Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter August 17, 2020

Principles of extinction learning of nonaversive experience

  • Metin Uengoer

    Metin Uengoer is an expert on Associative Learning. His research interests include extinction and contextual control of behavior, error-correction modelling, and attentional mechanisms of learning. He studied Psychology at Philipps-Universität Marburg, where he also received a doctoral degree in Natural Sciences. Between 2010 and 2011, and between 2015 and 2016, he was acting professor for Experimental and Biological Psychology at the Psychology Department of Philipps-Universität Marburg. He participated as a principal investigator in the DFG Research Unit 1581 between 2011 and 2017, and is currently working as a research fellow in the DFG Collaborative Research Centre 1280.

    ORCID logo EMAIL logo
    , Silke Lissek

    Silke Lissek studied Psychology and obtained her Ph.D. in Biopsychology at the Ruhr University Bochum. Since 2005 she works in the Neuroplasticity Imaging Group of the Department of Neurology, University Clinic Bergmannsheil, Ruhr University Bochum. Between 2010 and 2016 she was principal investigator in a project within the DFG research group FOR 1581, and since 2017 she is principal investigator in a project within the DFG Collaborative Research Centre 1280. Her research focuses on the neuronal basis of extinction learning and the renewal effect.

    , Martin Tegenthoff

    Martin Tegenthoff studied medicine and physics at the University of Münster. Doctoral thesis in medicine (Dr. med.). Board certification in neurology and psychiatry, specializing in pain therapy and rehabilitation medicine. Habilitation in Neurology at Ruhr University Bochum. Constitution of an interdisciplinary scientific study group for neuronal/cortical plasticity (www.ruhr-uni-bochum.de/neuroplasticity/index.html.de) with an emphasis on clinical and experimental neurophysiology and MR-neuroimaging. Since 2010 medical director and chair of the Department of Neurology and Neurological Traumatology and Neurorehabilitation, University Clinic Bergmannsheil, Ruhr University Bochum.

    , Denise Manahan-Vaughan

    Denise Manahan-Vaughan is a Neurophysiologist, Neuroscientist and Head of the Department of Neurophysiology within the Medical Faculty of the Ruhr University Bochum (www.rub.de/neurophys). She is also director and Dean of Studies of the International Graduate School of Neuroscience (www.rub.de/igsn) and speaker (primary coordinator) of the DFG-funded Collaborative Research Centre on Integration and Representation of Sensory Information Processes at the same university (www.rub.de/sfb874). After completion of her primary degree in Natural Sciences at Trinity College Dublin (TCD), Ireland, she obtained a PhD in Neuropharmacology at TCD and then moved to Germany to the Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology in Magdeburg. Having obtained an Habilitation degree in Physiology from the Otto von Guericke University in Magdeburg, she moved to the Johannes Müller Institute for Physiology at the Charité, Berlin, before moving to Bochum in 2003. Her research focusses on understanding the relationship between hippocampal and cortical synaptic plasticity and long-term associative and spatial memory.

    ORCID logo
    and Harald Lachnit

    Prof. Dr. Harald Lachnit, Dipl.-Psych., studied psychology at the Justus-Liebig University of Giessen (1975–1981). He received his doctoral degree in psychology in 1985 (“Contextual Dependence of Classical Conditioning”) and habilitated there in 1991 with the topic “Cognition and Brain: Building an Experimental Bridge”. During his doctoral and habilitation period he spent 6 research stays in North America. From 1992 to 1994 he was Professor of General Psychology at Philipps-Universität Marburg. Since 1994 he has been working there as Professor of General and Biological Psychology. From 1992 to 1997, he temporarily headed the Department of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. From 2010 to 2016 he served as vice president for studies and teaching.

From the journal Neuroforum

Abstract

This review outlines behavioral and neurobiological aspects of extinction learning, with a focus on nonaversive experience. The extinction of acquired behavior is crucial for readaptation to our environment and plays a central role in therapeutic interventions. However, behavior that has been extinguished can reappear owing to context changes. In the first part of the article, we examine experimental strategies aimed at reducing behavioral recovery after extinction of nonaversive experience, focusing on extinction learning in multiple contexts, reminder cues, and the informational value of contexts. In the second part, we report findings from human imaging studies and studies with rodents on the neural correlates of extinction and response recovery in nonaversive learning, with a focus on ventromedial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and neurotransmitter systems.

Zusammenfassung

Dieser Artikel gibt einen Überblick über verhaltens- und neurobiologische Aspekte der Verhaltenslöschung (Extinktion) mit einem Schwerpunkt auf nicht-aversive Lernerfahrungen. Die Löschung gelernten Verhaltens ist entscheidend für Wiederanpassungsleistungen an unsere Umwelt und spielt eine zentrale Rolle bei therapeutischen Interventionen. Gelöschtes Verhalten kann jedoch aufgrund von Kontextänderungen wieder auftreten. Im ersten Teil des Artikels stellen wir experimentelle Strategien vor, die darauf abzielen, das Wiedererstarken gelöschten Verhaltens zu reduzieren. Dabei stehen im Mittelpunkt die Extinktion in multiplen Kontexten, Erinnerungsreize und der Informationswert von Kontexten. Der zweite Teil liefert eine Übersicht über unsere Erkenntnisse zu neuronalen Korrelaten von Extinktion und Reaktionserholung, welche auf Studien zur Bildgebung beim Menschen und Studien mit Nagetieren beruhen. Hierbei liegt unser Schwerpunkt auf dem ventromedialen präfrontalen Kortex, dem Hippocampus und verschiedenen Neurotransmittersystemen.

