Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Spatiotemporal Dynamics of High-Gamma Activities during a 3-Stimulus Visual Oddball Task

  • Yoritaka Akimoto ,

    y-akimoto@idac.tohoku.ac.jp

    Affiliation Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan

  • Akitake Kanno,

    Affiliation Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan

  • Toshimune Kambara,

    Affiliation Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Kanagawa, Japan

  • Takayuki Nozawa,

    Affiliation Smart Ageing International Research Center, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan

  • Motoaki Sugiura,

    Affiliations Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan, International Research Institute of Disaster Science, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan

  • Eiichi Okumura,

    Affiliation Department of Epileptology, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, Sendai, Japan

  • Ryuta Kawashima

    Affiliations Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan, Smart Ageing International Research Center, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan, Division of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan

Abstract

Although many studies have investigated the neural basis of top-down and bottom-up attention, it still requires refinement in both temporal and spatial terms. We used magnetoencephalography to investigate the spatiotemporal dynamics of high-gamma (52–100 Hz) activities during top-down and bottom-up visual attentional processes, aiming to extend the findings from functional magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potential studies. Fourteen participants performed a 3-stimulus visual oddball task, in which both infrequent non-target and target stimuli were presented. We identified high-gamma event-related synchronization in the left middle frontal gyrus, the left intraparietal sulcus, the left thalamus, and the visual areas in different time windows for the target and non-target conditions. We also found elevated imaginary coherence between the left intraparietal sulcus and the right middle frontal gyrus in the high-gamma band from 300 to 400 ms in the target condition, and between the left thalamus and the left middle frontal gyrus in theta band from 150 to 450 ms. In addition, the strength of high-gamma imaginary coherence between the left middle frontal gyrus and left intraparietal sulcus, between the left middle frontal gyrus and the right middle frontal gyrus, and the high-gamma power in the left thalamus predicted inter-subject variation in target detection response time. This source-level electrophysiological evidence enriches our understanding of bi-directional attention processes: stimulus-driven bottom-up attention orientation to a salient, but irrelevant stimulus; and top-down allocation of attentional resources to stimulus evaluation.

Introduction

Attention is important for various cognitive functions, such as selection of visual information and attentional resource allocation. The oddball paradigm, wherein stimuli are presented with different probabilities, has been frequently used to study the mechanism of attention. The 3-stimulus oddball paradigm includes two different infrequent stimuli, namely, the target and the infrequent non-target in addition to one frequent stimulus. It has been hypothesized that the processing of the target stimulus entails top-down allocation of attentional resources for stimulus evaluation [1], [2], while the processing of the infrequent non-target stimulus involves stimulus-driven bottom-up attentional orientation to a salient but irrelevant stimulus [3], [4]. Previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies revealed the involvement of the occipital regions in the processing of infrequent non-target stimuli [4], [5], and the involvement of the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and thalamus during target detection [5][7]. These regions are involved in the cortical circuits that guide bottom-up and top-down attention [8]. A previous fMRI study also revealed that functional connectivity within attention network is linked to behavioral performance, i.e., reaction time [9].

Although fMRI has excellent spatial resolution, it has limited time resolution, due to the slow hemodynamic response. On the other hand, electrophysiological measurements, such as electroencephalogram (EEG) or magnetoencephalography (MEG) provide very good temporal resolution. It is well known that infrequent stimuli in the oddball paradigm elicit the P300 event-related potential (ERP), which contains at least two distinguishable subcomponents, namely, P3a and P3b. P3b is elicited by infrequent target stimuli, typically 300 to 600 ms after the stimulus onset [10]. P3a is elicited by infrequent non-target stimuli, and often occurs more quickly than P3b [11]. In addition to ERP, event-related synchronization (ERS), especially in the gamma band, is also important for attention [12]. Gamma ERS shows a close spatial correspondence with fMRI activations [13], and is believed to reflect neuronal processing or local encoding [14], [15]. Attention modulates gamma ERS in the early visual areas [16] and the gamma ERS in the visual cortex predicts the speed of change detection [17]. Long-range gamma band phase synchronization has been also reported for various cognitive tasks [18], [19] and is believed to reflect the neuronal communication that subserves various cognitive functions [20].

Gamma ERS also provides temporal information on time resolution comparable to that of ERP. In addition, focusing on the rhythmic activity enables assessing functional connectivity between distant brain regions. Therefore, it is expected that investigating the gamma activity will link the findings from fMRI and ERP studies and extend our understanding about the neural basis of top-down and bottom-up attention. There are several studies reporting gamma ERS elicited during oddball tasks [21][27]. Lee et al. determined that the generator of EEG high-gamma (65–85 Hz) ERS is localized in the MFG [27]. In addition, gamma band coherence among anterior and posterior electrodes was reported in EEG channel-level studies [28], [29]. However, understanding of the temporospatial brain activity is still incomplete, and moreover, source-level electrophysiological functional connectivity during the oddball task has not yet been reported. This is worth studying because it enables us to test whether the strength of electrophysiological functional connectivity within the attention network increases during the oddball task, and if so, whether this increase predicts behavioral performance.

