Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Does Presentation Format Influence Visual Size Discrimination in Tufted Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus spp.)?

  • Valentina Truppa ,

    valentina.truppa@istc.cnr.it

    Affiliation Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, National Research Council (CNR), Rome, Italy

  • Paola Carducci,

    Affiliations Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, National Research Council (CNR), Rome, Italy, Department of Biology, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy

  • Cinzia Trapanese,

    Affiliation Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, National Research Council (CNR), Rome, Italy

  • Daniel Hanus

    Affiliation Department of Developmental and Comparative Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany

Abstract

Most experimental paradigms to study visual cognition in humans and non-human species are based on discrimination tasks involving the choice between two or more visual stimuli. To this end, different types of stimuli and procedures for stimuli presentation are used, which highlights the necessity to compare data obtained with different methods. The present study assessed whether, and to what extent, capuchin monkeys’ ability to solve a size discrimination problem is influenced by the type of procedure used to present the problem. Capuchins’ ability to generalise knowledge across different tasks was also evaluated. We trained eight adult tufted capuchin monkeys to select the larger of two stimuli of the same shape and different sizes by using pairs of food items (Experiment 1), computer images (Experiment 1) and objects (Experiment 2). Our results indicated that monkeys achieved the learning criterion faster with food stimuli compared to both images and objects. They also required consistently fewer trials with objects than with images. Moreover, female capuchins had higher levels of acquisition accuracy with food stimuli than with images. Finally, capuchins did not immediately transfer the solution of the problem acquired in one task condition to the other conditions. Overall, these findings suggest that – even in relatively simple visual discrimination problems where a single perceptual dimension (i.e., size) has to be judged – learning speed strongly depends on the mode of presentation.

Introduction

Many comparative studies concerning learning abilities in the visual cognition domain focus on non-human primates. Like humans, all other diurnal primate species mainly rely on sight to gather information from the environment [1, 2]. Moreover, these species have visual systems that share many properties with that of human beings [35].

Virtually all experimental paradigms employed to study visual cognition in humans and non-human species are based on discrimination tasks involving the choice between two or more visual stimuli. For this purpose, different types of stimuli and stimulus presentations can be used. The necessity to carry out in-depth analyses of cognitive processes has led to the development of increasingly sophisticated methods for data collection. The last few decades in particular have seen widespread use of computerised procedures in an increasing number of taxonomic groups, such as apes [69], Old World monkeys [1013], New World monkeys [1422], dogs [23, 24], horses [25], pigeons [2633], corvids [34], and tortoises [35]. Despite this trend, little is known about the generalisability of such computerised methods to more natural settings involving ‘real’ three-dimensional stimuli (e.g., food). This highlights the necessity to compare data obtained with different methodological approaches, e.g., computerised procedures and procedures involving a human experimenter [3639].

Computerised procedures present both advantages and limits when compared with non-computerised methods. On the one hand, computerised tasks allow the administration of a higher number of trials within a scheduled time slot and also the presentation of stimuli and registration of responses very precisely (in terms of both accuracy and response time). Moreover, computerised procedures can allow to completely prevent experimenter biases. On the other hand, these types of procedures are almost exclusively administered by using images presented on a computer screen, with only very few exceptions involving, for example, computerised procedures where individuals are required to manipulate mechatronic objects [40] or to manipulate a joystick device controlling a projected laser dot in order to select food items [41].

Besides the type of experimental procedure used for stimuli presentation, the type of stimuli per se might also consistently affect subjects’ behaviour. There is evidence, for example, that the use of food as a stimulus to be discriminated in cognitively demanding choice tasks might positively [42] or negatively [43] influence capuchin monkeys’ performance according to task requirements. On the one hand, Addessi et al. [42] found that capuchins were significantly more accurate in quantity discrimination judgements with food items than with tokens (i.e., inherently non-valuable objects that acquire an associative value upon exchange with the experimenter, [44]). On the other hand, in a reverse-reward contingency task, where capuchins had to select the smaller option to receive the larger reward, their performance was significantly lower with food items than with tokens [43]. In addition, Boysen et al. [45] showed that when chimpanzees were required to solve a reverse-reward contingency task, four out of five individuals preferred to choose the larger option despite it meaning they received the smaller amount of food; moreover, they chose the option including the larger food item independently of the total amount of food items presented by the two options. Overall, findings on capuchins and chimpanzees suggest that the particular ecological salience of food may induce a powerful predisposition to select the option offering the larger amount of items very accurately and that this accuracy can also be biased by the size of individual food items.

Thus, systematic comparisons of the same subjects tested by using different types of methods (i.e., within-subjects experiments) would contribute to clarifying how methodological aspects may affect visual perceptual learning in a given task. Moreover, comparisons across different tasks in which the same visual discrimination problem is presented, also represent an opportunity to test the ability to use similarities to transfer the solution of a familiar problem across novel but functionally equivalent conditions.

The ability to understand similarities and analogies is a fundamental aspect of advanced cognition [46, 47]. Evidence of the ability to use an identity concept has been reported in several non-human species belonging to very different taxonomic groups (chimpanzees [48, 49]; baboons [50, 51]; macaques [12, 52, 53]; capuchin monkeys [21, 22, 54]; dolphins [5557]; sea lions [58, 59]; rats [60]; parrots [61]; jackdaws [62]; pigeons [28, 6365]). More recently, an increasing amount of data has also demonstrated that non-human primates other than apes are able under specific training conditions to solve analogical problems involving the ability to understand second-order relations, i.e., relations between relations [15, 6668]. Usually, studies to determine conditions under which this type of ability occurs are carried out presenting preliminary discrimination training followed by a testing phase in which completely novel stimuli are presented. In fact, only if a relationship between stimuli is coded in an abstract rule/concept can it be applied to new stimuli never seen before [69]. Standard experimental protocols basically rely on the presentation of novel stimuli in a task in which all other problem features are the same to guarantee a constant ‘framework of reference’ for the subject.

Conversely, studies in which the same type of problem was presented in different frameworks have shown only limited evidence of the ability of non-human species to generalise across conditions. For example, Martin-Ordas and Call [70] found that apes trained to solve functionally equivalent tool-using tasks that require them to overcome obstacles to get a reward (i.e., trap tasks), showed some knowledge about the effects that traps have on slow-moving unsupported objects. However, this knowledge did not seem to be abstract enough to allow them to establish general analogies between perceptually dissimilar tasks. The authors argued that perceptual components like different spatial arrangements of the tasks could not be perceived as functionally equivalent by the apes and therefore could have generated some novelty effect.

In the present study, we assessed whether visual learning ability of capuchin monkeys varies on the basis of both the type of stimuli and the type of procedure used. For this purpose, we evaluated learning speed and response accuracy of capuchin monkeys faced with different versions of the same visual discrimination problem, i.e., to choose the larger stimulus between two stimuli of the same shape. First, we compared two experimental conditions that can be considered to be the most different from each other: the discrimination of food items (that could be directly obtained and manipulated after the choice) and the discrimination of images presented on a computer screen. Next, we assessed monkeys’ ability to discriminate between non-edible objects. This allowed us to both evaluate learning processes to achieve acquisition in each condition and to evaluate monkeys’ generalisation ability across different conditions.

