Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Opinion
  • Published:

Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: a transcultural neuroimaging approach

Abstract

Our brains and minds are shaped by our experiences, which mainly occur in the context of the culture in which we develop and live. Although psychologists have provided abundant evidence for diversity of human cognition and behaviour across cultures, the question of whether the neural correlates of human cognition are also culture-dependent is often not considered by neuroscientists. However, recent transcultural neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that one's cultural background can influence the neural activity that underlies both high- and low-level cognitive functions. The findings provide a novel approach by which to distinguish culture-sensitive from culture-invariant neural mechanisms of human cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Rent or buy this article

Prices vary by article type

from$1.95

to$39.95

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Cultural modulation of the neural activity of the attentional network.
Figure 2: Cultural influence on the neural substrates of self representation and self awareness.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Markus, H. R., Kitayama, S. & Heiman, R. J. in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (eds Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W) 857–913 (Guilford, New York, 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. Culture, self, and the reality of the social. Psychol. Inq. 14, 277–283 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E. & Norenzayan, A. Causal attribution across cultures: variation and universality. Psychol. Bull. 125, 47–63 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  5. Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I. & Norenzayan, A. Culture and systems of thought: holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychol. Rev. 108, 291–310 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Nisbett, R. E. & Masuda, T. Culture and point of view. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 11164–11170 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  7. Nisbett, R. E. The Geography of Thought (Free Press, New York, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Chiu, C. Y. & Hong, Y. Y. Social Psychology of Culture (Psychology Press, New York, 2006).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Morris, M. & Peng, K. Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 949–971 (1994).

    Google Scholar 

  10. Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. Am. Psychol. 54, 741–754 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  11. Cacioppo, J. T. Social neuroscience: understanding the pieces fosters understanding the whole and vice versa. Am. Psychol. 57, 819–831 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ochsner, K. N. & Lieberman, M. D. The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. Am. Psychol. 56, 717–734 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Adolphs, R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165–178 (2003).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Blakemore, S., Winston, J. & Frith, U. Social cognitive neuroscience: where are we heading? Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 216–222 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Lieberman, M. D. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 58, 259–289 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  16. Gutchess, A. H., Welsh, R. C., Boduroglu, A. & Park, D. C. Cultural differences in neural function associated with object processing. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 6, 102–109 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Tang, Y. et al. Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by cultures. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 10775–10780 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Zhu, Y., Zhang, Li., Fan, J. & Han, S. Neural basis of cultural influence on self representation. Neuroimage 34, 1310–1317 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R. & Gabrieli, D. E. Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychol. Sci. 19, 12–17 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Sui, J. & Han, S. Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychol. Sci. 18, 861–866 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Lin, Z., Lin., Y. & Han, S. Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception. Biol. Psychol. 77, 93–97 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Han, S. et al. Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 1–15 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Nisbett, R. E. & Miyamoto, Y. The influence of culture: holistic versus analytic perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 467–473 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Ji, L., Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. E. Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 943–955 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kühnen, U. & Oyserman, D. Thinking about the self influences thinking in general: cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38, 492–499 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  26. Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T. & Larsen, J. T. Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: a cultural look at new look. Psychol. Sci. 14, 201–206 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Masuda, T. & Nisbett, R. E. Culture and change blindness. Cogn. Sci. 30, 381–399 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Masuda, T. & Nisbett, R. E. Attending holistically versus analytically: comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 922–934 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G. & Haxby, J. V. Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature 379, 649–652 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Goh, J. O. et al. Age and culture modulate object processing and object-scene binding in the ventral visual area. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 44–52 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Lin, Z. & Han, S. Self-construal priming modulates the scope of visual attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A (in the press).

  32. Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S. & Lee, A. Y. “I” value freedom, but “we” value relationships: self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychol. Sci. 10, 321–326 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  33. Navon, D. Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353–383 (1977).

