Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychiatry, 08 January 2020
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Progress in Translational Neuroimaging: Integrating Pathways, Systems and Phenomenology in Neurology and Psychiatry View all 20 articles

Current Challenges in Translational and Clinical fMRI and Future Directions

  • 1Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 2Mohn Medical Imaging and Visualization Centre, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway
  • 3Department of Education, UiT/The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway

Translational neuroscience is an important field that brings together clinical praxis with neuroscience methods. In this review article, the focus will be on functional neuroimaging (fMRI) and its applicability in clinical fMRI studies. In the light of the “replication crisis,” three aspects will be critically discussed: First, the fMRI signal itself, second, current fMRI praxis, and, third, the next generation of analysis strategies. Current attempts such as resting-state fMRI, meta-analyses, and machine learning will be discussed with their advantages and potential pitfalls and disadvantages. One major concern is that the fMRI signal shows substantial within- and between-subject variability, which affects the reliability of both task-related, but in particularly resting-state fMRI studies. Furthermore, the lack of standardized acquisition and analysis methods hinders the further development of clinical relevant approaches. However, meta-analyses and machine-learning approaches may help to overcome current shortcomings in the methods by identifying new, and yet hidden relationships, and may help to build new models on disorder mechanisms. Furthermore, better control of parameters that may have an influence on the fMRI signal and that can easily be controlled for, like blood pressure, heart rate, diet, time of day, might improve reliability substantially.

Introduction

Translational neuroscience is an important branch within the broad field of neuroscience. In the context of this opinion article, translational neuroscience will be seen as the attempt of bridging neuroscience, neuroimaging, and clinics for improving our understanding of symptoms and disorders, and for better diagnostics and treatments (1, 2).

This is not a new attempt. Neuroimaging, and in particular functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), has been considered as a revolutionary tool for exploring the healthy and the diseased brain for more than two decades (3, 4). Consequently, since fMRI entered the scene in the early 1990s, it had seen an enthusiastic phase over the first two decades. However, after this period, neuroimaging—like almost any other psychological and medical sciences—was overrun by the replication crisis. Recent studies have estimated the reproducibility of psychological studies to be 39% or less and indicated a severe limitation of neuroimaging (fMRI) study reliability (59). Furthermore, the neurophysiological mechanisms behind the BOLD/fMRI signal are only partly understood, which makes it difficult to generalise results or to use it on an individual level for diagnostic purposes. Thereby impeding the impact of highly needed neuroscience studies on theoretical and methodological progress, and, last but not least, the clinical application of fMRI.

In the following, this article will critically discuss current strategies and developments within the field of neuroimaging and tries to indicate possible future directions.

The Replication Crisis and Its Consequences

The neuroimaging research community has taken the “replication crisis” very seriously, like through the ReproNim initiative (10), and the Organisation of Human Brain Mapping (OHBM) announced in 2016 a new replication award, and put reproducibility high up on their agenda with several new best practice and data sharing initiatives (see, e.g., http://www.ohbmbrainmappingblog.com).

Jointly, psychology and neuroimaging suffer substantially from a lack of statistical power, meaning that the sample sizes are typically too small, and effect sizes are too low (11). This has not only been perceived as a critical challenge among scientists but has recently also received public attention. On the other hand, clinical applications require reliable single-case examinations but not group studies that may reliably show the general population effect but may vanish the information on interindividual and intraindividual variability. Consequently, the lack of information about the “naturally” occurring variability hinders the successful development of translational and clinical applications. Already in 2006, Paul Matthews and coworkers critically discussed the applicability of clinical fMRI for other applications than neurosurgical mapping (12). Although they wrote down their opinion more than a decade ago, it appears like that the development of clinical fMRI is in a “resting state,” as recently pointed out by O’Connor and Zeffiro (13). Presurgical mapping is still the only reliable and widely used clinical application of fMRI. The critical question is, why haven’t we yet achieved a breakthrough in clinical fMRI?

Current Status

This article will critically discuss three aspects that are relevant to consider in the context of clinical fMRI: First, the fMRI signal itself, second, current fMRI praxis, and, third, the next generation of analysis strategies.

The Bold Signal Perturbation

One of the major knowledge gaps in the field is the assumption that the fMRI signal, i.e., the underlying BOLD effect (BOLD = blood oxygenation level dependent), is sufficiently reliable and stable, where “sufficiently” has never been defined yet. It is of crucial importance to keep in mind that the BOLD signal represents only an indirect measure of neuronal activity, through a cascade of physiological processes, called neurovascular coupling. Consequently, the observed variability of the BOLD signal does not necessarily justify the conclusion that the underlying neuronal activity shows variability to the same degree. Scientifically speaking, the BOLD signal is a physiological response that only indirectly reflects neuronal activity, and which is easily and directly influenced by blood pressure, blood oxygenation, or any other parameters that have an effect on the vascular system, which in turn affect the balloon effect that generates the BOLD signal (14, 15). The corresponding balloon model became the most influential and mostly used model in fMRI research (1517). It is a neurophysiological model that describes the neuronal and vascular mechanisms that cause the BOLD signal given a neuronal activity. It rests on the assumption that the BOLD signal is caused by changes in the blood volume, blood flow, and the oxygen extraction rate. It is widely accepted that these are the main parameters that determine the strength of the BOLD signal. The balloon model and its corresponding hemodynamic response function is, for example, an integral part of several analysis models of fMRI data, but also for measures of functional and effective connectivity, like dynamic causal modelling (DCM) (18).