Experiment 1: Discrimination of food items and computer images

Two conditions were included in Experiment 1: (1) the Food condition, in which the stimuli to discriminate between were pairs of food items presented on a table, and (2) the Image condition, in which the stimuli were pairs of figures presented on a computer screen. The order of the presentation of the conditions was counterbalanced across subjects. Half of the monkeys were tested in the Image condition after having completed the Food condition, whereas the other half received the Image condition first. We hypothesised that the monkeys would succeed in achieving the learning criterion with both foods and images, although we expected them to need fewer trials and to be more accurate in the Food compared to the Image condition. Additionally, we expected that they would take a similar number of training days to achieve the criterion in both conditions since a higher number of daily trials could be administered in the computerised procedure.

Method

Subjects

The subjects were eight tufted capuchin monkeys derived from individuals of different provenience and considered to be unknown combinations of species of the genus Sapajus. Recent data have revealed that capuchin monkeys, traditionally identified as the single genus Cebus, are two genera: (i) the robust (tufted) forms are now classified as the genus Sapajus, and (ii) the gracile (untufted) forms are retained as the genus Cebus [71, 72]. The sample included four males (Robot, Sandokan, Pedro and Robin Hood) and four females (Roberta, Robiola, Rucola and Quincy). All subjects were adults (12–27 years old) born in captivity. Capuchins were hosted at the Primate Center of the Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, CNR, Rome, Italy. They belonged to three groups, each housed in an indoor–outdoor enclosure (indoor: 5 m2 × 2.5 m high; outdoor: 40–130 m2 × 3 m high). Each indoor enclosure included two large cages, an experimental box and an area for the experimenter. Capuchins were individually tested in the experimental box (180 cm × 75 cm × 75 cm; the box floor is 80 cm higher than the floor of the rest of the room) to which they had access through a sliding door from the adjacent indoor cage. Each subject was separated from the group just before the daily testing session solely for the purpose of testing. Testing occurred between 9:30 a.m. and 3:15 p.m. Water was freely available at all times. Fresh fruit, vegetables and monkey chow were provided in the afternoon after testing. In the Food condition, half of the monkeys (Roberta, Robin Hood, Sandokan, Pedro) were already familiar with the experimental apparatus because they took part in a precedent experiment on tool choice where the same apparatus was used [73]. The other monkeys were familiarised to with the apparatus immediately before starting this experiment. In the Image condition, all the subjects were already familiar with the computerised workstation because they took part in a training programme with computerised matching-to-sample tasks [1416]. None of the subjects had been tested with a computerised two-alternative choice task before.

Apparatus

Food condition.

The apparatus consisted of a metal trolley (59 cm long, 64 cm wide, 92 cm high) with a sliding tray (54 cm × 20 cm), which could be moved forward and backward on a wooden support (59 cm × 56 cm) and two Plexiglas barriers (51 cm × 53 cm) on the lateral sides (Fig 1A). The apparatus was installed in front of the experimental box (approximately at a distance of 6 cm), behind a transparent Plexiglas panel (56 cm × 73 cm) mounted on the front wall of the experimental box. The transparent panel had four holes, situated at the corners: Two small ones (1.5 cm diameter) at the bottom for ‘pointing’ actions (i.e., to insert a finger into a hole spatially matching the selected stimulus) and two larger ones (3.5 cm diameter) at the top through which the monkey received the reward. Stimuli were located on the sliding tray and were gradually moved closer to the subject, while the monkey was positioned in front of the apparatus looking at the stimuli.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Experimental apparatuses.

(a) Experimental apparatus used in the Food condition of Experiment 1 and in the Object condition of Experiment 2; (b) Experimental apparatus used in the Image condition of Experiment 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126001.g001

Image condition.

The apparatus consisted of a laptop (model Acer Aspire 1400 Series) connected to a 17” touch screen (model Tyco Electronics, ET1729L-7UEA-1-D-GY-G) and an automatic food dispenser (model ENV-203-45, MED Associates, Inc. Georgia, VT) (Fig 1B). The E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.) was used as the stimulus generator and served both to present the stimuli and to record the response behaviour. The food dispenser was programmed to deliver one 45-mg banana-flavoured pellet (TestDiet, Richmond, IN, USA) when the monkey provided a correct response during the experimental trial. The pellet was delivered into a PVC feeding cup (with a diameter of 4 cm) located 14.5 cm below the touch screen in the centre. A wooden frame (58 cm wide × 54 cm high) with a central aperture (36 cm wide × 30 cm high) surrounded the touch screen. The food dispenser was placed behind the wooden frame, out of sight of the subject. The laptop was placed at the back of the touch screen to allow the experimenter to follow the progress of the session. The touch screen, the food dispenser and the laptop were mounted on the top shelf of a trolley (90 cm long × 58 cm wide × 85 cm high). The apparatus was fastened to a metal panel (56.5 cm wide × 74 cm high) mounted on the front wall of the experimental box (Fig 1B). The metal panel had a central rectangular aperture (37.5 cm wide × 30 cm high) which corresponded spatially to the touch screen position.

Stimuli

Food condition.

The stimuli were two pairs of food items (Fig 2A), inserted into grey PVC (polyvinyl chloride) boards, held up vertically (approximately 45°) by a back support. The first pair of stimuli included two salted sticks (0.5 cm diameter) placed into PVC boards (14 cm × 20 cm): One stick was 10 cm high and the other was 6 cm high. The second pair included circular wafers placed into PVC boards (18 cm × 18 cm): One wafer had a 6.5 cm diameter and the other had a 3.7 cm diameter. The backs of the wafers had a little sugar glaze spread on them, in order to make them tastier for the monkeys.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Stimulus sets.

(a) Stimuli used in the Food condition of Experiment 1 were a pair of salted sticks of different lengths and a pair of wafers of different diameters; (b) Stimuli used in the Image condition of Experiment 1 were a pair of lines of different lengths and a pair of circles of different diameters; (c) Stimuli used in the Object condition of Experiment 2 were a pair of wooden sticks of different lengths and a pair of wooden spheres of different diameters.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126001.g002

Image condition.

The stimuli were two pairs of white computer images presented in grey frames (Fig 2B). The first pair consisted of vertical lines (0.4 cm wide): One line was 4.5 cm high and the other was 2.5 cm high. The second pair consisted of circles: One circle had a 2.8 cm diameter and the other had a 1.5 cm diameter. Lines were presented within a 9.5 cm × 4.5 cm grey frame and circles were presented within a 9.5 cm × 7.5 cm grey frame.

Procedure

Food condition.