Extinction and the role of context

Our environment is usually quite predictable: it does not rain when there is a cloudless sky; tasting your morning coffee is preceded by visual and olfactory perceptions of the beverage. Thus, certain events are related and often occur in a particular order. Humans and other animals are able to learn about event relationships, which allows us to predict future events based on the presence of preceding stimuli or actions (Lachnit et al., 2004; Melchers et al., 2005). This ability for associative learning is a considerable advantage for adaption and survival.

Classical conditioning and instrumental conditioning are two basic forms of associative learning. In classical conditioning (Pavlov, 1927), a neutral stimulus is repeatedly presented before a motivationally relevant outcome. As a result of these pairings, the neutral stimulus comes to elicit a response that indicates anticipation of the outcome. Consider Pavlov’s dog who salivated when hearing a bell that had been repeatedly presented before feeding. Instrumental conditioning (Skinner, 1938) reflects our ability to learn about the consequences of our actions. Reward or punishment that follows a behavior increases or decreases the probability with which that behavior will occur in the future.

Classical conditioning and instrumental conditioning are crucial for successful interactions with our environment. However, they are also involved in the development of maladaptive behavior. Both forms of associative learning play key roles for a variety of psychopathological disorders, including phobias, eating disorders, and drug abuse. Many therapeutic treatments aimed at overcoming maladaptive behavior are based on the principle of extinction learning (Craske et al., 2014; Podlesnik et al., 2017). When a stimulus or an action is no longer followed by the expected outcome, we will cease the acquired behavior: Pavlov’s dog will eventually stop salivating in response to the bell when subsequent feeding is repeatedly omitted; a patient’s fear of spiders will decrease significantly when repeatedly exposed to spiders in the absence of actual danger.

However, extinction of acquired behavior does not always endure. Rather, acquired responses have been observed to reappear after extinction under various conditions (Bouton, 1993; Bouton et al., 2012). An intriguing example is the renewal effect, which refers to the finding that changing the context in which a behavior was extinguished can restore (renew) the original response. In a typical renewal experiment, the conditioned response is first established in a particular context. Then, the acquired behavior is extinguished in a different context. During a final test, it has been observed that the original response reoccurs either when the individual is shifted to the context of initial conditioning or when the individual is exposed to a third, novel context (Bouton and Bolles, 1979). Renewal has also been observed when behavioral acquisition and extinction take place in the same context, but testing occurs in a different context (Bouton and Ricker, 1994). Analogous results have been reported for human associative learning with motivationally insignificant stimuli (Rosas and Callejas-Aguilera, 2006; Üngör and Lachnit, 2006, 2008). Thus, the absence of the context of extinction learning appears to be sufficient to induce a recovery of acquired behavior.

The renewal effect has rather challenging implications for therapeutic treatments involving extinction learning. It suggests that full expression of therapeutic success may be limited to the therapeutic environment: the likelihood of relapse increases outside the therapeutic setting.

Basic research has revealed several experimental strategies that reduce or even prevent the renewal effect. These findings may provide important insights for improving the long-term success of therapeutic interventions. One experimental strategy that has received considerable attention comprises extinction learning in multiple contexts (Craske et al., 2014; Laborda et al., 2011). However, experiments involving human associative learning (Bustamante et al., 2016b) and instrumental conditioning in rats (Bernal-Gamboa et al., 2017) have indicated that the impact of this strategy may depend on the type of renewal procedure: extinction in multiple contexts resulted in weaker response recovery than extinction in a single context, when testing for renewal occurred in a novel context. However, when the test took place in the context in which the response had been originally acquired, extinction in multiple contexts exerted no attenuating effect on renewal (Bernal-Gamboa et al., 2017; Bustamante et al., 2016b).

Another experimental strategy aimed to counter the renewal effect is the application of so-called reminder cues, which refer to discrete stimuli that are repeatedly presented during the extinction of a response. Using visual reminder cues in human associative learning (Bustamante et al., 2016a) and auditory reminder cues in instrumental conditioning with rats (Nieto et al., 2020), experiments have shown that the application of reminder cues during renewal testing in a novel context completely prevented the recovery of acquired responding. Although this level of effectiveness is not reached when testing occurs in the context of initial acquisition, reminder cues weaken the degree of response recovery in this test situation (Nieto et al., 2017).

The renewal effect is also influenced by experimental manipulations that target the informational value of contexts. For many cases, contexts have low informational value, in the sense that they are irrelevant for the relationship between events – the delicious taste after biting into an apple occurs regardless of whether you are at home or in your workplace. However, in other cases, the relationship between events varies across contexts – having a lively conversation is welcomed at a party, but the same behavior is considered inappropriate in a library. Thus, contexts can carry relevant information about the current relationship between events. Studies of human associative learning have revealed that response recovery after extinction is weaker when initial acquisition (Lucke et al., 2013) or extinction (Lucke et al., 2014) was conducted in a context that had been trained as being irrelevant for other stimulus-outcome relationships, compared with a context trained as being relevant. Measures of eye-gaze behavior (Lucke et al., 2013) and other experimental approaches (Uengoer et al., 2018) suggest that the impact of context information on context-dependent learning is based on processes of selective attention.

