skip to main content
research-article

Using direct and indirect input devices: Attention demands and age-related differences

Published:23 April 2009Publication History
Skip Abstract Section

Abstract

Researchers have suggested that attention is a key moderating variable predicting performance with an input device [Greenstein and Arnaut 1988], although the attention demands of devices have not been directly investigated. We hypothesized that the attentional demands of input devices are intricately linked to whether the device matches the input requirements of the on-screen task. Further, matching task and device should be more important for attentionally reduced groups, such as older adults. Younger and older adults used either a direct (touch screen) or indirect (rotary encoder) input device to perform matched or mismatched input tasks under a spectrum of attention allocation conditions. Input devices required attention—more so for older adults, especially in a mismatch situation. In addition, task performance was influenced by the match between task demands and input device characteristics. Though both groups benefited from a match between input device and task input requirements, older adults benefited more, and this benefit increased as less attention was available. We offer an a priori method to choose an input device for a task by considering the overlap between device attributes and input requirements. This data should affect design decisions concerning input device selection across age groups and task contexts.

References

  1. Albert, A. E. 1982. The effect of graphic input devices on performance in a cursor positioning task. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergonomics Soc. 26, 54--58.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  2. Bekker, M. M., Van Nes, F. L., and Juola, J. F. 1995. A comparison of mouse and speech input control of a text-annotation system. Behav. Inform. Technol. 14, 14--22.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  3. Card, S. K., MacKinlay, J. D., and Robertson, G. C. 1990. The design space of input devices. In Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. 117--124. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  4. Charness, N., Holley, P., Feddon, J., and Jastrzembski, T. 2005. Light pen use and practice minimize age and hand performance differences in pointing tasks. Human Factors 46, 373--384.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  5. Chipman, L. E., Bederson, B. B. and Golbeck, J. 2004. SlideBar: Analysis of a linear input device. Behav. Inform. Technol. 23, 1--9. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  6. English, W. K., Engelbart, D. C., and Berman, M. L. 1967. Display-selection techniques for text manipulation. IEEE Trans. Hum. Factors Electron. 1, 5--15.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  7. Fisk, A. D., Rogers, W. A., Czaja, S. J., Charness, N., and Sharit, J. 2004. Designing for Older Adults: Principles and Creative Human Factors Approaches. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  8. Foley, J. D., Wallace, V. L., and Chan, P. 1984. The human factors of computer graphics interaction techniques. IEEE Comput. Graph. Appl. 4, 13--48. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  9. Gokturk, M. and Siebert, J. L. 1999. An analysis of the index finger as a pointing device. In Proceedings of the CHI Conference. 286--287. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  10. Gopher, D. 1993. The skill of attention control: Acquisition and execution of attention strategies. In Attention and Performance XIV, D.E. Meyer and S. Kornblum Eds., MIT Press. Cambridge, MA, 299--322. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  11. Greenstein, J. S., and Arnaut, L. Y. 1988. Input devices. In The Handbook of Human-Computer Interaction, M. Helander Ed., Elsevier, Amsterdam, 495--516.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  12. Hancock, P. A. 1996. Effects of control order, augmented feedback, input device and practice on tracking performance and perceived workload. Ergonomics 39, 1146--1162.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  13. Jacob, R. J. K. 1996. Human-computer interaction: Input devices. ACM Comput. Surv. 28, 177--179. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  14. Jacob, R. J. K., Sibert, L. E., McFarlane, D. C., and Mullen, M. P. 1994. Integrality and separability of input devices. ACM Trans. Comput.-Hum. Interact. 1, 3--26. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  15. Jastrzembski, T., Charness, N., Holley, P., and Feddon, J. 2005. Input devices for web browsing: age and hand effects. Universal Access Inform. Soc. J. 4, 39--45. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  16. Juul-Kristensen B., Laursen B., Pilegaard M., and Jensen B. R. 2004. Physical workload during use of speech recognition and traditional computer input devices. Ergonomics 47, 119--133.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  17. Karat, J., Mcdonald, J. E., and Anderson, M. P. 1986. A comparison of menu selection techniques: touch panel, mouse and keyboard. Int. J. Man-Machine Stud. 25, 73--88. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  18. Kerr, B. 1973. Processing demands during mental operations. Memory Cognition 1, 401--412.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  19. Korteling, J. E. 1994. Effects of aging, skill modification, and demand alternation on multiple-task performance. Human Factors 36, 27--43.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  20. Kramer, A. F., Larish, J. F., and Strayer, D. L. 1995. Training for attentional control in dual task settings: A comparison of young and old adults. J. Exper. Psych. Applied 1, 50--76.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  21. Madden, D. J. 1986. Adult age differences in the attentional capacity demands of visual search. Cognitive Devel. 1, 335--363.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  22. Martin, T. A. and Allan, W. E. 1991. An evaluation of touch-screen input for a HyperCard based digit-span task.Behav. Resear. Meth. Instrum. Comput. 23, 253--255.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  23. McDowd, J. M. and Shaw, R. J. 2000. Attention and aging: A functional perspective. In The Handbook of Aging and Cognition 2nd Ed., Lawrence Erlbaum. Mahwah, NJ, 221--292Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  24. McLaughlin, A. C., Rogers, W. A., and Fisk, A. D. 2003. Effects of attentional demand on input device use in younger and older adults. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergonomics Soc. 25, 247--250.