The purpose of this study was to investigate the temporospatial dynamics of electrophysiological brain activity associated with top-down and bottom-up attentional processes. We focused on the high-gamma activities (52–100 Hz), because it appears to have more specific timing and localization than those of lower frequency [30], [31]. This study complements earlier fMRI studies by providing information about the temporal properties of brain activities. We used MEG with dual-state adaptive spatial filtering [32] and imaginary coherence analysis in source-space [33]. The dual-state adaptive spatial filtering technique results in increased fidelity of higher frequency source reconstruction, as compared to the traditional adaptive spatial filter technique. In the traditional filter technique, weights are computed from unfiltered or wideband data and are therefore inherently biased toward resolving low-frequency brain activity [32]. Imaginary coherence analysis is one of the functional connectivity analyses and can remove spurious coherence caused by non-interacting sources [34], [35]. We also examined the coherence of theta band activity because theta band oscillation plays a role in attentional processing [36] and modulates gamma activity in a phasic manner [37]. Theta activity is also reported to increase together with P300 during the oddball paradigm [38].

Materials and Methods

Participants

Fourteen healthy, right-handed participants (9 males, 5 females) took part in this study. The mean age of the subjects was 21 years (age range 18–25 years). In accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (1991), written informed consent was obtained from each subject. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine. Handedness was evaluated using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [39]. All subjects had normal or corrected to normal vision, and none had a history of neurological or psychiatric disease.

Three-stimulus Oddball Task

We used a 3-stimullus oddball task, using the same stimulus as Walhovd et al. [40] (Fig. 1). In this task, target, non-target, and standard stimuli were presented with an appearance rate of 10%, 10%, and 80%, respectively. Each stimulus was presented for 500 ms. Inter-stimulus intervals were 1500 ms. There were 260 trials in all. Subjects pushed a button with their right index finger only when the target stimulus appeared. The standard stimuli were blue elliptic shapes, the height of which subtended a visual angle of 4.6° and the width of which subtended a visual angel of 4.1°. Target stimuli were larger blue elliptic shapes, the height of which subtended a visual angle of 5.6° and the width of which subtended a visual angle of 4.6°. The infrequent non-target stimuli were large blue rectangles, the height of which subtended a visual angle of 6.4° and the width of which subtended a visual angle of 5.6°. The field of vision angle was considered to be central eye field to prevent eye movement. Viewing distance was 35 cm.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Experimental paradigm of 3-stimulus oddball task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059969.g001

DATA Acquisition

MEG data were acquired using a whole-head 200-channel MEG system (PQA160C, Yokogawa Electric Corporation, Japan) with a sample rate of 1000 Hz and with a low-pass 100 Hz filter. The head shape of each participant was digitized using a 3D digitizer (FastSCAN Cobra, Polhemus Inc., USA) and co-registered with individual structural MR images. Structural MR images were acquired using a 3T MR system (Achieva, Philips, Netherlands).

Preprocessing

The Fieldtrip software package (http://fieldtrip.fcdonders.nl) was used for independent component analysis (ICA) of MEG data and typical noise components (e.g., eye-blink) were removed by a researcher based on visual inspection. The average number of the remaining ICs were 195.9 (SE = 0.7). The data were reconstituted from the ICs remaining after the removal of the artifactual ICs. We applied the array-gain-constraint version of diagonal loading (Tikhonov regularization; see Supplementary of Ueno et al. (2012) [41]) to all source reconstructions in this study. After this preprocessing, the data were band-pass filtered in the 52–100 Hz frequency domain. Trials were epoched from 300 ms before stimulus onset to 600 ms after stimulus onset. Trials with incorrect responses or magnetic flux in excess of 2000 fT in any channel were excluded. The number of analyzed trials was nearly equalized among conditions in order to produce similar noise levels across conditions [42]. This was done by randomly selecting the same number of standard trials and non-target trials for analysis.

Localizing 3-stimulus Oddball Task-relevant High-gamma Activity

MEG data were analyzed using dual-state adaptive spatial filtering that was optimized for time-frequency source reconstructions from MEG or EEG data [32]. The entire brain was covered by virtual sensors with 5 mm voxels in MNI space. Functional images were created by calculation of the power increase relative to the baseline using a logarithmic conversion. The baseline period was defined as the time between −100 to 0 ms of the stimulus onset, and the active period was defined as the period between stimulus onset (0 ms) and 600 ms after stimulus onset. The active period was divided into 100 ms time windows. Images were normalized with a standard T1 template image and spatially smoothed with a 10 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK).

Group analysis was conducted using SPM8. In order to avoid any biases in the selection of ROIs for analysis, we evaluated the task relevant regions of the 3-stimuli oddball task using F-contrast that included two factors. These factors were stimulus (target, non-target, and standard) and time window (0–100 ms, 100–200 ms, 200–300 ms, 300–400 ms, 400–500 ms, and 500–600 ms). This F-contrast tests whether any stimulus in any time-windows is different from zero. The statistical threshold at the voxel level was set at P<0.05 (FWE, correcting for spatial dimension at the voxel level). The peak voxels obtained from clusters containing more than 10 voxels were defined as regions of interest (ROIs).