Brain regions involved in extinction and renewal of nonaversive experience

Extinction learning can comprise aversive/maladaptive (fear, phobias, addiction) or benign/appetitive elements. Extinction of aversive and maladaptive behavior has received the greatest degree of scrutiny to date, and it has become apparent that structures such as the amygdala, prefrontal cortex, and hippocampus play important roles in the processing of context in human subjects and in rodents during extinction of fear responses (Kalisch, 2006; Lang et al., 2009; Lingawi et al., 2019; Marek et al., 2019; Milad et al., 2007) and in fear renewal (Hermann et al., 2016). Extinction of appetitive, or nonaversive, learning in humans (Lissek et al., 2013) and rodents (Mendez-Couz et al., 2019) also involves the hippocampus.

Imaging studies investigating extinction related to nonaversive learning in humans (Figure 1) have demonstrated that the hippocampus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) mediate renewal of acquired behavior (Lissek et al., 2013). Both regions showed higher activation in participants who exhibited renewal than in those who did not: the hippocampus encoded context information during extinction, displaying even higher activation in response to a stimulus presented in a novel context, while the vmPFC retrieved this information during renewal testing to decide upon response recovery. Recent studies on rats have demonstrated that information processing in discrete hippocampal subfields contribute to specific elements of context-dependent acquisition, extinction, and renewal in an appetitive spatial learning task (Mendez-Couz et al., 2019; see Figure 1), indicating that the hippocampus may be intrinsically involved in determining the specificity of the learned response.

Figure 1: 
Paradigms for the study of extinction learning in humans or in rodents. A. In this paradigm, human subjects are presented with a sequence of trials each showing a compound of a food item (cue) and the name of a restaurant (context; e.g. “Zum Krug”). Each compound is associated with a specific outcome. Following an intertrial interval of 5–9 s, one cue/context compound is presented for 3 s. Then, a question appears asking the participant to predict whether consumption of the food in the restaurant will cause stomachache in a hypothetical patient, followed by a response period of maximally 4 s. Feedback, providing the correct answer, is then shown for 2 s (Golisch et al., 2017). B. The task comprises three phases: acquisition, extinction, and test. In the AAA condition, all phases occur in the same context, while in the ABA condition, the extinction context differs. During the test in both conditions, cues are presented in the same context as during acquisition (Golisch et al., 2017). C. Examples of food images used in the task (Golisch et al., 2017). D/E. In rodents, nonaversive extinction learning can be studied by examining associative spatial learning and memory. Over a period of days, rodents learn that a food reward can be found (with low probability) at a specific end of a T-Maze arm. The T-Maze has a specific floor pattern, and a mild odor is present at the end of both T-Maze arms and visuospatial cues are placed outside of the T-maze, in visible range. The food reward is hidden in an indentation in the floor near the end of the target arm. One day after the animals have reached at least 80% arm-choice accuracy, extinction learning is examined either in the presence (D) or absence (E) of a context change. Here, the floor pattern, odor cues, and distal visuospatial cues are changed. During extinction learning trials, no food reward is present. Renewal is assessed in the ABA paradigm (D) by returning the animals to the original context. In the AAA paradigm (E), animals are simply reexposed to the same context (André et al., 2015b; Mendez-Couz et al., 2019; Wiescholleck et al. 2014).
Figure 1:

Paradigms for the study of extinction learning in humans or in rodents. A. In this paradigm, human subjects are presented with a sequence of trials each showing a compound of a food item (cue) and the name of a restaurant (context; e.g. “Zum Krug”). Each compound is associated with a specific outcome. Following an intertrial interval of 5–9 s, one cue/context compound is presented for 3 s. Then, a question appears asking the participant to predict whether consumption of the food in the restaurant will cause stomachache in a hypothetical patient, followed by a response period of maximally 4 s. Feedback, providing the correct answer, is then shown for 2 s (Golisch et al., 2017). B. The task comprises three phases: acquisition, extinction, and test. In the AAA condition, all phases occur in the same context, while in the ABA condition, the extinction context differs. During the test in both conditions, cues are presented in the same context as during acquisition (Golisch et al., 2017). C. Examples of food images used in the task (Golisch et al., 2017). D/E. In rodents, nonaversive extinction learning can be studied by examining associative spatial learning and memory. Over a period of days, rodents learn that a food reward can be found (with low probability) at a specific end of a T-Maze arm. The T-Maze has a specific floor pattern, and a mild odor is present at the end of both T-Maze arms and visuospatial cues are placed outside of the T-maze, in visible range. The food reward is hidden in an indentation in the floor near the end of the target arm. One day after the animals have reached at least 80% arm-choice accuracy, extinction learning is examined either in the presence (D) or absence (E) of a context change. Here, the floor pattern, odor cues, and distal visuospatial cues are changed. During extinction learning trials, no food reward is present. Renewal is assessed in the ABA paradigm (D) by returning the animals to the original context. In the AAA paradigm (E), animals are simply reexposed to the same context (André et al., 2015b; Mendez-Couz et al., 2019; Wiescholleck et al. 2014).