In each trial, the experimenter inserted the food items into the PVC boards and placed the stimuli on the sliding tray with the frontal part out of the monkey’s view. Only when the monkey was facing towards the apparatus were the stimuli simultaneously rotated to show the food items to the subject, with the sliding tray being moved forward into the monkey’s reach. The monkey made its choice by inserting a finger into one of the two small holes; the experimenter then handed the chosen food item to the monkey through the corresponding large hole towards the top of the panel. As soon as the monkey finished eating the food item and approached the apparatus again, a new trial started. Each monkey received one session of 16 trials per day: Half of the trials included pairs of salted sticks and the other half included pairs of wafers. Each larger stimulus was presented an equal number of times in both the right and the left position. Trials were presented in a random order. Sessions were administered 5 days per week. The learning criterion was achieved when monkeys chose the larger food item of each pair at least 7 out of 8 times (87.5%, binomial p <. 032) during three consecutive sessions.

Image condition.

At the beginning of each trial, a white cross composed of two segments (0.8 cm high and 0.2 cm wide) appeared in the centre of the screen on a black background; when the monkey touched the cross two stimuli were simultaneously displayed. The initial touch of the central cross was adopted to ensure that monkeys were looking at the screen when the stimuli appeared. The subject had to indicate its choice by touching one of the two stimuli on the screen; the computer automatically recorded the choice. After the response, the display was immediately extinguished and, if the subject selected the larger stimulus, a food pellet was delivered. A correct response was followed by a 5-s inter-trial interval (ITI), whereas an incorrect response was followed by both a 10-s time out (TO) and a 5-s ITI. During the experimental trials and the ITI, the screen was light grey; during the TO, the screen was green. Each monkey received 64 trials per day (four sessions of 16 trials): Half of the trials included pairs of lines and the other half included pairs of circles. Each larger stimulus was presented an equal number of times in both the right and the left position. The trials were presented in a random order. Sessions were administered 5 days per week. The learning criterion was achieved when monkeys chose the larger image of each pair at least 7 out of 8 times (87.5%) during three consecutive sessions.

We presented a larger number of trials per day in the Image condition for two reasons: (1) to replicate procedures that usually featured computerised tasks, in which many trials separated by relatively short time intervals are administered and (2) to equalise the daily exposure time between the Food and the Image condition (i.e., about 20 minutes dealing with the task).

Data analyses

The measures considered for analysis were the number of trials, the number of days and the accuracy scores to achieve the learning criterion (individual data are reported in S1 Table). Accuracy scores were defined as the average percentage of correct responses during the last three training sessions. Because the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test showed that the distribution of data did not significantly deviate from normality, we used parametric statistics to compare the mean scores between different conditions. Mixed-model analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed with Task Condition (image/computerised vs. food/not computerised) and Stimulus Shape (sticks/lines vs. wafers/circles) as within-subjects factors, and Order of Presentation of the task and Sex as between-subjects factors. Separate analyses were performed on the number of trials, the number of days and the accuracy scores to achieve the learning criterion respectively. Statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05, apart from with analyses that required correction for multiple tests (i.e., Bonferroni correction).

Ethics Statement

This study and the research protocols used in Experiments 1 and 2 were approved by the Italian Health Ministry (Central Direction for the Veterinary Service, approvals n. 11/2011-C and n. 132/2014-C to V. Truppa). Housing conditions and experimental procedures were in full accordance with European law on humane care and use of laboratory animals and complied with the recommendations of the Weatherall Report [74]. To increase three-dimensional space available to the animals, indoor enclosures were furnished with perches and ropes and outdoor enclosures were furnished with logs, branches and ropes. Moreover, the presence of natural substrates, including woodchips on the ground, served to promote the monkeys’ exploratory behaviours. All subjects were habituated to the experimental cage, the experimental routine and the experimenters.

Results

Number of trials

Significant main effects of both Task Condition [food: M = 34.5, SE = 3.7; image: M = 327.5, SE = 72.9; F(1,4) = 13.2, p =. 022, η2p =. 767] and Stimulus Shape [sticks/lines: M = 255.0, SE = 43.3; wafers/circles: M = 107.0, SE = 31.4; F(1,4) = 81.6, p =. 0008, η2p =. 953] were found. Moreover, the analysis revealed a significant interaction between Task Condition and Stimulus Shape [F(1,4) = 82.9, p =. 0008, η2p =. 954] (Fig 3A). Post hoc analyses (Bonferroni test) across task conditions indicated that the number of learning trials with the salted sticks (M = 35, SE = 3.7) was significantly smaller than the number of learning trials with the lines (M = 475, SE = 86.9) (p =. 0002); similarly, the number of trials required with the wafers (M = 34, SE = 4.2) was significantly smaller than that required with the circles (M = 180, SE = 63.8) (p =. 018). Post hoc analyses across stimulus shapes revealed no significant difference in the number of learning trials between salted sticks (M = 35, SE = 3.7) and wafers (M = 34, SE = 4.2) in the Food condition (p = 1.0); whereas, in the Image condition, the number of learning trials for lines (M = 475, SE = 86.9) was significantly greater than for circles (M = 180, SE = 63.8) (p =. 001). No other main effects or interactions turned out to be significant.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Monkeys’ performance in Experiment 1.

Monkeys’ performance in the Food and Image conditions as a function of stimulus shape (sticks/lines and wafers/circles) was reported considering: (a) the number of trials to achieve the learning criterion (mean ± SE); (b) the number of days to achieve the learning criterion (mean ± SE); and (c) the percentage of correct responses in the last three training sessions (mean ± SE). *p <. 05; **p <. 01; ***p <. 001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126001.g003

Number of days

A significant main effect of Stimulus Shape [sticks/lines: M = 9.8, SE = 1.3; wafers/circles: M = 5.1, SE = 0.9; F(1,4) = 89.3, p =. 0007, η2p =. 957] and a Task Condition x Stimulus Shape interaction [F(1,4) = 71.1, p =. 001, η2p =. 947] were found (Fig 3B). Post hoc analyses across task conditions revealed that the number of learning days with the salted sticks (M = 4.4, SE = 0.5) was significantly smaller than the number of learning days with the lines (M = 15.3, SE = 2.6) (p =. 0008); whereas the number of learning days for wafers (M = 4.3, SE = 0.5) and that for circles (M = 6.0, SE = 1.9) were not significantly different (p =. 505). Moreover, post hoc analyses across stimulus shapes revealed no significant difference in the number of learning days between the salted sticks (M = 4.4, SE = 0.5) and the wafers (M = 4.3, SE = 0.5) in the Food condition (p = 1.0); whereas, in the Image condition, the number of learning days for lines (M = 15.3, SE = 2.6) was significantly greater than for circles (M = 6.0, SE = 1.9) (p =. 001). No other main effects or interactions turned out to be significant.

Accuracy scores

A significant main effect of Task Condition [food: M = 97.1%, SE = 0.7; image: M = 91.4%, SE = 1.1; F(1,4) = 80.5, p =. 0008, η2p =. 846] and a Task Condition x Sex interaction [F(1,4) = 10.6, p =. 031, η2p =. 727] were found (Fig 3C). Post hoc analyses revealed that female capuchins had higher accuracy scores in the Food condition (M = 96.9%, SE = 0.7) than in the Image condition (M = 89.1%, SE = 0.8) (p =. 006); while males’ accuracy scores in the Food condition (M = 97.4%, SE = 1.1) did not significantly differ from accuracy scores in the Image condition (M = 93.8%, SE = 2.1) (p =. 094). No other main effects or interactions turned out to be significant.