    Google Scholar 

  34. Heinze, H. J. et al. Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature 372, 543–546 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Di Russo, F., Martinez, A. & Hillyard, S. A. Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. Cereb. Cortex 13, 486–499 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Milham, M. P., Banich, M. T., Claus, E. D. & Cohen, N. J. Practice-related effects demonstrate complementary roles of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortices in attentional control. Neuroimage 18, 483–493 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Bolger, D. J., Perfetti, C. A. & Schneider, W. Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: universal structures plus writing system variation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 92–104 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Tan, L. H., Laird, A., Li, K. & Fox, P. T. Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 83–91 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Paulesu, E. et al. A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neurosci. 3, 91–96 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Shaywitz, S. E. et al. Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 2636–2641 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Temple, E. et al. Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: evidence from functional MRI. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 2860–2865 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z. & Tan, L. H. Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature 431, 71–76 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Hoeft, F. et al. Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 4234–4239 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Siok, W. T., Niu, Z., Jin, Z., Perfetti, C. A. & Tan, L. H. A structural-functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 5561–5566 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Morrison, S. J., Demorest, S. M., Aylward, E. H., Cramer, S. C. & Maravilla, K. R. fMRI investigation of cross-cultural music comprehension. Neuroimage 20, 378–384 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Nan, Y., Knösche, T. R., Zysset, S. & Friederici, A. D. Cross-cultural music phrase processing: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 312–328 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Nan, Y., Knösche, T. R. & Friederici, A. D. The perception of musical phrase structure: a cross-cultural ERP study. Brain Res. 1094, 179–191 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Ansari, D. Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 9, 278–291 (2008).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Baddeley, A. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 417–423 (2000).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Markham, R. & Wang, L. Recognition of emotion by Chinese and Australian children. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 27, 616–643 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  51. Elfenbein, H. A. & Ambady, N. On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 128, 243–249 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Chiao, J. Y. et al. Cultural specificity in amygdala response to fear faces. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5 May 2008 (doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20151).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Wellman, H. M. The Child's Theory of Mind (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1990).

    Google Scholar 

  54. Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. Interacting minds — a biological basis. Science 286, 1692–1695 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Frith, C. & Frith, U. Theory of mind. Curr. Biol. 15, R644–R646 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Fletcher, P. C. et al. Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of 'theory of mind' in story comprehension. Cognition 57, 109–128 (1995).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Abu-Akel, A. A neurobiological mapping of theory of mind. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 43, 29–40 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Gallagher, H. L. & Frith, C. D. Functional imaging of 'theory of mind'. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 77–83 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Saxe, R., Carey, S. & Kanwisher, N. Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging Ann. Rev. Psychol. 55, 87–124 (2004).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Frith, U. & Frith, C. D. Development and neurophysiology and mentalizing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 459–473 (2003).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50, 531–534 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Baylé, M. & Decety, J. A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. Neuroimage 11, 157–166 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Gallagher, H. L. et al. Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of 'theory of mind' in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 38, 11–21 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Han, S., Jiang, Y., Humphreys, G. W., Zhou, T. & Cai, P. Distinct neural substrates for the perception of real and virtual visual worlds. Neuroimage 24, 928–935 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Saxe, R. & Kanwisher, N. People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. Neuroimage 19, 1835–1842 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Saxe, R. & Wexler, A. Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 43, 1391–1399 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Sommer, M. et al. Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning. Neuroimage 35, 1378–1384 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Amodio, D. M. & Frith, C. D. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 7, 268–277 (2006).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  69. Kobayashi, C., Glover, G. H. & Temple, E. Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of 'theory of mind': an fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals. Brain Lang. 98, 210–220 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Allman, J. M., Watson, K. K., Tetreault, N. A. & Hakeem, A. Y. Intuition and autism: a possible role for Von Economo neurons. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 367–373 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Gorno-Tempini, M. L. et al. Explicit and incidental facial expression processing: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 1, 465–473 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  72. Moran, M. A., Mufson, E. J. & Mesulam, M. M. Neural inputs into the temporopolar cortex of the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 256, 88–103 (1987).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E. & Grafman, J. Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. Neuroimage 16, 696–703 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Kobayashi, C., Glover, G. H. & Temple, E. Cultural and linguistic effects on neural bases of 'theory of mind' in American and Japanese children. Brain Res. 1164, 95–107 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Decety, J. & Grèzes, J. The power of simulation: imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Res. 1079, 4–14 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Decety, J. & Lamm, C. The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: how low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist 13, 580–593 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Ames, D. R. et al. in Intentions and Intentionality (eds Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J. & Baldwin, D. A.) 208–329 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2001).