In line with this, individuals with, and without, a propensity for renewal differ in context-related hippocampal activation not only during extinction but also during initial acquisition, where context is irrelevant (Lissek et al., 2016). All individuals – regardless of their propensity for renewal – showed increased activation of the posterior hippocampus in a novelty response to the presentation of only the context. However, only those participants with a propensity for renewal maintained this hippocampal activation when a cue was added to the context, indicating processing of the context/cue compound.

While the amygdala is consistently active during extinction of fear responses (Hermann et al., 2020; Merz et al., 2013), it is also active in extinction related to nonaversive experience (Lissek et al., 2013). The finding supports a proposed broader role of the amygdala in aversive and appetitive learning (Everitt et al., 2003; Knapska et al., 2006). Other regions previously shown to be involved in fear extinction (Sehlmeyer et al., 2009) that are regularly found to be active during nonaversive extinction learning comprise the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and insula, which exhibited higher activity in participants with a propensity for renewal (Lissek et al., 2013). This increased activity indicates that attentional processing mediated by the ACC and processing of salient events by the insula (Menon and Uddin, 2010) are more pronounced in these participants.

Neurotransmitter systems involved in extinction and renewal of nonaversive experience

The creation of associative memories depends on cortical and hippocampal plasticity processes that in turn critically depend on the activation and regulation of neurotransmitter receptor systems including glutamatergic N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors (Hansen et al., 2017), gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptors (Swanson and Maffei, 2019), and catecholaminergic receptors (Hagena et al., 2016; Hansen and Manahan-Vaughan, 2014). Although studies of nonaversive extinction learning are less numerous than the wealth of data available with regard to extinction of aversive learning, it is apparent that neurotransmitter receptors that are essential for cortical and synaptic plasticity serve to modulate the efficacy of extinction of nonaversive learning (Table 1).

Table 1:

Overview of the effect of treatment with neurotransmitter receptor ligands on nonaversive extinction learning.

Ligand Human Rodent Reference
NMDAR agonist enhances n.t. Golisch et al., 2017; Klass et al., 2017
NMDAR antagonist enhances impairs Goodmann et al., 2019
DA agonist enhances no effect Andrè and Manahan-Vaughan, 2016; Lissek et al., 2018
DA antagonist impairs D1/D5 enhances Andrè and Manahan-Vaughan, 2016; Lissek et al., 2015b
D2/D3 no effect
NA agonist enhances enhances Janak and Corbit, 2011; Lissek et al., 2015a
NA antagonist n.t. no effect André et al., 2015
GABA agonist impairs impairs Corcoran, 2005; Corcoran and Maren, 2001; Lissek et al., 2015a, 2017
  1. Note: DA: dopamine, GABA: gamma amino-butyric acid, NA: noradrenaline, NMDAR: N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor, n.t.: not tested.

Pharmacological manipulation of NMDA receptors modulated extinction related to nonaversive learning in human subjects when conducted within the same context as for initial acquisition: strikingly both the NMDA receptor agonist, d-cycloserine, DCS, (Klass et al., 2017) and the NMDA receptor antagonist, memantine (Golisch et al., 2017), enhanced extinction learning. This latter finding, which was associated with dose-related effects of memantine modulated by body mass index, suggests that fine-tuning of the degree of activation of NMDA receptors is a key facet of effective extinction learning. This may relate to a possible differential regulation, by the ligands used in these studies, of GluN2A- or GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors, which determine, in turn, the amplitude and persistency of synaptic plasticity (Ballesteros et al., 2016).

Research on extinction and renewal related to nonaversive learning in humans demonstrated a specific role for dopamine (DA) receptors for extinction learning in a novel context, whereas the DA antagonist, tiapride, when administered as a single dose before the extinction phase, impaired performance (Lissek et al., 2015b), and the DA agonist, bromocriptine, enhanced extinction learning, particularly in those individuals with a propensity for renewal (Lissek et al., 2018). The role of specific DA receptors was scrutinized in rodent experiments: Studies of extinction learning using a spatial appetitive task in rats demonstrated that dopamine acting on the D1/D5 receptor modulates both the acquisition and the consolidation of extinction learning. D2 receptors modulated context-independent aspects of extinction learning (André and Manahan-Vaughan, 2016).

The noradrenergic system also contributes to extinction learning. Administration of the noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor, atomoxetine, to human subjects (Lissek et al., 2015a) or to rats (Janak and Corbit, 2011) enhanced extinction in nonaversive or appetitive tasks. In rats, extinction learning within a spatial appetitive task was unaffected by antagonism of beta-adrenergic receptors (André et al., 2015), however, suggesting that either this process is supported by alpha-adrenergic receptors or attentional demand is a determinant of the involvement of the noradrenergic system in extinction learning. Consistent with the latter possibility, activation of beta-adrenergic receptors is required for extinction learning in the absence of a context change (André et al., 2015a). This latter process is also supported by metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluR; André et al., 2015b).