Transfer across tasks

To evaluate subjects’ immediate ability to generalise their discrimination performance across conditions, we analysed the percentage of correct responses at the beginning of their second-order condition (Food or Image). The four subjects that received the Image condition second did not achieve the learning criterion within the first three sessions (trials 1–48; Lines: Robot 41.67%, Robin Hood 45.83%, Roberta 50.0%, Robiola 50.0%; Circles: Robot 83.33%, Robin Hood 75.0%, Roberta 58.33%, Robiola 50.0%). Specifically, subjects that received the Image condition second were equally accurate in discriminating circles compared to subjects that received the Image condition first [Image second: M = 66.65%, SE = 7.60; Image first: M = 64.57%, SE = 2.69; t(6) = 0.26, p =. 806]. Furthermore, subjects that received the Image condition second were even significantly less accurate in discriminating the lines than subjects that received the Image condition first, again considering only the first three sessions [Image second: M = 46.87%, SE = 1.99; Image first: M = 63.55%, SE = 2.01; t(6) = -5.90, p =. 001] (Bonferroni correction for multiple tests, p =. 025).

On the other hand, two out of the four subjects that received the Food condition as the second task achieved the learning criterion within the first three sessions in discriminating between sticks (Pedro 92%) and between wafers (Sandokan 100%) respectively. However, it should be mentioned that two out of the four subjects that received the Food condition first also successfully discriminated between stimuli during the first three sessions (Sticks: Robot 100%, Robin Hood 96%; Wafers: Robot 100%, Robin Hood 100%). Thus, although some of the subjects that received the Food condition second seemed to exhibit an immediate transfer of performance from the Image to the Food condition, comparable performances were observed in subjects that received the Food condition first.

Experiment 2: Discrimination of objects

The second experiment was carried out to clarify whether the better performance in the Food compared to the Image condition found in Experiment 1 was due to the use of edible items as stimuli for discrimination, or due to the influence of other characteristics of the non-computerised task (e.g., concrete 3D stimuli) which might have improved monkeys’ performance. For this purpose, monkeys were required to discriminate between pairs of (non-edible) objects of the same shape presented with the same apparatus used for the food items in Experiment 1.

Method

Subjects

The subjects were seven capuchin monkeys, the same tested in Experiment 1, except for one female, Rucola. We did not test her because she exhibited alarm vocalisations and threat reactions towards the objects used in this experiment, both behaviours that in this species are usually associated with the presence of dangerous stimuli [75]. All the monkeys were tested in Experiment 2 after they had completed Experiment 1. Since the Object condition was presented to all experimental subjects as the third task, we also presented this condition to a control group of capuchins that did not take part in Experiment 1, namely four adult capuchin monkeys, two females (Carlotta and Paprica) and two males (Paté and Vispo), with experimental histories comparable to those of the individuals tested in Experiment 1. This comparison between the experimental and control group allowed us to evaluate the occurrence of potential facilitation effects due to previous experience.

Apparatus

The apparatus and the Plexiglas panel were the same as those used in the Food condition of Experiment 1 (Fig 1A).

Stimuli

The stimuli were two pairs of white wooden objects, mounted on grey wooden boards, held up vertically by a back support (Fig 2C). The first pair of stimuli included two cylindrical wooden rods (0.8 cm diameter), each mounted on a wooden board (14 cm × 20 cm): One rod was 10 cm high and the other was 6 cm high. The second pair included two wooden spheres, each mounted on a wooden board (18 cm × 18 cm): One sphere had a diameter of 6.2 cm, and the other 3.7 cm.

Procedure

At the beginning of each trial, the experimenter placed the wooden boards on the sliding tray with the frontal part out of the monkey’s view. Only when the monkey was facing towards the apparatus were the stimuli simultaneously rotated to show the objects to the subject, with the sliding tray being moved forward to get the stimuli into the subject’s reach. The monkey made its choice by inserting a finger into one of the two small holes of the Plexiglas panel. Whenever the monkey chose the larger stimulus, the experimenter handed over a piece of peanut through the corresponding large hole towards the top of the panel. As soon as the subjects finished eating the reward and approached the apparatus again, a new trial started. Each monkey received one session of 16 trials per day: Half of the trials included pairs of rods and the other half included pairs of spheres. Each larger stimulus was presented an equal number of times in the right and the left position. The trials were presented in a random order. Sessions were administered 5 days per week. The learning criterion was achieved when monkeys chose the larger object of each pair at least 7 out of 8 times (87.5%) during three consecutive sessions.

Data analyses

The measures considered for analysis were the number of trials and the percentage of correct responses to achieve the learning criterion with objects (individual data are reported in S2 Table). Because the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test showed that the distribution of data did not deviate from normality, we used parametric statistics to compare the mean scores between different conditions. We performed mixed-model ANOVAs on the two measures considered, with Stimulus Shape (rods and spheres) as a within-subjects factor and Sex as a between-subjects factor. Moreover, we used dependent t-tests to compare performance on objects with performance on foods and images, both in terms of learning speed (number of trials to criterion) and percentage of correct responses to acquisition. Finally, experimental and control subjects were compared both in terms of learning speed and response accuracy to acquisition by using mixed-model ANOVAs with Stimulus Shape (rods and spheres) as a within-subjects factor and Group (Experimental and Control) as a between-subjects factor.

Statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05, apart from with analyses that required correction for multiple tests (i.e., Bonferroni correction).

Results

The difference between rods and spheres was not significant, neither for the number of trials to acquisition (Rods M = 132.6, SE = 40.2, Spheres M = 104.0, SE = 22.4, F(1,5) = 0.42, p =. 547; Fig 4A) nor for the percentage of correct responses to acquisition (Rods M = 93.4, SE = 1.8, Spheres M = 95.2, SE = 1.4, F(1,5) = 0.33, p =. 592; Fig 4B). Therefore, in the following analyses we collapsed all data for the different stimuli shapes (rods and spheres). Neither main effects nor interactions due to sex differences were significant.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Monkeys’ performance in Experiment 2.

Monkeys’ performance in the Object condition as a function of stimulus shape (rods and spheres) was reported considering: (a) the number of trials to achieve the learning criterion (mean ± SE); and (b) the mean percentage of correct responses in the last three training sessions (mean ± SE).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126001.g004

Number of trials

Capuchins required significantly fewer trials to achieve the learning criterion with foods than with objects [Foods: M = 34.8, SE = 4.3; Objects: M = 118.3, SE = 21.6; t(7) = -3.84, p =. 008]. Moreover, they required significantly fewer trials with objects than images [Images: M = 328.6, SE = 84.2; t(7) = -3.15, p =. 019]. Both these differences were still significant after Bonferroni correction for multiple tests (statistical significance was set at p =. 025).

