evidence suggests that the experience of having been deceived generates suspicion that in turn is likely to affect the judgment and decision making of a non-negligible number of participants. Second, we find little evidence for the reputational spillover effects that have been hypothesized by a number of authors in psychology and economics (e.g., Kelman, H.C., 1967. Psychological Bulletin. 67, 1–11; Davis, D.D. and Holt, C.A., 1993. Experimental Economics. Princeton University Press, Princeton). Based on a discussion of the methodological costs and benefits of deception, we conclude that experimental economists' prohibition of deception is a sensible convention that economists should not abandon.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adair, J.G. (1972). “Demand Characteristics or Conformity? Suspiciousness of Deception and Experimenter Bias in Conformity Research.” Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science. 4, 238–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adair, J.G., Dushenko, T.W., and Lindsay, R.C.L. (1985). “Ethical Regulation and Their Impact on Research Practice.” American Psychologist. 40, 59–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aitkenhead, M. and Dordoy, J. (1985). “What the Subjects Have to Say.” British Journal of Social Psychology. 24, 293–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, D.F. (1983). “Follow-up Analysis of Use of Forewarning and Deception in Psychological Experiments.” Psychological Reports. 52, 899–906.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychological Association. (1992). “Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct.” American Psychologist. 47, 1597–1611.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyris, C. (1968). “Some Unintended Consequences of Rigorous Research.” Psychological Bulletin. 70, 185–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S.E. (1956). “Studies of Independence and Conformity: A Minority of one Against a Unanimous Majority.” Psychological Monographs. 70(9), no. 416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bardsley, N. (2000). “Control Without Deception: Individual Behaviour in Free-Riding Experiments Revisited.” Experimental Economics. 3, 215–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1964). “Some Thoughts on Ethics of Research. After Reading Milgram's Behavioral Study of Obedience.” American Psychologist. 19, 421–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1971). “Principles of Ethical Conduct in the Treatment of Subjects: Reaction to the Draft Report of the Committee on Ethical Standards in Psychological Research.” American Psychologist. 26, 887–896.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1985). “Research Using Intentional Deception: Ethical Issues Revisited.” American Psychologist. 40, 165–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berg, J., Dickhaut, J.W., and McCabe, K.A. (1995). “Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History.” Games and Economic Behavior. 10, 122–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L. (1974). “Some Determinants of Impulsive Aggression: Role of Mediated Associations with Reinforcements for Aggression.” Psychological Review. 81, 165–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L. and LePage, L. (1967). “Weapons as Aggression-Eliciting Stimuli.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 7, 202–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonetti, S. (1998a). “Experimental Economics and Deception: Reply.” Journal of Economic Psychology. 19, 411–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonetti, S. (1998b). “Experimental Economics and Deception.” Journal of Economic Psychology. 19, 377–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brock, T.C. and Becker, L.A. (1966). “Debriefing and Susceptibility to Subsequent Experimental Manipulations.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2, 314–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chipman, A. (1966). “Conformity as a Differential Function of Social Pressure and Judgment Difficulty.” The Journal of Social Psychology. 70, 299–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, L. (1977). “The negative Subject: Myth, Reality, or a Prior Experimental Experience Effect?” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 35, 392–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, L. (1988). “Deception in Psychological Research: When is its Use Justified?” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 14, 664–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum. Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T.D., Bean, J.R., Calder, B.J., Frey, R., Krovetz, M.L., and Reisman, S.R. (1970). “Demand Characteristics and Three Conceptions of the Frequently Deceived Subject.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 14, 185–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T.D. and Perrin, B.F. (1971). “The Effects of Suspiciousness of Deception and the Perceived Legitimacy of Deception on Task Performance in an Attitude Change Experiment.” Journal of Personality. 39, 204–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coulter, X. (1986). “Academic Value of Research Participation by Undergraduates.” American Psychologist. 41, 317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darley, J.M. and Latané, B. (1968). “Bystander Intervention in Emergencies: Diffusion of Responsibility.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 8, 377–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, D.D. and Holt, C.A. (1993). Experimental Economics. Princeton University Press.