Time Course of High-gamma Power Change in ROIs

Firstly, we extracted the high-gamma power change in ROIs, using the time between −300 and 0 ms of the stimulus onset as the baseline to obtain stable results. We then compared the power change in each time window of interest between 0 and 600 ms with moving 100 ms windows for target stimuli to that for standard stimuli, and the power change for non-target stimuli to that for standard stimuli using paired t-tests. The statistical threshold was set at p<0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons using the FDR method [43] controlling the number of time windows, ROIs, and comparison pairs (i.e. target vs. standard, and non-target vs. standard).

Imaginary Coherence Analysis

A serious problem in coherence analysis of EEG or MEG arises from nil coherence caused by non-interacting sources (i.e., coherence which reflects artifacts rather than brain interaction). To identify true brain interaction, Nolte et al. [34] proposed the use of the imaginary part of coherency, which is only sensitive to synchronizations of two processes that are time-lagged to each other. Because volume conduction and inversion leakages do not cause a time-lag, imaginary coherence is insensitive to artifactual coherence.

In the imaginary coherence analysis, we selected the ROIs located in the regions related to control of attention (i.e. frontal region, parietal region, and thalamus) as seed and searched for regions in which the target or non-target condition showed stronger imaginary coherence than the standard condition, assuming the whole brain as the search volume. For the analysis of the high-gamma band, we used the same time windows as the time-course analysis. For the analysis of the theta band, reconstituted data were band-pass filtered in the 4 Hz to 8 Hz frequency domain. The baseline was defined as −300 to 0 ms, and imaginary coherences in three time windows were statistically evaluated (0 to 300 ms, 150 to 450 ms, and 300 to 600 ms). The statistical threshold was set at p<0.05 (FWE), corrected for the spatial dimension at the voxel level using non-parametric mapping software (SnPM; http://go.warwick.ac.uk/tenichols/snpm). We used a non-parametric statistical test due to unknown nature of the data distribution. Only results that included more than 10 significant voxels are reported.

Results

Behavioral Results

The rate of correct responses to non-target, target, and standard stimuli were 99.7% (SE = 0.3), 84.9% (SE = 3.4), and 99.3% (SE = 0.3), respectively. The average reaction time to target stimuli was 543 ms (SE = 34.7). The average number of analyzed non-target-, target-, and standard-stimulus trials were 22.7 (SE = 0.89), 18.8 (SE = 0.87), and 22.7 (SE = 0.89), respectively.

Localizing High-gamma Activity Relevant to the 3-stimulus Oddball Task

The F-contrast obtained from the 3-stimulus oddball task revealed task-relevant regions, including the bilateral occipital region, the left frontal region, the left parietal region, and the left thalamus (Fig. 2). The peak voxels were defined as ROIs for further analysis (Table 1).

thumbnail
Figure 2. High-gamma activity relevant to a 3-stimulus oddball task (p<0.05, FWE corrected).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059969.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. High-gamma activity relevant to a 3-stimulus oddball task (p<0.05, FWE corrected).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059969.t001

Time Course of High-gamma Power Change in ROIs

Results of time-course analysis of ROIs are shown in Figure 3. Time-frequency representations are shown in Figure 4. The earliest significant differences between the non-target and standard conditions were observed in the left MFG and in the right lingual gyrus at the time window of 100 to 200 ms. In the 200 to 300 ms time window, the left IPS, the left middle occipital gyrus, the right cuneus, the left thalamus, the left supramaginal gyrus, and the left postcentral gyrus were significantly different for non-target and target stimuli. On the other hand, the earliest significant differences between the target and standard conditions were observed in the left MFG, in the left thalamus, and in the left supramaginal gyrus at the time window of 200 to 300 ms. Significant differences were found between the target and standard conditions in the left paracental lobule after 300 ms and in the left postcentarl gyrus after 400 ms. At the time window of 500 to 600 ms, the left IPS, the left middle occipital gyrus, and the left midcingulate cortex were significantly different for the target and the standard conditions.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Time course of high-gamma power changes in each region of interest.

Blue bars indicate significant differences between the non-target condition and the standard condition, while red bars indicate significant differences between the target condition and the standard condition (p<0.05, FDR corrected).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059969.g003

thumbnail
Figure 4. Time-frequency representation of high-gamma power changes in each region of interest.

Warm colors indicate synchronization, while cold colors indicate desynchronization.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059969.g004

Additional Response-locked Analysis

In order to examine the effect of motor response, we conducted additional response-locked analysis in the left IPS, the left supramaginal gyrus, and the left postcentral gyrus, which were located near the somatosensory area and showed significant effects not only in non-target-standard comparisons but also in the target-standard comparisons around the response time. We extracted the high-gamma power change from −800 to 100 ms of the response onset in the target condition. The baseline period was defined as the time between −800 to −500 ms.