Accuracy scores

No significant differences were found in the mean percentage of correct responses to acquisition between objects and foods [Objects: M = 94.3%, SE = 1.0; Foods: M = 97.3%, SE = 0.7; t(7) = 2.10, p =. 082] and between objects and images [Images: M = 91.4%, SE = 1.2; t(7) = 2.50, p =. 046] (with Bonferroni correction for multiple tests, statistical significance was set at p =. 025).

Transfer across tasks

Although all subjects received the Object condition as their third task, none of the subjects was able to achieve the learning criterion within the first three sessions (trial 1–48), neither for discriminating rods (Robot 71%, Pedro 75%, Robin Hood 67%, Sandokan 50%, Quincy 54%, Roberta 42%, Robiola 50%) nor for discriminating spheres (Robot 42%, Pedro 79%, Robin Hood 8%, Sandokan 92%, Quincy 29%, Roberta 54%, Robiola 50%). Even though one individual (Sandokan) showed an outstanding mean performance with the spheres within the first three sessions (92%), he did not achieve the criterion before the fourth session.

Comparison between experimental and control groups

Experimental (Exp) and control (Ctrl) subjects were compared both in terms of learning speed and response accuracy to acquisition. The two measures considered were analysed by using a mixed model of ANOVA with Stimulus Shape (rods and spheres) as a within-subjects factor and Group (Exp and Ctrl) as a between-subjects factor. No significant main effects or interactions were found, neither concerning the number of trials to acquisition [Group: Exp, M = 118.29, SE = 21.64; Ctrl, M = 184.0, SE = 38.05; F(1,9) = 2.67, p =. 137; Stimulus Shape: rods, M = 170.18, SE = 35.47; spheres, M = 114.18, SE = 16.08; F(1,9) = 3.17, p =. 109; Group x Stimulus Shape: Exp rods, M = 132.57, SE = 40.22; Exp spheres, M = 104.0, SE = 22.36, Ctrl rods, M = 236.0, SE = 60.0; Ctrl spheres, M = 132.0, SE = 21.29; F(1,9) = 1.03, p =. 337] nor concerning the accuracy to acquisition [Group: Exp, M = 94.35%, SE = 0.99; Ctrl, M = 93.23%, SE = 1.97; F(1,9) = 0.33, p =. 581; Stimulus Shape: rods, M = 92.80%, SE = 1.39; spheres, M = 95.08%, SE = 1.23; F(1,9) = 1.42, p =. 264; Group x Stimulus Shape: Exp rods, M = 93.45%, SE = 1.79; Exp spheres, M = 95.24%, SE = 1.42; Ctrl rods, M = 91.67%, SE = 2.40; Ctrl spheres, M = 94.79%, SE = 2.62; F(1,9) = 0.11, p =. 753].

Discussion

This study showed that tufted capuchin monkeys succeeded in learning to discriminate computer images, food items and objects of different sizes and that the presentation format significantly affected their learning speed and accuracy to acquisition. In keeping with our predictions, capuchins achieved the learning criterion faster with food stimuli compared to both images (Experiment 1) and objects (Experiment 2). They also required fewer trials to achieve the criterion with objects than with images (Experiment 2). Moreover, female capuchins showed higher levels of accuracy to acquisition with foods than with images (Experiment 1). Finally, we did not find evidence that capuchins immediately transferred the solution acquired in one task condition to other conditions (Experiment 1 and 2).

Capuchins’ ability to already perform accurately with food items after a few trials is consistent with results from previous studies on food choice tasks demonstrating that chimpanzees have a spontaneous powerful propensity to select food items on the basis of their size, preferring the larger items over the smaller ones [37, 39, 45, 76]. For example, Menzel [39] found that chimpanzees faced with arrays including four pieces of banana immediately selected them in order of size, with larger pieces being taken first. Additional studies also demonstrated that chimpanzees required to choose between different amounts of food can fail to select the larger amount because of biases towards the individual largest food items presented in the smaller option [37, 45]. Furthermore, in a quantity discrimination task Addessi et al. [42] showed that capuchins’ rate of correct choices was higher with food items than with tokens (i.e., objects with symbolic value) (but see also [77] for different results in baboons and macaques). Overall, these findings suggested that the ecological salience of food could affect choice behaviour in visual discrimination tasks. To maximise food intake during foraging activities, species might have evolved enhanced levels of attention towards—and/or strong propensity to detect—large (edible) items present in the visual field.

Besides the different salience of the stimuli, the type of procedure might also have played a significant role in at least three different ways, which are not mutually exclusive. First, the possibility to receive the chosen stimulus in the Food condition, where monkeys actually obtained what they chose, most resembles the ‘natural’ way of choosing and, as such, it might elicit improved judgement accuracy. Second, the possibility to directly manipulate food items after the choice might have facilitated subsequent size judgements in a highly manually skilled species like capuchin monkeys. Recent data on humans suggest that haptic information dominates in size discrimination processes in children younger than 8 years of age, i.e., when the integration of visual and haptic information is not fully developed [78]. Performance on visual size discrimination tasks was also found to be significantly worse in children of 9–16 years of age with movement disorders of the upper limbs (compared to age-matched typical children). This is possibly because, according to the cross-sensory calibration hypothesis, visual discrimination is impaired when the haptic sense is unavailable for manipulation and cannot be readily used to calibrate and fine-tune the visual experience of size [79]. Moreover, data on human adults showed that training subjects to discriminate shape categories by touch also improved their ability to visually discriminate the same stimuli and vice versa, indicating transfer of implicit knowledge across modalities [80]. Third, trials involving food items and objects forced monkeys to spend more time looking at the stimuli before they made a choice (i.e., the time needed to move the stimuli into the subject’s reach). Conversely, in the computerised procedure monkeys were able to choose as soon as the two stimuli appeared on the screen. Previous studies using computerised procedures have indicated that monkeys have a spontaneous tendency to inspect visual stimuli on a screen only for a very short time before taking a decision, and an increase of stimulus presentation time does not improve subjects’ discrimination performance [81]. Nevertheless, subjects’ response accuracy is positively affected by forcing them to actively attend to the stimulus, for example by repeatedly touching it to indicate a choice [12].

In the Image condition, capuchins required significantly more trials to achieve the criterion with the lines than with the circles. This difference between the shapes did not emerge when discriminating between food items (Experiment 1) or objects (Experiment 2). This finding indicates that some shapes require more training to be accurately discriminated than others, at least when stimuli are presented as two-dimensional images. From a perceptual point of view, 2D images carry less visual information compared to concrete 3D stimuli such as foods and objects, and this limitation could be more pronounced in certain shapes. Capuchins encountered more difficulties in discriminating different lines lengths, although their ratio (small/large = 0.56) was comparable to the ratio between the circles (small/large = 0.54). It is plausible to hypothesise that a decrease of the line length ratio might improve capuchins’ performance. Such a distance effect follows Weber’s law, according to which the discriminability of two magnitudes does not depend on their absolute magnitude differences but on their ratio, and has been reported in studies on line length discrimination using computer images with macaques [82] and crows [83]. Such an effect has also been reported for discrete quantity judgements in several animal species (e.g., ants [84]; birds [85]; fish [86]; monkeys [42, 87, 88]; apes [45, 89, 90]).