  • Dickhaut, J., Hubbard, J., and McCabe, K. (1995). “Trust, Reciprocity, and Interpersonal History: Fool Me Once, Shame on You, Fool Me Twice, Shame on Me.” (Working paper). University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, N.S. and Hartley, S. (1973). “Relative Competence, Reinforcement and Conformity.” European Journal of Social Psychology. 3, 63–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, N.S., Wiesenthal, D.L., and Geller, S.H. (1972). “The Generalization of the Effects of Agreement and Correctness on Relative Competence Mediating Conformity.” Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science. 4, 322–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N. and Huff, C. (1998). “Suspicion, Affective Response, and Educational Benefit as a Result of Deception in Psychology Research.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 24, 759–768.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ettinger, R.F., Marino, C.J., Endler, N.S., Geller, S.H., and Natziuk, T. (1971). “The Effects of Agreement and Correctness on Relative Competence and Conformity.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 19, 204–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillenbaum, S. (1966). “Prior Deception and Subsequent Experimental Performance: The 'faithful' subject.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 4, 532–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillenbaum, S. and Frey, R. (1970). “More on the 'Faithful' Behavior of Suspicious Subjects.” Journal of Personality. 38, 43–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finney, P.D. (1987). “When Consent Information Refers to Risk and Deception: Implications for Social Research.” Journal of Social Behavior and Personality. 2, 37–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C.B. and Fyrberg, D. (1994). “Participant Partners: College Students Weigh the Costs and Benefits of Deceptive Research.” American Psychologist. 49, 417–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallo, P.S., Smith, S., and Mumford, S. (1973). “Effects of Deceiving Subjects upon Experimental Results.” The Journal of Social Psychology. 89, 99–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geller, S.H. and Endler, N.S. (1973). “The Effects of Subject Roles, Demand Characteristics, and Suspicion on Conformity.” Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science. 5, 46–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geller, S.H., Endler, N.S., and Wiesenthal, D.L. (1973). “Conformity as a Function of Task Generalization and Relative Competence.” European Journal of Social Psychology. 3, 53–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerdes, E.P. (1987). “College Students' Reactions to Social Psychological Experiments Involving Deception.” Journal of Social Psychology. 107, 99–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glinski, R.J., Glinski, B.C., and Slatin, G.T. (1970). “Nonnaivety Contamination in Conformity Experiments: Sources, Effects, and Implications for Control.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 16, 478–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golding. S.L. and Lichtenstein, E. (1970). “Confession of Awareness and Prior Knowledge of Deception as a Function of Interview Set and Approval Motivation.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 14, 213–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, A.E. and Fleming, I. (1982). “Twenty Years of Deception in Social Psychology.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 8, 402–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruder, C.L., Strumpfhauser, and Wyer, R.S. (1977). “Improvement in Experimental Performance as a Result of Debriefing About Deception.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 3, 434–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hausken (1995a). “The Dynamics of Within-Group and Between-Group Interaction.” Journal of Mathematical Economics. 24, 655–687.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hausken, K. (1995b). “Intra-Level and Inter-Level Interaction.” Rationality and Society. 7, 465–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertwig, R. and Ortmann, A. (2001). “Experimental Practices in Economics: A Methodological Challenge for Psychologists?” Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24, 383–451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertwig, R. and Ortmann, A. (2002a). “Economists' and Psychologists' Experimental Practices: How They Differ, Why They Differ, And How they Could Converge.” In I. Brocas and J. Carrillo (eds.), Psychology and Economics Decisions (pp. 253–272). Oxford University Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertwig, R. and Ortmann, A. (2002b). “Deception and Experimental Control.” Manuscript (Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hey, J.D. (1998). “Experimental Economics and Deception.” Journal of Economic Psychology. 19, 397–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higbee, K.L. (1978). “How Credible are Psychological Researchers to College Students?” Journal of Psychology. 99, 129–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., and Smith, V.L. (1996). “Social Distance and Other-Regarding Behavior in Dictator Games.” American Economic Review. 86, 653–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelman, H.C. (1967). “Human Use of Human Subjects: The Problem of Deception in Social Psychology.” Psychological Bulletin. 67, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimmel, A.J. (1996). Ethical Issues in Behavioral Research: A Survey. Blackwell, Publishers.

  • Kimmel, A.J. (1998). “In Defense of Deception.” American Psychologist. 53, 803–805.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreps, D.M. (1990). Game Theory and Economic Modelling. Clarendon Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krupat, E. and Garonzik, R. (1994). “Subjects' Expectations and the Search for Alternatives to Deception in Social Psychology.” British Journal of Social Psychology. 33, 211–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ledyard, J.O. (1995). “Public Goods: A Survey of Experimental Research.” In J. Kagel and A.E. Roth (eds.), Handbook of Experimental Economics (pp. 111–194). Princeton University Press, Princeton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, L. (1967). “Awareness, Learning and the Beneficent Subject as ExpertWitness.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 6, 363–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacCoun, R.J. and Kerr, N.L. (1987). “Suspicion in the Psychological Laboratory: Kelman's Prophecy Revisited.” American Psychologist. 42, 199.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, T. and Starmer, C. (1998). “Experimental Economics and Deception: A Comment.” Journal of Economic Psychology. 19, 403–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S. (1963). “Behavioral Study of Obedience.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 67, 371–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S. (1964). “Issues in the Study of Obedience: A Reply to Baumrind.” American Psychologist. 19, 848–852.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newberry, B.H. (1973). “Truth Telling in Subjects with Information About Experiments: Who is Being Deceived?” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 25, 369–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicks, S.D., Korn, J.H., and Mainieri, T. (1997). “The Rise and Fall of Deception in Social Psychology and Personality Research, 1921 to 1994.” Ethics and Behavior. 7, 69–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliansky, A. (1991). “A Confederate's Perspective on Deception.” Ethics and Behavior. 1, 253–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortmann, A. and Colander, D. (1997). “Teaching Tools: A Simple Principal-Agent Experiment for the Classroom.” Economic Inquiry. 35, 443–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortmann, A., Fitzgerald, J., and Boeing, C. (2000). “Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History: A Re-examination.” Experimental Economics. 3, 81–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortmann, A. and Hertwig, R. (1997). “Is Deception Acceptable?” American Psychologist. 52, 746–747.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortmann, A. and Hertwig, R. (1998). “The Question Remains: Is Deception Acceptable?” American Psychologist. 53, 806–807.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortmann, A. and Tichy, L. (1999). “Understanding Gender Effects in the Laboratory: Evidence from Prisoner's Dilemma Games.” Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization. 39, 327–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Page, M.M. and Scheidt, R.H. (1971). “The Elusive Weapons Effect: Demand Awareness, Evaluation Apprehension, and Slightly Sophisticated Subjects.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 20, 304–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. and Rosnow, R.L. (1991). Essentials of Behavioral Research: Methods and Data Analysis (2nd edn.). McGraw Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosnow, R.L. and Rosenthal, R. (1997). People Studying People: Artifacts and Ethics in Behavioral Research. Freeman, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, Z. (1985). “Deceiving Ourselves About Deception: A Comment on Smith and Richardson's 'Amelioration of Deception and Harm in Psychological Research.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 48, 252–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, Z. and Moore, J.C. (1971). “Assessment of Subjects' Suspicions.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 17, 163–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sagarin, B.J., Rhoads, K.v.L., and Cialdini, R.B. (1998). “Deceiver's Distrust: Denigration as a Consequence of Undiscovered Deception.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 24, 1167–1176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, D.P. (1969). “The Human Subject in Psychological Research.” Psychological Bulletin. 72, 214–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharpe, D., Adair, J.G., and Roese, N.J. (1992). “Twenty Years of Deception Research: A Decline in Subjects' Trust?” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 18, 585–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sieber, J.E., Iannuzzo, R., and Rodriguez, B. (1995). “Deception Methods in Psychology: Have They Changed in 23 Years?” Ethics and Behavior. 5, 67–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sieber, J.E. and Saks, M.J. (1989). “A Census of Subject Pool Characteristics and Policies.” American Psychologist. 44, 1053–1061.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, I., Shulman, A.D., and Wiesenthal, D.L. (1970). “Effects of Deceiving and Debriefing Psychological Subjects on Performance in Later Experiments.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 14, 203–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, L.S. and Turner, C.W. (1976). “Evaluation Apprehension, Hypothesis Awareness, and the Weapons effect.” Aggressive Behavior. 2, 77–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, V.L. (1982). “Microeconomic Systems as an Experimental Science.” American Economic Review. 72, 923–955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S.S. and Richardson, D. (1983). “Amelioration of Deception and Harm in Psychological Research: The Important Role of Debriefing.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 44, 1075–1082.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinner, B., Adair, J.G., and Barnes, G.E. (1977). “A Reexamination of the Faithful Subject Role.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 13, 543–551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stang, D.J. (1976). “Ineffective Deception in Conformity Research: Some Causes and Consequences.” European Journal of Social Psychology. 6, 353–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Straits, B.C., Wuebben, P.L., and Majka, T.J. (1972). “Influences on Subjects' Perceptions of Experimental Research Situations.” Sociometry. 35, 499–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stricker, L. (1967). “The True Deceiver.” Psychological Bulletin. 68, 13–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stricker, L.J., Messick, S., and Jackson, D.N. (1969). “Evaluating Deception in Psychological Research.” Psychological Bulletin. 71, 343–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, K.M. and Shepperd, J.A. (1996). “Probing Suspicion Among Participants in Deception Research.” American Psychologist. 51, 886–887.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toy, D., Olsen, J., and Wright, L. (1989). “Effects of Debriefing in Marketing Research Involving 'Mild' Deceptions.” Psychology and Marketing. 6, 69–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, C.W., and Simons, L.S. (1974). “Effects of Subject Sophistication and Evaluation Apprehension on Aggressive Responses to Weapons.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 30, 341–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, C.W., Simons, L.S., Berkowitz, L., and Frodi, A. (1977). “The Stimulating and Inhibiting Effects of Weapons on Aggressive Behavior.” Aggressive Behavior. 3, 355–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vitelli, R. (1988). “The Crisis Issue Assessed: An Empirical Analysis.” Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 9, 301–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weimann, J. (1994). “Individual Behavior in a Free-Riding Experiment.” Journal of Public Economics. 54, 185–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, R.L. and Erker, P.V. (1986). “The Effects of Prebriefing Misinformed Research Participants on Their Attributions of Responsibility.” Journal of Psychology. 120, 397–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiesenthal, D.L., Endler, N.S., and Geller, S.H. (1973). “Effects of Prior Group Agreement and Task Correctness on Relative Competence Mediating Conformity.” European Journal of Social Psychology. 3, 193–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis, R. and Willis, Y.A. (1970). “Role Playing Versus Deception: An Experimental Comparison.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 16, 472–477.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andreas Ortmann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ortmann, A., Hertwig, R. The Costs of Deception: Evidence from Psychology. Experimental Economics 5, 111–131 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020365204768

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020365204768

Navigation