As a result, the left supramaginal gyrus and the left postcentral gyrus showed rapid increase of high-gamma ERS around the response onset (Figure 5). Although high-gamma ERS from 0 to 100 ms of response onset was not statistically significant in one sample t-test (p = 0.17, p = 0.10, respectively), we cannot perfectly exclude the possibility of the motor response effect in these ROIs. In contrast, the left IPS did not show an increase in high-gamma ERS around the response onset (p = 0.50 at −100 to 0 ms, and p = 0.95 at 0 to 100 ms), suggesting that observed high-gamma ERS in the left IPS in the stimulus-locked analysis cannot be explained by the motor response alone.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Time course of high-gamma power changes in response-locked analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059969.g005

Imaginary Coherence Analysis

The results of imaginary coherence analysis are summarized in Table 2. The ROIs located in the left MFG, the left IPS, and the left thalamus were selected as seed. There was greater high-gamma imaginary coherence between the left IPS and the right MFG, and between the left IPS and the left postcentral gyrus in the 300 to 400 ms window of the target condition than in the standard condition. In the theta band, the left thalamus showed greater imaginary coherence with the left MFG in the non-target condition than in the standard condition from 150 to 450 ms.

thumbnail
Table 2. Results of imaginary coherence analysis for a 3-stimulus oddball task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059969.t002

Correlation between Reaction Time and Coherence in the Attention Network

We examined whether the coherences in the attention network would predict individual differences in mean reaction time to target stimuli. Therefore, we examined the correlation between the reaction time and the strength of imaginary coherence among the left MFG, the right MFG, the left IPS, and the left thalamus. We calculated the correlations between the reaction time and the strength of imaginary coherence in each pair in each time window of interest between 200 and 600 ms with moving 100 ms windows, because the earliest significant difference between target and standard was found at 200 ms after the stimulus onset. We also examined the correlations between mean reaction time and regional high-gamma power.

Two subjects were excluded from the correlation analysis because their reaction times were extremely large (2 SDs above the mean). The results are summarized in Table 3. We found that reaction time was negatively correlated with imaginary coherence between the left MFG and the left IPS at the time window of 300 to 400 ms and at 400 to 500 ms, (r = −0.58, p<0.05; r = −0.58, p<0.05, respectively). Reaction time was also negatively correlated with imaginary coherence between the left MFG and the right MFG at the time window of 400 to 500 ms and 500 to 600 ms, (r = −0.60, p<0.05; r = −0.70, p<0.05, respectively). Reaction time was negatively correlated with the regional high-gamma power in the left thalamus at the time window of 300 to 400 ms (r = −0.78, p<0.01). The scatter plots are shown in Figure 6.

thumbnail
Figure 6. Significant correlation between reaction time and the strength of high-gamma imaginary coherence or regional high-gamma power.

(a) Reaction time vs. the strength of high-gamma imaginary coherence between the left IPS and the left MFG during the time window of 300 to 400 ms and (b) during the time window of 400 to 500 ms. (c) Reaction time vs. the strength of high-gamma imaginary coherence between the left IPS and the left MFG during the time window of 400 to 500 ms and (d) during the time window of 500 to 600 ms. (e) Reaction time vs. the high-gamma power in the left thalamus during the time window of 300 to 400 ms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059969.g006

thumbnail
Table 3. Correlation coefficient between reaction time and the strength of high-gamma imaginary coherence or the high-gamma power.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059969.t003

Discussion

We examined high-gamma activities during visual attentional processes using MEG with dual-state adaptive spatial filtering. Our results showed the involvement of the frontoparietal attention network and visual areas and clarified the temporal aspects of their activation. In addition, this is the first study to demonstrate source-level electrophysiological functional connectivity that underlies bottom-up and top-down attentional processes during a 3-stimulus visual oddball task. The strength of high-gamma coherences in attention network were linked to behavior performance (i.e. reaction time). The high-gamma power in the left thalamus was also correlated with the reaction time.

We found that several regions including the left MFG, the left IPS, and the left thalamus showed high-gamma ERS, both in the target and non-target conditions, which is consistent with previous fMRI studies. According to Corbetta et al. (2008) [44], the dorsal attention network is comprised of the bilateral IPS and the frontal eye field, but the left MFG is not involved. However, these authors argue that the prefrontal cortex is also a possible source of top-down signal. Indeed, several authors have reported that the left MFG activates during voluntary attentional control [6], [45][47]. Moreover, fMRI functional connectivity analysis suggests that the left MFG is a source of the top-down modulation of the visual association cortex [48]. We also observed increased high-gamma ERS in the right lingual gyrus and the right cuneus in the non-target condition, and increased high-gamma ERS in the left paracentral lobule and the left midcingulate cortex in the target condition. This is also reasonable because the lingual gyrus and the cuneus are a visual area, and they are therefore naturally involved in bottom-up attention. The paracentral lobule and the midcingulate cortex are motor-related areas. Therefore, the involvement of these areas in the target condition is not surprising because motor responses were included only in the target condition.