When learning speed was considered in terms of the number of days needed to achieve the learning criterion in Experiment 1 (i.e., total amount of time spent dealing with the tasks independently of the number of trials), performance in the Food and Image conditions no longer differed significantly, at least when circular-shaped stimuli were considered. This finding raises questions about memory mechanisms underlying learning performance such as: (1) the mere repetition of trials and (2) the neurophysiological time to generate an effective encoding, storage and retrieval of information in long-term memory. Although on the basis of our observations we can reasonably suppose that the increase of the number of trials may systematically improve subjects’ performance, we cannot completely exclude the possibility that time needed for memory consolidation partially acts independently of the number of trials that subjects receive in a daily experimental session. Other studies suggest that time and sleep have crucial impact on memory consolidation and, therefore, retrieval performance in great apes [91]. Further studies are needed to assess performance when the number of trials per day is equated for the Image and the Food condition.

No clear evidence emerged in support of capuchins’ ability to generalise size discrimination across different tasks. Capuchins did not immediately solve the discrimination problem when they received the images after foods and when they were presented with objects after both images and foods. An immediate high performance with food was observed in half of the individuals when food was presented either as the first or second condition, and this was probably due to this particular type of stimulus and its ecological relevance for the subjects. Thus, perceptual similarities among the tasks used in this study seemed insufficient to promote transfer processes. In other words, capuchins were obviously not capable of detecting functional similarities between the tasks, which prevented successful knowledge transfer. This is consistent with previous data, which showed that capuchins trained to solve an identity matching-to-sample problem with figures glued to vertical PVC boards failed to transfer their matching ability when confronted with figures of the same shape but presented as computer images on a touch-screen apparatus [16]. On the other hand, we know from past studies that capuchin monkeys trained to match stimuli according to rules are able to generalise these rules to novel stimuli when stimulus type (e.g., figures) and apparatus are identical [15, 16, 21, 22]. Capuchins also demonstrated that they recognise the correspondence between objects and their pictorial representations (i.e., photographs, silhouettes and line drawings) when presented simultaneously on the same apparatus [36]. Hence, in capuchin monkeys the transfer of knowledge and the recognition of correspondences in the visual modality seem to be possible, but only under certain circumstances and when crucial perceptual features in presentation format do not vary. At this point, it remains an open question as to what kind of feature similarity is required to enable a successful transfer, and which generalisation rules are preferentially internalised by capuchin monkeys.

The claim that the transfer of learned knowledge is mediated by the degree of perceptual similarity among stimulus environments dates back at least to the dawn of behaviourism and, more generally, to the study of learning principles [92]. However, exactly what constitutes perceptually salient features facilitating the transfer of knowledge across tasks has yet to be clearly established. Current knowledge concerning variations of behaviour in relation to changes of physical dimensions in the presentation format does not allow strong and cogent claims to be made. Among others, at least two aspects of the presentation format could constitute salient features potentially improving generalisation processes during transfer tests. First, the use of the same apparatus with an identical spatial arrangement of the stimuli could operate as a cue to indicate that a specific, familiar problem has to be solved even if novel stimuli are presented. Second, the use of the same type of stimuli, specifically 3D or 2D stimuli, should also avoid the need for object-picture correspondence abilities.

Overall, this study indicates that learning processes and transfer of knowledge in the visual domain strongly depend on task features, even in relatively simple discrimination problems. The same problem presented with different—although functionally equivalent—methods led to consistent variations in learning behaviour. Especially for manually skilled species, the possibility to exploit tactile feedback from the direct manual exploration of the stimuli seems to be crucial in order to calibrate and improve visual judgements. Taking the results and their implications together, one should be very cautious when comparing cognitive performances between different presentation formats, not to mention different experimental paradigms. Further studies are needed to clarify whether, and to what extent, visual discrimination may vary depending on the types of stimuli and/or different procedures and whether different animal species respond to procedural changes in similar ways or not.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Individual data for Experiment 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126001.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Individual data for Experiment 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126001.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Elsa Addessi and two anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments on the first version of the manuscript. We wish to thank Diego Antonio De Simone for experimental assistance and Arianna Manciocco, Massimiliano Bianchi and Simone Cartarinacci for help with animal management. We also thank the Comune di Roma-Museo Civico Zoologia and the Fondazione Bioparco for hosting the Unit of Cognitive Primatology and Primate Center.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: VT DH. Performed the experiments: VT PC CT. Analyzed the data: VT PC CT. Wrote the paper: VT PC CT DH.