Extinction related to nonaversive learning in human subjects was impaired by pharmacological activation of GABA receptors with the agonist lorazepam, irrespective of the context in which extinction occurred (Lissek et al., 2015a, 2017). These results correspond to animal studies reporting impairments of extinction learning by local hippocampal GABA receptor agonism (Corcoran, 2005; Corcoran and Maren, 2001).

Consistent with the likelihood that extinction learning involves de novo encoding of associative experience (Mendez-Couz et al., 2019), enhanced hippocampal activation during extinction learning and renewal testing was observed after stimulation of noradrenergic, dopaminergic, or glutamatergic NMDA receptors in human subjects before extinction training. In contrast, hippocampal activity was reduced by dopaminergic antagonism and GABA agonism (Lissek et al., 2015a, 2015b, 2017). Activation of the vmPFC was enhanced by noradrenergic stimulation during extinction learning and by GABA agonism during renewal testing and reduced by DA antagonism during extinction in the acquisition context, but not in a novel one. NMDA or noradrenergic receptor activation increased activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and inferior frontal gyrus, whereas the DA receptor antagonism, GABA receptor activation, and NMDA receptor antagonism reduced activation. In addition, both noradrenergic and NMDA receptor stimulation increased ACC and insula activation in extinction and renewal testing, while GABA receptor agonism and the DA receptor antagonism reduced activation in these regions (Lissek et al., 2015a, 2015b; Klass et al., 2017).

Taken together, results obtained in pharmacological studies on humans and rodents indicate that during extinction learning, dopamine, acting in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus, is involved in readjusting the cue-outcome relationship in the presence of a novel context. Hippocampal dopamine is important for the encoding and provision of context information and is, thus, essentially involved in the renewal effect. In contrast, prefrontal and hippocampal NMDA receptors appear to be specifically involved in the modification of established stimulus-outcome associations in the context of initial acquisition. Moreover, the noradrenergic system is involved in the modification of established associations during extinction learning, regardless of context, underlining the supposed importance of attentional processes in extinction learning.

Catecholaminergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic regulation of extinction learning is not restricted to nonaversive experience. Noradrenaline acting on beta-adrenergic receptors in the amygdala impairs extinction of fear, whereas noradrenaline acting on alpha-adrenergic receptors in the prefrontal cortex enhances it (Likhtik and Johansen, 2019). Furthermore, the robustness of fear memory and consequently the effectiveness of extinction learning is regulated by dopamine release from the central tegmental area acting on key brain circuitry such as the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and amygdala (Likhtik and Johansen, 2019). GABAergic transmission and mGluR and NMDA receptor activity in these structures also modulate fear memory and fear extinction (Courtin et al., 2014; Kaplan and Moore, 2011; Myers et al., 2001; Walker and Davis, 2002).

In conclusion, despite their clear differences in terms of behavior and cognition, extinction learning of aversive and nonaversive experience shares many functional similarities in terms of the brain regions that are engaged by these processes and the neurotransmitter receptors that mediate the behavioral outcome. This suggests that knowledge gained through studies of processes that optimize extinction learning in an experimental setting harbors significant potential in translation into therapeutic strategies for maladaptive behavior.


Corresponding author: Metin Uengoer, Faculty of Psychology, Philipps-Universität Marburg, Gutenbergstraße 18, 35032 Marburg, Germany, E-mail:

Award Identifier / Grant number: FOR 1581 & SFB 1280

About the authors

Metin Uengoer

Metin Uengoer is an expert on Associative Learning. His research interests include extinction and contextual control of behavior, error-correction modelling, and attentional mechanisms of learning. He studied Psychology at Philipps-Universität Marburg, where he also received a doctoral degree in Natural Sciences. Between 2010 and 2011, and between 2015 and 2016, he was acting professor for Experimental and Biological Psychology at the Psychology Department of Philipps-Universität Marburg. He participated as a principal investigator in the DFG Research Unit 1581 between 2011 and 2017, and is currently working as a research fellow in the DFG Collaborative Research Centre 1280.

Silke Lissek

Silke Lissek studied Psychology and obtained her Ph.D. in Biopsychology at the Ruhr University Bochum. Since 2005 she works in the Neuroplasticity Imaging Group of the Department of Neurology, University Clinic Bergmannsheil, Ruhr University Bochum. Between 2010 and 2016 she was principal investigator in a project within the DFG research group FOR 1581, and since 2017 she is principal investigator in a project within the DFG Collaborative Research Centre 1280. Her research focuses on the neuronal basis of extinction learning and the renewal effect.

Martin Tegenthoff

Martin Tegenthoff studied medicine and physics at the University of Münster. Doctoral thesis in medicine (Dr. med.). Board certification in neurology and psychiatry, specializing in pain therapy and rehabilitation medicine. Habilitation in Neurology at Ruhr University Bochum. Constitution of an interdisciplinary scientific study group for neuronal/cortical plasticity (www.ruhr-uni-bochum.de/neuroplasticity/index.html.de) with an emphasis on clinical and experimental neurophysiology and MR-neuroimaging. Since 2010 medical director and chair of the Department of Neurology and Neurological Traumatology and Neurorehabilitation, University Clinic Bergmannsheil, Ruhr University Bochum.