In terms of the temporal aspect of high-gamma ERS, our results indicated that the earliest significant effects were observed in the left MFG, the left thalamus, and the left supramaginal gyrus at the 200 to 300 ms time window in the target condition, and in the right lingual gyrus and the left MFG at the 100 to 200 ms time window in the non-target condition. These results are basically compatible with previous findings. For example, the earliest effect in MFG in the target condition was consistent with a study in which monkey prefrontal neurons reflected the target location faster than parietal neurons during top-down attention [49], and with fMRI studies that used effective connectivity analysis to demonstrate causal streams from frontal to parietal during attention holding tasks [47], [50]. The earliest effect in the thalamus in the target condition was also consistent with a deep brain stimulation study that showed that the thalamus supports the early recognition of targets [51] and with fMRI studies that showed the involvement of thalamus in higher-order mental processes, including attention [52], [53]. In the non-target condition, the earliest effect in the right lingual gyrus was also consistent with the nature of bottom-up attention. At first glance, frontal activities preceding parietal regions in the non-target condition appears somewhat inconsistent with the nature of stimulus-driven attention. Because our analysis focused on examining the entire brain activity, it might be not suitable to strictly determine the sequence of activities. For example, we analyzed brain activities by consecutive 100 ms time windows that started at 0 ms. This time window might not be useful for comparison of the earliest high-gamma activity in the frontal and parietal regions. This possibility is supported by the p-values of the statistical test between the non-target and the standard conditions during the 0 to 100 ms time window in the left MFG and the left IPS, both of which were marginally significant (p = 0.07 and p = 0.10, respectively).

Even if exact conclusions about difference in the processing sequence are not possible, we observed a clear difference in the temporal pattern of high-gamma ERS between top-down and bottom-up attention. In bottom-up attention, high-gamma ERS occurred quickly, even earlier than the time window of P3a, probably due to the perceptual salience of the non-target stimuli. However, high-gamma ERS decreased quickly because non-target stimuli are irrelevant to the current goal. In top-down attention, while high-gamma ERS started earlier than the time window for P3b, it was relatively slow. However, in contrast to the bottom-up attention, high-gamma ERS was sustained, or even increased, after reaching significance, probably due to the relevance of the stimulus to the current goal.

We also observed increased imaginary coherence in the high-gamma band between the left IPS and the right MFG in the target condition and in the theta band between the left thalamus and the left MFG in the non-target condition. Both of these observations are in accordance with the time windows associated with P300. Theta activity in frontal regions is associated with working memory [54], prediction errors [55], and also visual attention [36], [56]. Causal flow between the left thalamus and the left MFG during attention-holding tasks has also been reported in previous fMRI studies [47], [50]. It has also been proposed that the thalamus plays a role in regulating activities across cortical regions [31], [57], [58]. Thus, the observed theta band coherence may reflect these processes, although there is some controversy as to whether theta ERS and P300 reflect physiologically distinct mechanisms [59]. The right MFG is associated with the ventral network [44] and may link the dorsal and ventral networks [60]. The ventral network is strongly activated by stimuli that are important, even if they are not very distinctive [44]. The target stimuli in our study had these properties. Therefore, both the dorsal and ventral networks might be activated by the target. Based on this assumption, the observed imaginary coherence between the left IPS and the right MFG might reflect the linking process of the dorsal network with the ventral network.

Correlation analysis revealed that individual differences in functional connectivity within attention networks predicted individual differences in the speed of responses to target stimuli. All of the significant correlations were negative, indicating that individuals with higher coherence in attention networks exhibited faster responses. Although imaginary coherence analysis per se did not allow us to determine the direction of the information flow, we speculated that the left MFG was the upper stream, given the following temporal sequence: starting at the time window of 200 to 300 ms, there was increased high-gamma ERS in the left MFG; at 300 to 400 ms, the strength of imaginary coherence between the left MFG and the left IPS was negatively correlated with response time and the strength of imaginary coherence between the left IPS and the right MFG increased; at 400 to 500 ms, the strength of imaginary coherence between the left MFG and the right MFG negatively correlated with response time; and finally, at 500 to 600 ms, the left IPS increased high-gamma ERS. Based on this assumption, our results indicated that functional connectivity between the sources of top-down signal and the dorsal attention network (i.e. the left MFG - the left IPS) or the ventral attention network (i.e. the left MFG - the right MFG) predict behavioral performance, but those between the dorsal attention network and the ventral attention network do not (i.e. the left IPS - the right MFG). We observed significant correlations with the reaction time in coherence pairs in which only one of regions (i.e. the left MFG) showed significantly increased high-gamma ERS. This was interpretable because non-significance implies that there are considerable individual differences, and therefore they were observable. We also found a negative correlation between the reaction time and high-gamma power in the thalamus at the time window of 300 to 400 ms. As discussed previously, the thalamus plays an important role in attention modulation and behavioral response modulation. These results are in line with previous studies that indicate the amplitude of hemodynamic response in the MFG and that in the thalamus predict the response time of visual target detection [5].

We selected a similar high-gamma band (52 to 100 Hz) to that of previous studies [61], [62]. The ranges of reported frequencies of high-gamma varies widely among studies and include 59 to 85 Hz, 65 to 90 Hz, 70 to 85 Hz, 70 to 90 Hz, 75 to 100 Hz, and 81 to 101 Hz [63]. Recently, Belluscio et al. (2012) [64] reported different connectivity properties for 30 to 50 Hz, 50 to 90 Hz, and 90 to 150 Hz. Ray and Maunsell (2011) [65] also reported that frequencies above 80 Hz are mostly due to spiking whereas gamma activity of 30 to 80 Hz is rhythmic. Thus, it is possible that the band we chose might overlap these two bands and may not be well characterized. However, in most of the time-frequency representations shown in Fig. 4, there were oscillatory peaks around 80 Hz. Therefore, we think our results primarily reflect gamma rhythms. Indeed, the boundary between gamma rhythm and spikes should not be strict [65], given that the oscillation frequency of a network critically depends on excitation-inhibition balance [66].

A limitation of the present study is the small number of trials per condition per subject. We selected the number of infrequent stimuli based on the previous studies that indicated approximately 20 target trials stabilize the amplitude of P300 [67] and the response to novel stimuli are rapidly habituated with repetition [68]. However, the number of infrequent stimuli may have been low for analysis of ERS of the high-gamma band. Thus, our results of individual subject analysis should be considered as preliminary and even though the significant correlation obtained between the reaction time and the imaginary coherences seemed to be interpretable, future investigation is required.

Conclusions

We investigated the spatiotemporal dynamics of electrophysiological activity related to attentional processes using MEG with a dual-state adaptive spatial filtering technique, which may increase fidelity in source reconstruction. We identified high-gamma event-related synchronization in the left middle frontal gyrus, the left intraparietal sulcus, the left thalamus, and the visual areas in different time windows for the target and non-target conditions. We also observed elevated high-gamma coherence between the left IPL and the right MFG in the target condition at 300 to 400 ms, which might reflect the linking process of the dorsal network with the ventral network. In addition, the strength of high-gamma imaginary coherence between the left MFG and left IPL, between the left MFG and the right MFG, and the high-gamma power in the left thalamus predicted the speed of target detection. This source-level electrophysiological evidence enriches our understanding of the bi-directional attention processes: stimulus-driven bottom-up attentional orienting to a salient but irrelevant stimulus, and top-down allocation of attentional resources to stimulus evaluation.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Yuki Yamada for her support in MEG experiment, and to Editage for providing editorial assistance and publication support.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: YA AK TK TN MS RK. Performed the experiments: YA AK TK TN. Analyzed the data: YA AK TN. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: YA AK TK TN MS EO RK. Wrote the paper: YA AK TK TN MS EO RK.