    Google Scholar 

  78. Perner, J. & Lang, B. Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 337–344 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. & Watson, J. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev. 72, 655–684 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H. & Norasakkunkit, V. Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 1245–1267 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A. & Kirker, W. S. Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 35, 677–688 (1977).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Craik, F. I. M. et al. In search of the self: a positron emission tomography study. Psychol. Sci. 10, 26–34 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  83. Fossati, P. et al. In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1938–1945 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Heatherton, T. F. et al. Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1, 18–25 (2006).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Kelley, W. M. et al. Finding the self? An event-related fMRI Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 785–794 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Lieberman, M. D., Jarcho, J. M. & Satpute, A. B. Evidence-based and intuition-based self-knowledge: an fMRI study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 421–435 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Macrae, C. N., Moran, J. M., Heatherton, T. F., Banfield, J. F. & Kelley, W. M. Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cereb. Cortex 14, 647–654 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Moran, J. M., Macrae, C. N., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L. & Kelley, W. M. Neuroanatomical evidence for distinct cognitive and affective components of self. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1586–1594 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. D'argembeau, A. et al. Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study. Neuroimage 25, 616–624 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Northoff, G. & Bermpohl, F. Cortical midline structures and processing of the self. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 102–107 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Northoff, G., DeGreck, M., Bermpohl, F. & Panksepp, J. Self-referential processing in our brain – a metaanalysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31, 440–457 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Zhang, L. et al. In search of the Chinese self: an fMRI study. Sci. China C Life Sci. 49, 89–96 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Burns, C. “Soul-less” Christianity and the Buddhist empirical self: Buddhist-Christian convergence? Budd.-Christ. Stud. 23, 87–100 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  94. Ching, J. Paradigms of the self in Buddhism and Christianity. Budd.-Christ. Stud. 4, 31–50 (1984).

    Google Scholar 

  95. Lin, H. in Dialogue Between Budhism and Christianity (eds Wu, Y., Lai, P. & Wang, W.) 317–338 (Zhong Hua Book Company, Beijing, 2005).

    Google Scholar 

  96. Platek, S. M., Keenan, J. P., Gallup, G. G. & Mohamed, F. B. Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. Cogn. Brain Res. 19, 114–122 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  97. Platek, S. M. et al. Neural substrates for functionally discriminating self-face from personally familiar faces. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 91–98 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Uddin, L. Q., Kaplan, J. T., Molnar-Szakaca, I., Zaidel, E. & Iacoboni, M. Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal “mirror” network in the right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 25, 926–935 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Sugiura, M. et al. Passive and active recognition of one's own face. Neuroimage 11, 36–48 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Sugiura, M. et al. Cortical mechanisms of visual self-recognition. Neuroimage 24, 143–49 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Northoff, G. Philosophy of the Brain. The Brain Problem (Benjamin, Amsterdam, 2004)

    Google Scholar 

  102. Pi, E. H. Transcultural psychopharmacology: present and future. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 52, S185–S187 (1998).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Munoz, C. & Hilgenberg, C. Ethnopharmacology. Am. J. Nurs. 105, 40–48 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Nisbett, R. E. & Norenzayan, A. in Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology: Cognition Vol. 2 (eds Pashler, H. & Medin, D. L.) 561–597 (Wiley, New York, 2002).

    Google Scholar 

  105. Wu, S. & Keysar, B. The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychol. Sci. 18, 600–606 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Wang, Q. & Conway, M. A. The stories we keep: autobiographical memory in American and Chinese middle-aged adults. J. Pers. 72, 911–938 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Klein, S. B., Loftus, J. & Burton, H. A. Two self-reference effects: the importance of distinguishing between self-descriptiveness judgments and autobiographical retrieval in self-referent encoding. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56, 853–865 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  108. Zhu, Y. & Zhang, L. An experimental study on the self-reference effect. Sci. China C Life Sci. 45, 120–128 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Choi, I. & Nisbett, R. E. Situational salience and cultural differences in the correspondence bias and actor-observer bias. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 24, 949–960 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  110. Norenzayan, A., Choi, I. & Nisbett, R. E. Cultural similarities and differences in social inference: evidence from behavioral predictions and lay theories of behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 109–120 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  111. Peng, K. & Knowles, E. Culture, education, and the attribution of physical causality. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1272–1284 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Ji, L., Zhang, Z. & Nisbett, R. E. Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 57–65 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Singer, T. et al. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439, 466–469 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  114. Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D. & Williams, K. D. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290–292 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Park, D. C. & Gutchess, A. H. Aging, cognition, and culture: a neuroscientific perspective. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 859–867 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Rushton, J. P. & Ankney, C. D. Brain size and cognitive ability: correlations with age, sex, social class, and race. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3, 21–36 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Project 30630025) (S.H.), the Salus Foundation (G.N.) and the Hope of Depression Research Foundation (G.N.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Related links

Related links

FURTHER INFORMATION

Shihui Han's homepage

Georg Northoff's homepage

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Han, S., Northoff, G. Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: a transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 646–654 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2456

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2456

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing