Skip to main content
Log in

Attraction and Social Coordination: Mutual Entrainment of Vocal Activity Rhythms

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

To investigate factors that affect the mutual entrainment of vocal activity rhythms, female general psychology students paired according to attitude similarity questionnaires engaged in 40-minute introductory conversations. Fourier analyses performed on speakers' on-off vocal activity demonstrated periodic oscillations in talkativeness. Although some dyads coordinated their vocal activity rhythms, speech accommodation was not predicted by attitude similarity or attraction and did not affect ratings of conversation quality. These rhythms of dialogue appear resistant to change, their behavioral momentum rooted perhaps in an underlying chronobiology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Reference

  • Aronsson, K., Jonsson, L., & Linell, P. (1987). The courtroom hearing as middle ground: Speech accommodation by lawyers and defendants. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 6, 99-116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bales, R. F. (1950). Interaction process analysis. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bochner, A. P. (1984). The functions of human communicating in interpersonal bonding. In C. Arnold & J. Bowers (Eds.), Handbook of rhetorical and communication theory (pp. 544-621). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Box, G. E. P., & Jenkins, G. M. (1970). Time series analysis: Forecasting and control. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D. (1961). Interpersonal attraction and attitude similarity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 713-715.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D., & Clore, G. L. (1966). Predicting interpersonal attraction toward strangers presented in three different stimulus modes. Psychonomic Science, 4, 239-240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D., & Clore, G. L. (1970). A reinforcement model of evaluative responses. Personality: An International Journal, 1, 103-128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N., & Palmer, M. T. (1990). Attitude similarity, relational history and attraction: The mediating effects of kinesic and vocal behaviors. Communication Monographs, 57, 161-183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N., & Palnap, S. (1981). Talk and silence sequences in informal conversations III: Interspeaker influence. Human Communication Research, 7, 117-132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapple, E. D. (1970). Culture and biological man: Explorations in behavioral anthropology. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapple, E. D. (1982). Movement and sound: The musical language of body rhythms in interaction. In M. Davis (Ed.), Interactional rhythms (pp. 31-52). New York, NY: Human Sciences Press, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1967). A segmentation of behavior. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 5, 221-235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dabbs, J. M. (1983). Fourier analysis of the rhythm of conversation.(ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 222 959). Paper presented at The Annual Meeting of The American Psychological Association (Washington DC, August 23-27, 1982).

  • Duck, S. W. (1976). Interpersonal communication in developing acquaintance. In G. R. Biller (Ed.), Explorations in interpersonal communication. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H. (1973). Accent mobility: A model and some data. Anthropological Linguistics, 15, 87-105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H., Coupland, N., & Coupland, J. (1991). Accommodation theory: Communication, context, and consequence. In H. Giles, N. Coupland, & J. Coupland (Eds.), Contexts of Accommodation: Developments in Applied Sociolinguistics (pp. 1-68). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M. (1979). Marital Interaction: Experimental Investigations. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hale, J. L., & Burgoon, J. K. (1984). Models of reactions to changes in nonverbal immediacy. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8, 287-314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, D. P., & Cobb, L. (1970). Ultradian rhythms in social interaction. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Of Speech and Time: Temporal Speech Rhythms in Interpersonal Contexts. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development No. 265, Vol. 66(2), vi-131.

  • Jaffe, J., & Feldstein, S. (1970). Rhythms of dialogue. New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M. (1981). Time-series analysis: A comprehensive introduction for social scientists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimberly, R. P. (1970). Patterns in human interaction. Nature, 228, 88-90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matarazzo, J. D., Weins, A. N., Matarazzo, R. G., & Saslow, G. (1968). Speech and silence behavior in clinical psychotherapy and its laboratory correlates. In J. Schlier, H. Hunt, J. D. Matarazzo, & C. Savage (Eds.). Research in psychotherapy (pp. 347-394). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauer, R. E., & Tindall, J. H. (1983). Effects of postural congruence on client's perceptions of counselor empathy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 30, 158-163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natale, M. (1975). Convergence of mean vocal intensity in dyadic communications as a function of social desirability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 790-804.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A., & Grace, R. C. (2000). Behavioral momentum and the law of effect. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 73-130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, T. M. (1956). The prediction of interpersonal attraction. Psychological Review, 60, 393-404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, T. M. (1961). The acquaintance process. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, K., & Lee, A. (1903). On the laws of inheritance in man. I. Inheritance of physical characters. Biometrika, 2, 357-462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, R. J. L. (1985). Significance tables for the results of Fast Fourier transforms. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 38, 116-119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Street, R. L., Jr. (1983). Noncontent speech convergence in adult-child interactions. In R. N. Bostrom (Ed.), Communication yearbook, Vol. 7 (pp. 369-395). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. M. (1979). Periodic rhythms in conversational speech. Language and Speech, 22, 381-389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. M. (1992a). Dimensions of social interaction tempo: A factor analytic study of time and frequency domain indexes of interaction structure. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 21, 173-191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. M. (1992b). Sequential analysis of social interaction: Assessing internal versus social determinants of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 51-60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. M. (1992c). Cyclicity of vocal activity increases during conversation: Support for a nonlinear systems model of dyadic social interaction. Behavioral Science, 37, 128-138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. M., Kenny, D. A., & Stoto, M. (1979). A new round robin analysis of variance for social interaction data. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1742-1757.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. M., Malloy, D., Schneider, K., Knoth, R., & Wilder, B. (1987). Rhythmic organization of social interaction and observer ratings of positive affect and involvement. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 57-74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. M., & Mooney, K. (1988). Individual differences in vocal activity rhythm: Fourier analysis of cyclicity in amount of talk. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 17, 99-111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. M., Waggener, T. B., & Kronauer, R. E. (1983). Synchronized cycles in ventilation and vocal activity during spontaneous conversational speech. Journal of Applied Physiology: Respiratory, Environmental and Exercise Physiology, 54, 1324-1334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welkowitz, J., & Feldstein, S. (1969). Dyadic interaction and induced differences in perceived similarity. Proceedings of the 77th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 4, 343-344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welkowitz, J., Feldstein, S., Finkelstein, M., & Aylesworth, L. (1972). Changes in vocal intensity as a function of interspeaker influence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 35, 715-718.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wever, R. A. (1979). The circadian systems of man: Results of experiments under temporal isolation. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wever, R. A. (1982). Behavioral aspects of circadian rhythmicity. In B. F. Brown & R. C. Graeber (Eds.), Rhythmic aspects of behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McGarva, A.R., Warner, R.M. Attraction and Social Coordination: Mutual Entrainment of Vocal Activity Rhythms. J Psycholinguist Res 32, 335–354 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023547703110

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023547703110

Navigation