References

  1. 1. Fleagle JG (2013) Primate adaptation and evolution. USA: Elsevier Academic Press. 423 p.
  2. 2. Gilad Y, Weibe V, Przeworski M, Lancet D, Paabo S (2004) Loss of olfactory receptor genes coincides with the acquisition of full trichromatic vision in primates. Plos Biol 2. 120–125.
  3. 3. Walker S (1983) Animal thought. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. 437 p.
  4. 4. Fobes JL, King JE (1982) Vision: the dominant primate modality. In: Fobes JL, King JE, editors. Primate behavior. New York: Academic Press. pp. 219–243.
  5. 5. Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M (1982) Two cortical visual systems. In: Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJW, editors. Analysis of visual behaviour. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 549–586.
  6. 6. Vonk J (2013) Matching based on biological categories in Orangutans (Pongo abelii) and a Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). PeerJ. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.158
  7. 7. Vonk J (2003) Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and Orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of first- and second-order relations. Anim Cogn 6: 77–86. pmid:12687418
  8. 8. Hopkins WD, Washburn DA (2002) Matching visual stimuli on the basis of global and local features by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Anim Cogn 5: 27–31. pmid:11957399
  9. 9. Beran MJ, Rumbaugh DM, Savage-Rumbaugh ES (1998) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) counting in a computerized testing paradigm. Psychol Rec 48: 3–20. pmid:9846123
  10. 10. Fagot J, Bonté E (2010) Automated testing of cognitive performance in monkeys: Use of a battery of computerized test systems by a troop of semi-free ranging baboons (Papio papio). Behav Res Meth 42: 507–516. pmid:20479182
  11. 11. Fagot J, Cook R (2006) Evidence for large long-term memory capacities in baboons and pigeons and its implications for learning and the evolution of cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 17564–17567. pmid:17088563
  12. 12. Katz JS, Wright AA, Bachevalier J (2002) Mechanisms of same/different abstract-concept learning by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 28: 358–68. pmid:12395493
  13. 13. Wasserman EA, Fagot J, Young ME (2001) Same-different conceptualization by baboons (Papio papio): The role of entropy. J Comp Psychol 115: 42–52. pmid:11334218
  14. 14. Truppa V, De Simone D, Piano Mortari E, De Lillo C (2014) Effects of brief time delays on matching-to-sample abilities in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.). Behav Brain Res 271: 240–248. pmid:24861708
  15. 15. Truppa V, Piano Mortari E, Garofoli D, Privitera S, Visalberghi E (2011) Same/different concept learning by capuchin monkeys in matching-to-sample tasks. PloS ONE 6(8): e23809. pmid:21858225
  16. 16. Truppa V, Garofoli D, Castorina G, Piano Mortari E, Natale F, Visalberghi E (2010) Identity concept learning in matching-to-sample tasks by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Anim Cogn 13: 835–848. pmid:20574839
  17. 17. Evans TA, Beran MJ, Chan B, Klein ED, Menzel CR (2008) An efficient computerized testing method for the capuchin monkey (Cebus apella): Adaptation of the LRC-CTS to a socially housed nonhuman primate species. Behav Res Meth 40: 590–596.
  18. 18. Galvão OF, Soares-Filho PSD, Barros RS, Souza CBA (2008) Matching-to-sample as a model of symbolic behaviour for bio-behavioral investigations. Rev Neurosci 19. 149–156. pmid:18751521
  19. 19. Galvão OF, Barros RS, Dos Santos JR, Brino ALF, Brandão S, Lavratti CM, et al.(2005) Extent and limits of the matching concept in Cebus apella: a matter of experimental control? Psychol Rec 55: 219–232.
  20. 20. Fragaszy D, Johnson-Pynn J, Hirsh E, Brakke K (2003) Strategic navigation of two-dimensional alley mazes: Comparing capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees. Anim Cogn 6: 149–160. pmid:12955584
  21. 21. Barros RDS, Galvao ODF, McIlvane WJ (2002) Generalized identity matching-to-sample in Cebus apella. Psychol Rec 52: 441–460.
  22. 22. D’Amato MR, Salmon DP, Colombo M (1985) Extent and limits of the matching concept in monkeys (Cebus apella). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 11: 35–51. pmid:3989475
  23. 23. Bayer K, Range F, Aust U, Steurer M, Huber L (2009) The touch-screen method as an implement for dog experiments. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res 4: 51.
  24. 24. Range F, Aust U, Steurer M, Huber L (2008) Visual categorization of natural stimuli in domestic dogs. Anim Cogn 11: 339–347. pmid:18026761
  25. 25. Hanggi EB, Ingersoll JF (2009) Long-term memory for categories and concepts in horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn 12: 451–462. pmid:19148689
  26. 26. Katz JS, Wright AA (2006) Mechanisms of same/different abstract-concept learning by pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 32: 80–86. pmid:16435967
  27. 27. Cook RG, Kelly DM, Katz JS (2003) Successive two-item same–different discrimination and concept learning by pigeons. Behav Process 62: 125–144.
  28. 28. Cook RG, Katz JS, Cavoto BR (1997) Pigeon same–different concept learning with multiple stimulus classes. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 23: 417–433. pmid:9411018
  29. 29. Wasserman EA, Young ME, Dalrymple RM (1997) Memory-based same–different conceptualization by pigeons. Psychon Bull Rev 4: 552–558.
  30. 30. Cook RG, Cavoto KK, Cavoto BR (1995) Same–different texture discrimination and concept learning by pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 21: 253–260.
  31. 31. Wasserman EA, Hugart JA, Kirkpatrick-Steger K (1995) Pigeons show same–different conceptualization after training with complex visual stimuli. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 21: 248–252. pmid:7602259
  32. 32. Wasserman EA, DeVolder CL, Coppage DJ (1992) Non-similarity-based conceptualization in pigeons via secondary or mediated generalization. Psychol Sci 3: 374–379.
  33. 33. Wasserman EA, Kiedinger RE, Bhatt RS (1988) Conceptual behavior in pigeons: Categories, subcategories, and pseudocategories. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 14: 235–246.
  34. 34. Veit L, Nieder A (2013) Abstract rule neurons in the endbrain support intelligent behaviour in corvid songbirds. Nat Commun https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms3878
  35. 35. Mueller-Paul J, Wilkinson A, Austa U, Steurer M, Hall G, Huber L (2014) Touchscreen performance and knowledge transfer in the red-footed tortoise (Chelonoidis carbonaria). Behav Proc 106: 187–192. pmid:24946312
  36. 36. Truppa V, Spinozzi G, Stegagno T, Fagot J (2009) Picture processing in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behav Proc 82: 140–152.
  37. 37. Beran MJ, Evans TA, Harris EH (2008) Perception of food amounts by chimpanzees based on the number, size, contour length and visibility of items. Anim Behav 75: 1793–1802. pmid:19412322
  38. 38. Spinozzi G, De Lillo C, Truppa V (2003) Global and local processing in hierarchical visual stimuli in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol 117: 15–23. pmid:12735359
  39. 39. Menzel EW (1960) Selection of food by size in the chimpanzee, and comparison with human judgments. Science 131: 1527–1528. pmid:17802497
  40. 40. Polizzi di Sorrentino E, Sabbatini G, Truppa V, Bordonali A, Taffoni F, Formica D, et al. (2014) Exploration and learning in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.): the role of action-outcome contingencies. Anim Cogn 17: 1081–1088. pmid:24638875
  41. 41. Howard AM, Fragaszy DM (2014) Multi-step routes of capuchin monkeys in a laser pointer travelling Salesman task. Am J Primatol 76: 828–841. DOI: 0.1002/aip.22271. pmid:24700520
  42. 42. Addessi E, Crescimbene L, Visalberghi E (2008) Food and token quantity discrimination in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Anim Cogn 11: 275–282. pmid:17901990
  43. 43. Addessi E, Rossi S (2011) Tokens improve capuchin performance in the reverse–reward contingency task. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 278: 849–854.