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  25. Meyer, S., Cohen, O., and Nilsen, E. 1994. Device comparisons for goal-directed drawing tasks. In Proceedings of the CHI Conference, 251--252. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  26. Murata, A. and Iwase, H. 2005. Usability of touch-panel interfaces for older adults. Human Factors 47, 766--776.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  27. Navon, D. and Gopher, D. 1979. On the economy of the human-processing system. Psych. Rev. 86, 214--255.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  28. Norman, D. A. and Bobrow, D. G. 1975. On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive Psych. 7, 44--64.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  29. Oldfield, R. C. 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97--113.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  30. Park, D. C., Smith, A. D., Dudley, W. N., and Lafronza, V. N. 1989. Effects of age and a divided attention task presented during encoding and retrieval on memory. J. Exper. Psych. Learn. Memory Cognition 15, 1185--1191.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  31. Pashler, H. and Johnston, J. C. 1998. Attentional limitations in dual-task performance. In Attention, H. Pashler Ed., Psychology Press/Erlbaum. Hove, England, 155--189.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  32. Ponds, R. W., Brouwer, W. H., and Van Wolffelaar, P. C. 1988. Age differences in divided attention in a simulated driving task. J. Gerontology 43, 151--156.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  33. Posner, M. I. 1978. Chronometric Explorations of Mind. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  34. Reinvang, I. 1998. Validation of reaction time in continuous performance tasks as an index of attention by electro-physiological measures. J. Clinical Exper. Neuropsych. 20, 885--897.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  35. Rogers, W. A. and Fisk, A. D. 2001. Understanding the role of attention in cognitive aging research. In Handbook of the Psychology of Aging 5th Ed., J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie Eds., Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 267--287.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  36. Rogers, W. A., Fisk, A. D., McLaughlin, A. C., and Pak, R. 2005. Touch a screen or turn a knob: Choosing the best device for the job. Human Factors 47, 271--288.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  37. Rogers, W. A., Hertzog, C., and Fisk, A. D. 2002. An individual differences analysis of ability and strategy influences: Age-related differences in associative learning. J. Exper. Psych. Learn. Memory Cognition 26, 359--394.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  38. Scheneider, W. and Chein, J. M., 2003. Controlled & automatic processing: behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cognitive Science 27, 525--559.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  39. Schneider, W. and Fisk, A. D. 1982. Degree of consistent training: improvements in search performance and automatic process development. Perception Psychophysics 31, 160--168.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  40. Shiffrin, R. M. 1988. Attention. In Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology 2nd Ed., R.C. Atkinson et al. Eds., Wiley, New York, 739--811.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  41. Shiffrin, R. M. and Schneider, W. 1977. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psych. Rev. 84, 127--190.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  42. Shipley, W. 1940. Shipley Institute of Living Scale. Western Psychological Press, Los Angeles, CA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  43. Tsang, P. S. and Shaner, T. L. 1998. Age, attention, expertise, and time-sharing performance. Psych. Aging 13, 323--347.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  44. Valk, A. M. 1985. An experiment to study touch screen “button” design. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergonomics Soc. 29, 127--131.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  45. Walker, N., Philbin, D. A., and Fisk, A. D. 1997. Age-related differences in movement control: Adjusting sub-movement structure to optimize performance. J. Gerontology: Psych. Sciences 52, 40--52.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  46. Wechsler, D. 1997. Wechsler Memory Scale III. 3rd Ed. Psychological Corp., San Antonio, TX.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  47. Wickens, C. D. 1980. The structure of attentional resources. In Attention and Performance VIII, R.S. Nickerson Ed., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 239--257.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  48. Wickens, C. D. 1984. Processing resources in attention. In Varieties of Attention, R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies Eds., Academic Press. New York, 63--98.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar

Index Terms

  1. Using direct and indirect input devices: Attention demands and age-related differences

                Recommendations

                Comments

                Login options

                Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

                Sign in

                Full Access

                • Published in

                  cover image ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction
                  ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction  Volume 16, Issue 1
                  April 2009
                  199 pages
                  ISSN:1073-0516
                  EISSN:1557-7325
                  DOI:10.1145/1502800
                  Issue’s Table of Contents

                  Copyright © 2009 ACM

                  Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

                  Publisher

                  Association for Computing Machinery

                  New York, NY, United States

                  Publication History

                  • Published: 23 April 2009
                  • Revised: 1 August 2008
                  • Accepted: 1 August 2008
                  • Received: 1 December 2007
                  Published in tochi Volume 16, Issue 1

                  Permissions

                  Request permissions about this article.

                  Request Permissions

                  Check for updates

                  Qualifiers

                  • research-article
                  • Research
                  • Refereed

                PDF Format

                View or Download as a PDF file.

                PDF

                eReader

                View online with eReader.

                eReader