Denise Manahan-Vaughan

Denise Manahan-Vaughan is a Neurophysiologist, Neuroscientist and Head of the Department of Neurophysiology within the Medical Faculty of the Ruhr University Bochum (www.rub.de/neurophys). She is also director and Dean of Studies of the International Graduate School of Neuroscience (www.rub.de/igsn) and speaker (primary coordinator) of the DFG-funded Collaborative Research Centre on Integration and Representation of Sensory Information Processes at the same university (www.rub.de/sfb874). After completion of her primary degree in Natural Sciences at Trinity College Dublin (TCD), Ireland, she obtained a PhD in Neuropharmacology at TCD and then moved to Germany to the Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology in Magdeburg. Having obtained an Habilitation degree in Physiology from the Otto von Guericke University in Magdeburg, she moved to the Johannes Müller Institute for Physiology at the Charité, Berlin, before moving to Bochum in 2003. Her research focusses on understanding the relationship between hippocampal and cortical synaptic plasticity and long-term associative and spatial memory.

Harald Lachnit

Prof. Dr. Harald Lachnit, Dipl.-Psych., studied psychology at the Justus-Liebig University of Giessen (1975–1981). He received his doctoral degree in psychology in 1985 (“Contextual Dependence of Classical Conditioning”) and habilitated there in 1991 with the topic “Cognition and Brain: Building an Experimental Bridge”. During his doctoral and habilitation period he spent 6 research stays in North America. From 1992 to 1994 he was Professor of General Psychology at Philipps-Universität Marburg. Since 1994 he has been working there as Professor of General and Biological Psychology. From 1992 to 1997, he temporarily headed the Department of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. From 2010 to 2016 he served as vice president for studies and teaching.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) through the research unit FOR 1581 (P2, P4, P6) and the collaborative research center SFB 1280 (project number 316803389; A04, A08, A15).

  1. Author contribution: All the authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this submitted manuscript and approved submission.

  2. Research funding: This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) through the research unit FOR 1581 (P2, P4, P6) and the collaborative research center SFB 1280 (project number 316803389; A04, A08, A15).

  3. Conflict of interest statement: The authors declare no conflicts of interest regarding this article.

References

André, M.E., and Manahan-Vaughan, D. (2016). Involvement of dopamine D1/D5 and D2 receptors in context-dependent extinction learning and memory reinstatement. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 125, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00372.Search in Google Scholar

André, M.E., Wolf, O., and Manahan-Vaughan, D. (2015a). Beta-adrenergic receptors support attention to extinction learning that occurs in the absence, but not the presence, of a context change. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 125. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00125.Search in Google Scholar

André, M.E., Güntürkün, O., and Manahan-Vaughan, D. (2015b). The metabotropic glutamate receptor, mGlu5 is required for extinction learning that occurs in the absence of a context change. Hippocampus, 25, 149–158, https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22359.Search in Google Scholar

Ballesteros, J.J., Buschler, A., Köhr, G., and Manahan-Vaughan, D. (2016). Afferent input selects NMDA receptor subtype to determine the persistency of hippocampal LTP in freely behaving mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 33, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2016.00033.Search in Google Scholar

Bernal-Gamboa, R., Nieto, J., and Uengoer, M. (2017). Effects of extinction in multiple contexts on renewal of instrumental responses. Behav. Process. 142, 64–69, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2017.06.003.Search in Google Scholar

Bouton, M.E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol. Bull. 114, 80–99, https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80.Search in Google Scholar

Bouton, M.E., and Bolles, R.C. (1979). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learn. Motiv. 10, 445–466, https://doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(79)90057-2.Search in Google Scholar

Bouton, M.E., and Ricker, S.T. (1994). Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context. Anim. Learn. Behav. 22, 317–324, https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03209840.Search in Google Scholar

Bouton, M.E., Winterbauer, N.E., and Todd, T.P. (2012). Relapse processes after the extinction of instrumental learning. Renewal, resurgence, and reacquisition. Behav. Process. 90, 130–141, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2012.03.004.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Bustamante, J., Uengoer, M., and Lachnit, H. (2016a). Reminder cues modulate the renewal effect in human predictive learning. Front. Psychol. 7, 80, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01968.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Bustamante, J., Uengoer, M., Thorwart, A., and Lachnit, H. (2016b). Extinction in multiple contexts: Effects on the rate of extinction and the strength of response recovery. Learn. Behav. 44, 283–294, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-016-0212-7.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Corcoran, K.A. (2005). Hippocampal inactivation disrupts the acquisition and contextual encoding of fear extinction. J. Neurosci. 25, 8978–8987, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2246-05.2005.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Corcoran, K.A., and Maren, S. (2001). Hippocampal inactivation disrupts contextual retrieval of fear memory after extinction. J. Neurosci. 21, 1720–6, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.21-05-01720.2001.Search in Google Scholar