  44. 44. Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM (2004) A concept of value during experimental exchange in brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Folia Primatol 75: 317–330. pmid:15486443
  45. 45. Boysen ST, Berntson GG, Mykobi KL (2001) Size matters: impact of item size and quantity on array choice by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115: 106–110. pmid:11334213
  46. 46. French R (1995) The subtlety of sameness: A theory and computer model of analogy-making. Cambridge: MA: MIT Press. 190 p.
  47. 47. Vosniadou S, Ortony A (1989) Similarity and analogical reasoning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 592 p.
  48. 48. Oden DL, Thompson RKR, Premack D (1988) Spontaneous transfer of matching by infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 14: 140–145. pmid:3367100
  49. 49. Nissen HW, Blum JS, Blum RA (1948) Analysis of matching behavior in chimpanzees. J Comp Physiol Psychol 41: 62–74. pmid:18904766
  50. 50. Bovet D, Vauclair J (2001) Judgment of conceptual identity in monkeys. Psychon Bull Rev 8: 470–475. pmid:11700897
  51. 51. Bovet D, Vauclair J (1998) Functional categorization of objects and of their pictures in baboons (Papio anubis). Learn Motiv 29: 309–322.
  52. 52. Wright AA, Rivera JJ, Katz JS, Bachevalier J (2003) Abstract-concept learning and list-memory processing by capuchin and rhesus monkeys. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 29: 184–198. pmid:12884678
  53. 53. Mishkin M, Delacour J (1975) An analysis of short-term visual memory in the monkey. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1: 326–334. pmid:811754
  54. 54. D’Amato MR, Salmon DP (1984) Cognitive processes in Cebus monkeys. In: Roitblat HL, Bever TG, Terrace HS, editors. Animal Cognition. NJ: Erlbaum, Hillsdale. pp. 149–168.
  55. 55. Mercado E III, Killebrew DA, Pack AA, Mácha IVB, Herman LM (2000) Generalization of ‘same–different’ classification abilities in bottlenosed dolphins. Behav Proc 50: 79–94.
  56. 56. Herman LM, Hovancik JR, Gory JD, Bradshaw GL (1989) Generalization and visual matching by a Bottle-nosed Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Evidence for invariance of cognitive performance with visual and auditory materials. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 15: 124–136.
  57. 57. Herman LM, Gordon JA (1974) Auditory delayed matching in the bottlenose dolphin. J Exp Anal Behav 21: 19–26. pmid:4204143
  58. 58. Kastak D, Schusterman RJ (1994) Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two Californian sea lions (Zalophus californianus). Anim Learn Behav 22: 427–435.
  59. 59. Pack AA, Herman LM, Roitblat HL (1991) Generalization of visual matching and delayed matching by a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Anim Learn Behav 19: 37–48.
  60. 60. Peña T, Pitts RC, Galizio M (2006) Identity matching-to-sample with olfactory stimuli in rats. J Exp Anal Behav 85: 203–221. pmid:16673826
  61. 61. Pepperberg IM (1987) Acquisition of the same/different concept by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Learning with respect to categories of color, shape, and material. Anim Learn Behav 15: 423–432.
  62. 62. Wilson B, Mackintosh N, Boakes R (1985) Transfer of relational rules in matching and oddity learning by pigeons and corvids. Q J Exp Psychol B 37: 313–332.
  63. 63. Wright AA (1997) Concept learning and learning strategies. Psychol Sci 8: 119–123.
  64. 64. Wright AA, Cook RG, Rivera JJ, Sands SF, Delius JD (1988) Concept learning by pigeons: Matching-to-sample with trial unique video picture stimuli. Anim Learn Behav 16: 436–444.
  65. 65. Zentall TR, Edwards CA, Moore BS, Hogan DE (1981) Identity: The basis for both matching and oddity learning in pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 7: 70–86.
  66. 66. Fagot J, Thompson RKR (2011) Generalized relational matching by Guinea baboons (Papio papio) in two by two-item analogy problems. Psychol Sci 22: 1304–1309. pmid:21934135
  67. 67. Flemming TM, Thompson RKR, Beran MJ, Washburn DA (2011) Analogical reasoning and the differential outcome effect: transitory bridging of the conceptual gap for rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 37: 353–360. pmid:21574737
  68. 68. Kennedy EH, Fragaszy DM (2008) Analogical reasoning in a capuchin monkey (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol 122: 167–175. pmid:18489232
  69. 69. Premack D (1978) On the abstractness of human concepts: Why it would be difficult to talk to a pigeon. In: Hulse SH, Fowler H, Honig WK, editors. Cognitive processes in animal behavior. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale. pp. 423–451.
  70. 70. Martin-Ordas G, Call J (2009) Assessing generalization within and between trap tasks in the great apes. Int J Comp Psychol 22: 43–60.
  71. 71. Lynch Alfaro JW, Boubli JP, Olson LE, Di Fiore A, Wilson B, Gutiérrez-Espeleta GA, et al. (2012) Explosive Pleistocene range expansion leads to widespread Amazonian sympatry between robust and gracile capuchin monkeys. J Biogeogr 39: 272–88.
  72. 72. Lynch Alfaro JW, De Souza Silva J, Rylands AB (2012) How different are robust and gracile capuchin monkeys? An argument for the use of Sapajus and Cebus. Am J Primatol 74: 273–86. pmid:22328205
  73. 73. Manrique HM, Sabbatini G, Call J, Visalberghi E (2011) Tool choice on the basis of rigidity in capuchin monkeys. Anim Cog 14: 775–786.
  74. 74. Weatherall D (2006) The use of non-human primates in research: A working group report. Available: http://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/images/project/nhpdownl.pdf.
  75. 75. Osman-Hill WC (1960) Primates. Comparative anatomy and taxonomy. IV Cebidae, Part A. Edinburgh: University Press. 511p.
  76. 76. Menzel EW Jr (1961) Perception of food size in the chimpanzee. J Comp Physiol Psychol 54: 588–591.
  77. 77. Schmitt V, Fisher J (2011) Representational format determines numerical competence in monkeys. Nat Commun 2: 257 pmid:21448156
  78. 78. Gori M, Del Viva M, Sandini G, Burr D (2008) Young children do not integrate visual and haptic form information. Curr Biol 18: 694–698. pmid:18450446
  79. 79. Gori M, Tinelli F, Sandini G, Cioni G, Burr D (2012) Impaired visual size-discrimination in children with movement disorders. Neuropsychologia 50: 1838–1843. pmid:22569216
  80. 80. Wallraven C, Bülthoff HH, Waterkamp S, van Dam L, Gaißert N (2014) The eyes grasp, the hands see: Metric category knowledge transfers between vision and touch. Psychon Bull Rev 21: 976–985. pmid:24307250
  81. 81. D’Amato MR, Worsham RW (1972) Delayed matching in the capuchin monkey with brief sample durations. Learn Motiv 3: 304–312.
  82. 82. Tudusciuc O, Nieder A (2010) Comparison of length judgments and the Mueller-Lyer illusion in monkeys and humans. Exp Brain Res 207: 221–231. pmid:20972775
  83. 83. Moll FW, Nieder A (2014) The long and the short of it: Rule-based relative length discrimination in carrion crows, Corvus corone. Behav Proc 107: 142–149.
  84. 84. Cronin AL (2014) Ratio-dependent quantity discrimination in quorum sensing ants. Anim Cogn 17: 1261–1268. pmid:24844665
  85. 85. Scarf D, Hayne H, Colombo M (2011) Pigeons on Par with Primates in Numerical Competence. Science 334: 1664. pmid:22194568
  86. 86. Agrillo C, Dadda M, Bisazza A (2007) Quantity discrimination in female mosquitofish. Anim Cogn, 10: 63–70. pmid:16868736
  87. 87. Beran MJ (2008) Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) succeed in a test of quantity conservation. Anim Cogn 11:109–116. pmid:17549530
  88. 88. Brannon EM, Terrace HS (2000) Representation of the numerosities by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Proc 26: 31–49. pmid:10650542
  89. 89. Hanus D, Call J (2007) Discrete quantity judgments in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus): The effect of presenting whole sets versus item-by-item. J Comp Psychol 121: 241–249. pmid:17696650
  90. 90. Beran MJ (2001) Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115:181–191. pmid:11459165
  91. 91. Martin-Ordas G, Call J (2011) Memory processing in great apes: the effect of time and sleep. Biol Lett 7: 829–832. pmid:21632621
  92. 92. Thorndike EL, Woodworth RS (1901) The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. Psychol Rev 8: 247–261.