References

  1. 1. Nieuwenhuis S, Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD (2005) Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. Psychol Bull 131(4): 510–532.
  2. 2. Verleger R, Jaśkowski P, Wascher E (2005) Evidence for an Integrative Role of P3b in Linking Reaction to Perception. Journal of Psychophysiology 19(3): 165–181.
  3. 3. Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 118(10): 2128–2148.
  4. 4. O’Connell RG, Balsters JH, Kilcullen SM, Campbell W, Bokde AW, et al. (2012) A simultaneous ERP/fMRI investigation of the P300 aging effect. Neurobiol Aging 33(10): 2448–2461.
  5. 5. Madden DJ, Whiting WL, Provenzale JM, Huettel SA (2004) Age-related changes in neural activity during visual target detection measured by fMRI. Cereb Cortex 14(2): 143–155.
  6. 6. Ardekani BA, Choi SJ, Hossein-Zadeh GA, Porjesz B, Tanabe JL, et al. (2002) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of brain activity in the visual oddball task. Cogn Brain Res 14(3): 347–356.
  7. 7. Stevens AA, Skudlarski P, Gatenby JC, Gore JC (2000) Event-related fMRI of auditory and visual oddball tasks. Magn Reson Imaging 18(5): 495–502.
  8. 8. Miller EK, Buschman TJ (2012) Cortical circuits for the control of attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23: 1–7.
  9. 9. Prado J, Carp J, Weissman DH (2011) Variations of response time in a selective attention task are linked to variations of functional connectivity in the attentional network. Neuroimage 54(1): 541–549.
  10. 10. Comerchero MD, Polich J (1999) P3a and P3b from typical auditory and visual stimuli. Clin Neurophysiol 110(1): 24–30.
  11. 11. Courchesne E, Hillyard SA, Galambos R (1975) Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 39(2): 131–143.
  12. 12. Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie1 AE, Desimonen R (2001) Modulation of Oscillatory Neuronal Synchronization by Selective Visual Attention. Science 291(5508): 1560–1563.
  13. 13. Lachaux JP, Fonlupt P, Kahane P, Minotti L, Hoffmann D, et al. (2007) Relationship between task-related gamma oscillations and BOLD signal: new insights from combined fMRI and intracranial EEG. Hum Brain Mapp 28(12): 1368–1375.
  14. 14. Fries P (2009) Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation. Annu Rev Neurosci 32: 209–224.
  15. 15. Donner TH, Siegel M (2011) A framework for local cortical oscillation patterns. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(5): 191–199.
  16. 16. Kahlbrock N, Butz M, May ES, Schnitzler A (2012) Sustained gamma band synchronization in early visual areas reflects the level of selective attention. Neuroimage 59(1): 673–681.
  17. 17. Hoogenboom N, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Fries P (2010) Visually induced gamma-band activity predicts speed of change detection in humans. Neuroimage 51(3): 1162–1167.
  18. 18. Doesburg SM, Roggeveen AB, Kitajo K, Ward LM (2008) Large-scale gamma-band phase synchronization and selective attention. Cereb Cortex 18(2): 386–396.
  19. 19. Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O, Peronnet F, Pernier J (1998) Induced gamma-band activity during the delay of a visual short-term memory task in humans. J Neurosci. 18(11): 4244–4254.
  20. 20. Fries P (2005) A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci 9(10): 474–480.
  21. 21. Stefanics G, Jakab A, Bernáth L, Kellényi L, Hernádi I (2004) EEG early evoked gamma-band synchronization reflects object recognition in visual oddball tasks. Brain Topogr 16(4): 261–264.
  22. 22. Padmanabhapillai A, Porjesz B, Ranganathan M, Jones KA, Chorlian DB, et al. (2006) Suppression of early evoked gamma band response in male alcoholics during a visual oddball task. Int J Psychophysiol 60(1): 1526.
  23. 23. Padmanabhapillai A, Tang Y, Ranganathan M, Rangaswamy M, Jones KA, et al. (2006) Evoked gamma band response in male adolescent subjects at high risk for alcoholism during a visual oddball task. Int J Psychophysiol 62(2): 262271.
  24. 24. Gurtubay IG, Alegre M, Labarga A, Malanda A, Iriarte J, et al. (2001) Gamma band activity in an auditory oddball paradigm studied with the wavelet transform. Clin Neurophysiol 112(7): 1219–1228.
  25. 25. Gurtubay IG, Alegre M, Labarga A, Malanda A, Artieda J (2004) Gamma band responses to target and non-target auditory stimuli in humans. Neurosci Lett 367(1): 6–9.
  26. 26. Kaiser J, Lutzenberger W (2004) Frontal gamma-band activity in magnetoencephalogram during auditory oddball processing. Neuroreport 15(14): 2185–2188.
  27. 27. Lee B, Park KS, Kang DH, Kang KW, Kim YY, et al. (2007) Generators of the gamma-band activities in response to rare and novel stimuli during the auditory oddball paradigm. Neurosci Lett 413(3): 210–215.
  28. 28. Choi JW, Jung KY, Kim CH, Kim KH (2010) Changes in gamma- and theta-band phase synchronization patterns due to the difficulty of auditory oddball task. Neurosci Lett 468(2): 156–160.
  29. 29. Kim KH, Yoon J, Kim JH, Jung KY (2008) Changes in gamma-band power and phase synchronization with the difficulty of a visual oddball task. Brain Res 1236: 105–112.
  30. 30. Crone NE, Sinai A, Korzeniewska A (2006) High-frequency gamma oscillations and human brain mapping with electrocorticography. Prog Brain Res 159: 275–295.
  31. 31. Miller KJ, Leuthardt EC, Schalk G, Rao RPN, Anderson NR, et al. (2007) Spectral changes in cortical surface potentials during motor movement. J Neurosci 27(9): 2424–2432.
  32. 32. Dalal SS, Guggisberg AG, Edwards E, Sekihara K, Findlay AM, et al. (2008) Five-dimensional neuroimaging: localization of the time-frequency dynamics of cortical activity. Neuroimage 40(4): 1686–1700.
  33. 33. Sekihara K, Owen JP, Trisno S, Nagarajan SS (2011) Removal of spurious coherence in MEG source-space coherence analysis. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 58(11): 3121–3129.