Courtin, J., Chaudun, F., Rozeske, R.R., Karalis, N., Gonzalez-Campo, C., Wurtz, H., Abdi, A., Baufreton, J., Bienvenu, T.C., and Herry, C. (2014). Prefrontal parvalbumin interneurons shape neuronal activity to drive fear expression. Nature 505, 92–96, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12755.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Craske, M.G., Treanor, M., Conway, C.C., Zbozinek, T., and Vervliet, B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy. An inhibitory learning approach. Behav. Res. Ther. 58, 10–23, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Everitt, B.J., Cardinal, R.N., Parkinson, J.A., and Robbins, T.W. (2003). Appetitive behavior: impact of amygdala-dependent mechanisms of emotional learning. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 985, 233–50.10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07085.xSearch in Google Scholar

Goodman, J.., Hsu, E., Packard, M.G. (2019). NMDA receptors in the basolateral amygdala mediate acquisition and extinction of an amphetamine conditioned place preference. Neurosci 133, 428–436, https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000323.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Golisch, A., Heba, S., Glaubitz, B., Tegenthoff, M., and Lissek, S. (2017). Enhancing effects of NMDA-receptor blockade on extinction learning and related brain activation are modulated by BMI. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00034.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Hagena, H., Hansen, N., and Manahan-Vaughan, D. (2016). β-adrenergic control of hippocampal function: subserving the choreography of synaptic information storage and memory. Cerebral Cortex. 26, 1349–1364, https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv330.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Hansen, N., and Manahan-Vaughan, D. (2014). Dopamine D1/D5 receptors mediate informational saliency that promotes persistent hippocampal long-term plasticity. Cerebral Cortex. 24, 845–858, https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs362.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Hansen, K.B., Yi, F., Perszyk, R.E., Menniti, F.S., and Traynelis, S.F. (2017). NMDA receptors in the central nervous system. Methods Mol. Biol. 1677, 1–80, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-7321-7_1.Search in Google Scholar

Hermann, A., Stark, R., Milad, M.R., and Merz, C.J. (2016). Renewal of conditioned fear in a novel context is associated with hippocampal activation and connectivity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1411–1421, https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw047.Search in Google Scholar

Hermann, A., Stark, R., Müller, E.A., Kruse, O., Wolf, O.T., and Merz, C.J. (2020). Multiple extinction contexts modulate the neural correlates of context-dependent extinction learning and retrieval. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 168, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2019.107150.Search in Google Scholar

Janak, P.H., and Corbit, L.H. (2011). Deepened extinction following compound stimulus presentation: Noradrenergic modulation. Learn. Mem. 18, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.1923211.Search in Google Scholar

Kalisch, R. (2006). Context-dependent human extinction memory is mediated by a ventromedial prefrontal and hippocampal network. J. Neurosci. 26, 9503–9511, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2021-06.2006.Search in Google Scholar

Kaplan, G.B., and Moore, K.A. (2011). The use of cognitive enhancers in animal models of fear extinction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 99, 217–28, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2011.01.009.Search in Google Scholar

Klass, A., Glaubitz, B., Tegenthoff, M., and Lissek, S. (2017). D-Cycloserine facilitates extinction learning and enhances extinction-related brain activation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 144, 235–247, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2017.08.003.Search in Google Scholar

Knapska, E., Walasek, G., Nikolaev, E., Neuhäusser-Wespy, F., Lipp, H.P., Kaczmarek, L., and Werka, T. (2006). Differential involvement of the central amygdala in appetitive versus aversive learning. Learn. Mem. 13, 192–200, https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.54706.Search in Google Scholar

Laborda, M.A., McConnell, B.L., and Miller, R.R. (2011). Behavioral techniques to reduce relapse after exposure therapy: Applications of studies of experimental extinction. Associative Learning and Conditioning Theory: Human and Non-Human Applications. T. R. Schachtman and S. Reilly, eds. (Oxford Univ. Press), pp. 79–103.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199735969.003.0025Search in Google Scholar

Lachnit, H., Giurfa, M., and Menzel, R. (2004). Odor processing in honeybees: Is the whole equal to, more than, or different from the sum of its parts?. Adv. Study. Behav. 34, 241–264, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-3454(04)34006-4.Search in Google Scholar