  34. 34. Nolte G, Bai O, Wheaton L, Mari Z, Vorbach S, et al. (2004) Identifying true brain interaction from EEG data using the imaginary part of coherency. Clin Neurophysiol 115(10): 2292–2307.
  35. 35. Brookes MJ, Hale JR, Zumer JM, Stevenson CM, Francis ST, et al. (2011) Measuring functional connectivity using MEG: methodology and comparison with fcMRI. Neuroimage 56(3): 1082–1104.
  36. 36. Green JJ, McDonald JJ (2008) Electrical neuroimaging reveals timing of attentional control activity in human brain. PLoS Biol 6(4): e81.
  37. 37. Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, et al. (2006) High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science 313(5793): 1626–1628.
  38. 38. Demiralp T, Ademoglu A, Comerchero M, Polich J (2001) Wavelet analysis of P3a and P3b. Brain Topogr 13(4): 251–267.
  39. 39. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113.
  40. 40. Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Reinvang I, Lundervold A, Fischl B, et al. (2005) Cortical volume and speed-of-processing are complementary in prediction of performance intelligence. Neuropsychologia 43(5): 704–713.
  41. 41. Ueno S, Okumura E, Remijn GB, Yoshimura Y, Kikuchi M, et al. (2012) Spatiotemporal frequency characteristics of cerebral oscillations during the perception of fundamental frequency contour changes in one-syllable intonation. Neurosci Lett 515(2): 141–146.
  42. 42. Herdman AT, Cheyne D (2009) A practical guide for MEG and beam forming. In: Handy TC, editor. Brain Signal Analysis, Cambridge: The MIT Press. 99–140.
  43. 43. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B: Statistical Methodology 57(1): 289–300.
  44. 44. Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL (2008) The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58(3): 306–324.
  45. 45. Hopfinger JB, Buonocore MH, Mangun GR (2000) The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat Neurosci 3(3): 284–291.
  46. 46. Hahn B, Ross TJ, Stein EA (2006) Neuroanatomical dissociation between bottom-up and top-down processes of visuospatial selective attention. Neuroimage 32(2): 842–853.
  47. 47. Ozaki TJ, Ogawa S (2009) Causality analysis defines neural streams of orienting and holding of attention. Neuroreport 20: 1371–1375.
  48. 48. Gazzaley A, Rissman J, Cooney J, Rutman A, Seibert T, et al. (2007) Functional interactions between prefrontal and visual association cortex contribute to top-down modulation of visual processing. Cereb Cortex. 17: 125–135.
  49. 49. Buschman TJ, Miller EK (2007) Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices. Science 315(5820): 1860–1862.
  50. 50. Ozaki TJ (2011) Frontal-to-parietal top-down causal streams along the dorsal attention network exclusively mediate voluntary orienting of attention. PLoS ONE 6: e20079.
  51. 51. Klostermann F, Wahl M, Marzinzik F, Schneider GH, Kupsch A, et al. (2006) Mental chronometry of target detection: human thalamus leads cortex. Brain 129: 923–931.
  52. 52. Van Der Werf YD, Tisserand DJ, Visser PJ, Hofman PA, Vuurman E, et al. (2001) Thalamic volume predicts performance on tests of cognitive speed and decreases in healthy aging. A magnetic resonance imaging-based volumetric analysis. Cogn Brain Res 11: 377–385.
  53. 53. Woldorff MG, Hazlett CJ, Fichtenholtz HM, Weissman DH, Dale AM, et al. (2004) Functional parcellation of attentional control regions of the brain. J Cogn Neurosci 16: 149–165.
  54. 54. Mitchell DJ, McNaughton N, Flanagan D, Kirk IJ (2008) Frontal-midline theta from the perspective of hippocampal “theta”. Prog Neurobiol 86(3): 156–185.
  55. 55. Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ, Klein TJ, Allen JJ (2010) Frontal theta links prediction errors to behavioral adaptation in reinforcement learning. Neuroimage 49(4): 3198–3209.
  56. 56. Missonnier P, Deiber MP, Gold G, Millet P, Gex-Fabry PM, et al. (2006) Frontal theta event-related synchronization: comparison of directed attention and working memory load effects. J Neural Transm 113(10): 1477–1486.
  57. 57. Saalmann YB, Kastner S (2011) Cognitive and perceptual functions of the visual thalamus. Neuron 71(2): 209–223.
  58. 58. Saalmann YB, Pinsk MA, Wang L, Li X, Kastner S (2012) The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science 337(6095): 753–756.
  59. 59. Wang X, Ding M (2011) Relation between P300 and event-related theta-band synchronization: a single-trial analysis. Clin Neurophysiol 122(5): 916–924.
  60. 60. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME (2006) Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103: 10046–10051.
  61. 61. Mazzoni A, Brunel N, Cavallari S, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S (2011) Cortical dynamics during naturalistic sensory stimulations: experiments and models. J Physiol Paris 105(1–3): 2–15.
  62. 62. Goto T, Hirata M, Umekawa Y, Yanagisawa T, Shayne M, et al. (2011) Frequency-dependent spatiotemporal distribution of cerebral oscillatory changes during silent reading: a magnetoencephalograhic group analysis. Neutoimage 54(1): 560–567.
  63. 63. Darvas F, Scherer R, Ojemann JG, Rao RP, Miller KJ, et al. (2010) High gamma mapping using EEG. Neuroimage 49(1): 930–938.
  64. 64. Belluscio MA, Mizuseki K, Schmidt R, Kempter R, Buzsaki G (2012) Cross-frequency phase-phase coupling between theta and gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. J Neurosci 32(2): 423–435.
  65. 65. Ray S, Maunsell JHR (2011) Different origins of gamma rhythm and high-gamma activity in macaque visual cortex. PLoS Biol 9(4): e1000610.
  66. 66. Atallah BV, Scanziani M (2009) Instantaneous modulation of gamma oscillation frequency by balancing excitation with inhibition. Neuron 62(4): 566–577.
  67. 67. Cohen J, Polich J (1997) On the number of trials needed for P300. Int J Psychophysiol 25(3): 249–255.
  68. 68. Yamaguchi S, Hale LA, D’Esposito M, Knight RT (2004) Rapid prefrontal-hippocampal habituation to novel events. J Neurosci 24(23): 5356–5363.