Lang, S., Kroll, A., Lipinski, S.J., Wessa, M., Ridder, S., Christmann, C., Schad, L.R., and Flor, H. (2009). Context conditioning and extinction in humans: Differential contribution of the hippocampus, amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 823–832, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06624.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Likhtik, E., and Johansen, J.P. (2019). Neuromodulation in circuits of aversive emotional learning. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1586–1597, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0503-3.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Lingawi, N.W., Laurent, V., Westbrook, R.F., and Holmes, N.M. (2019). The role of the basolateral amygdala and infralimbic cortex in (re)learning extinction. Psychopharmacology. 236, 303–312, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-4957-x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Lissek, S., Glaubitz, B., Uengoer, M., and Tegenthoff, M. (2013). Hippocampal activation during extinction learning predicts occurrence of the renewal effect in extinction recall. Neuroimage 81, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.025.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Lissek, S., Glaubitz, B., Güntürkün, O., and Tegenthofl, M. (2015a). Noradrenergic stimulation modulates activation of extinction-related brain regions and enhances contextual extinction learning without affecting renewal. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00034.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Lissek, S., Glaubitz, B., Wolf, O.T., and Tegenthoff, M. (2015b). The DA antagonist tiapride impairs context-related extinction learning in a novel context without affecting renewal. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00238.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Lissek, S., Glaubitz, B., Schmidt-Wilcke, T., and Tegenthoff, M. (2016). Hippocampal context processing during acquisition of a predictive learning task is associated with renewal in extinction recall. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00928.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Lissek, S., Golisch, A., Glaubitz, B., and Tegenthoff, M. (2017). The GABAergic system in prefrontal cortex and hippocampus modulates context-related extinction learning and renewal in humans. Brain Imaging Behav. 11, 1885–1900, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9662-y.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Lissek, S., Glaubitz, B., Klass, A., and Tegenthoff, M. (2018). The effects of dopaminergic D2-like receptor stimulation upon behavioral and neural correlates of renewal depend on individual context processing propensities. Neuroimage 169, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.022.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Lucke, S., Lachnit, H., Koenig, S., and Uengoer, M. (2013). The informational value of contexts affects context-dependent learning. Learn. Behav. 41, 285–297, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-013-0104-z.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Lucke, S., Lachnit, H., Stüttgen, M.C., and Uengoer, M. (2014). The impact of context relevance during extinction learning. Learn. Behav. 42, 256–269, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-014-0143-0.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Marek, R., Sun, Y., and Sah, P. (2019). Neural circuits for a top-down control of fear and extinction. Psychopharmacology 236, 313–320, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-5033-2.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Melchers, K.G., Lachnit, H., Üngör, M., and Shanks, D.R. (2005). Prior experience can influence whether the whole is different from the sum of its parts. Learn. Motiv. 36, 20–41, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2004.06.002.Search in Google Scholar

Mendéz-Couz, M., Becker, J.M., and Manahan-Vaughan, D. (2019). Functional compartmentalization of the contribution of hippocampal subfields to context-dependent extinction learning. Front. Beh. Neurosci. 13, 256, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00256.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Menon, V., and Uddin, L.Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655–667, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Merz, C.J., Hermann, A., Stark, R., and Wolf, O.T. (2013). Cortisol modifies extinction learning of recently acquired fear in men. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1426–1434, https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst137.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Milad, M.R., Wright, C.I., Orr, S.P., Pitman, R.K., Quirk, G.J., and Rauch, S.L. (2007). Recall of fear extinction in humans activates the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in concert. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 446–454, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.011.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Myers, K.M., Carlezon, W.A. Jr., and Davis, M. (2011). Glutamate receptors in extinction and extinction-based therapies for psychiatric illness. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 274–293, https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.88.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Nieto, J., Uengoer, M., and Bernal-Gamboa, R. (2017). A reminder of extinction reduces relapse in an animal model of voluntary behavior. Learn. Mem. 24, 76–80, https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.044495.116.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Nieto, J., Mason, T. A., Bernal-Gamboa, R., & Uengoer, M. (2020). The impacts of acquisition and extinction cues on ABC renewal of voluntary behaviors. Learn. Mem. 27, 114–118, https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.050831.119.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex (London: Oxford Univ. Press).Search in Google Scholar

Podlesnik, C.A., Kelley, M.E., Jimenez-Gomez, C., and Bouton, M.E. (2017). Renewed behavior produced by context change and its implications for treatment maintenance. A review. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 50, 675–697, https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.400.Search in Google Scholar

Rosas, J.M., and Callejas-Aguilera, J.E. (2006). Context switch effects on acquisition and extinction in human predictive learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32, 461–474, https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.461.Search in Google Scholar

Sehlmeyer, C., Schöning, S., Zwitserlood, P., Pfleiderer, B., Kircher, T., Arolt, V., and Konrad, C. (2009). Human fear conditioning and extinction in neuroimaging: a systematic review. PLoS One 4, e5865, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0005865.Search in Google Scholar

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: an experimental analysis. Appleton-Century.Search in Google Scholar

Swanson, O.K., and Maffei, A. (2019). From hiring to firing: activation of inhibitory neurons and their recruitment in behavior. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 3, 168.10.3389/fnmol.2019.00168Search in Google Scholar

Uengoer, M., Lucke, S., and Lachnit, H. (2018). Attention toward contexts modulates context-specificity of behavior in human predictive learning. Evidence from the n-back task. Learn. Behav. 46, 320–326, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-018-0318-1.Search in Google Scholar

Üngör, M., and Lachnit, H. (2006). Contextual control in discrimination reversal learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process 32, 441–453, https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.32.4.441.Search in Google Scholar

Üngör, M., and Lachnit, H. (2008). Dissociations among ABA, ABC, and AAB recovery effects. Learn. Motiv. 39, 181–195, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2007.08.001.Search in Google Scholar

Walker, D.L., and Davis, M. (2002). The role of amygdala glutamate receptors in fear learning, fear-potentiated startle, and extinction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 71, 379–392. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00698-0.Search in Google Scholar

Wiescholleck, V., André, M., and Manahan-Vaughan, D. (2014). Early age-dependent impairments of context-dependent extinction learning, object recognition, and object-place learning occur in rats. Hippocampus 24, 270–279, https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22220.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Published Online: 2020-08-17
Published in Print: 2020-09-25

© 2020 Metin Uengoer et al., published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 18.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/nf-2020-0013/html
Scroll to top button