However, it is less studied, how susceptible the BOLD signal is to endogenous and exogenous influences and individual variability of the underlying mechanisms. Hence, it might occur that a change in the BOLD signal is detected while the true neuronal activity and connectivity remains unchanged. It is known that hormones (like cortisol), blood pressures, body mass index, time of the day (circadian rhythm), time of the year, sleep duration, and age influence blood volume, blood flow, and other vascular parameter, and hence the BOLD signal (1923). Whether the individual variability of these parameters has a significant influence on the BOLD signal is largely unknown. To give another example, using magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), it has been shown that the individually varying concentration of the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA is reflected in the amplitude and shape of the BOLD signal (24). Complementary, comparable effects have been shown for the excitatory neurotransmitter glutamate (25, 26). The list of those endogenous parameters can be continued, including parameter that may predominantly affect neuronal signal transmission or vascular processes. In other words, the BOLD signal is most likely not stable within and not necessarily comparable between subjects. These factors are just additional sources of variability of the fMRI signal that comes in addition to all other sources of noise that are affecting the measurement, like other environmental factors, thermal noise, noise of the measurement system itself, movements of the subjects, daylight length, temperature, and whether, to name a few, that may affect brain functions but also the stability of the MR system (23, 27, 28).

Current fMRI Methods

Current Clinical Applications of fMRI

As outlined above, the only routinely used clinical application of fMRI is the presurgical mapping (see Table 1). This is mostly done in patients with brain tumours or epilepsy, since these diseases may cause substantial displacement of brain functions, and functional mapping with fMRI may help surgeons to localize important areas despite their unusual neuroanatomical localization (29). However, most of those clinical applications are task-related fMRI with simple paradigms, and the first reports date far back to the beginning of fMRI (30, 31).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Schematic overview on reliability and current clinical applications of the different fMRI techniques.

One of the most common application is the localization of language areas and their lateralization, which is an essential information in the treatment of patients with epilepsy (3238). In most occasions, this clinical application of fMRI shows comparable results as the invasive WADA test but might deviate in cases with atypical language dominance (39).

Good experience also exists for paradigms probing the localization of motor, sensory, and memory functions, which are often not only used for localization but also for predicting outcome (4044). Nowadays, clinical fMRI is often combined with diffusion tensor imaging for localizing relevant fibre tracts (42).

In contrast to the presurgical mapping where it is sufficient enough to localize a function, any application in psychiatry, for instance, needs to focus on the strength of activations. Accordingly, there exists no routinely used clinical application of fMRI outside of the field of presurgical mapping due to the lack of sufficient reliability in the measurement of individual activation strength—for example task, instruction, and different levels of attention may influence reliability (45, 46). Group studies, by contrast, show a much higher reliability in detecting deviations in activation strength (47). Therefore, almost exclusively all fMRI studies in psychiatry have explored cohorts of patients (see Table 1). However, one possible way to circumvent this lack of reliability on the individual level has been recently suggested by Paek et al. in connection with a study on dementia by proposing repeated (baseline) measurements of the patients (48).

Another problem in clinical fMRI in psychiatry is the heterogeneity of patient populations. The disorders are often spectrum disorders with a continuum that ranges from normality to pathological (49), but also that varies between various symptoms and diagnosis, like between schizoaffective disorder, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder (50). Furthermore, the disorders often manifest in varies subtypes, and different studies may use different diagnostic criteria. Consequently, imaging results often differ substantially even on the level of group studies (51).

Is Resting-State fMRI the Solution?

While the first two decades of fMRI were mostly dominated by task-related fMRI, i.e., fMRI acquisitions while research subjects performed an active task, like a working memory, attention, or language task, the more recent years have seen an alternative approach, which is called “resting-state” fMRI (rs-fMRI). Here, research subjects are just scanned over a certain period without any concrete, active task—they are presumably “at rest.” Surprisingly, the measured BOLD signal that is measured during such an rs-fMRI examination is not random but fluctuates in a spatially and temporally systematic manner (52, 53). It has been shown that even in the absence of a concrete task, certain brain areas are forming networks through characteristic correlated fluctuations of the BOLD signal, called resting-state or intrinsic networks. These network patterns can be detected by focusing on low frequent (<0.01Hz) fluctuations of the BOLD signal, since these fluctuations propagate through the underlying neuronal network structures, indicating an information exchange within the networks even in the absence of a concrete task. It has been further shown that these networks are very similar across individuals (52, 53). They are therefore assumed to reflect some fundamental —trait- or biomarker-like —brain processes.

From resting-state data, it is possible to identify neuronal networks that show in their spatial organization a striking similarity with those networks that have been identified through task-related fMRI (54). These networks are often identifiable also on single subject levels, but depends on the method that is used for extracting the information (46). Since this discovery, there has been a tremendously interest in resting-state fMRI and the examination of the related intrinsic-brain networks and their dynamics (17, 5562). One of the most investigated networks in this respect is the “default mode network” (DMN) (61, 63). The DMN network is related to processes, like mind wandering, intrinsically focused attention, daydreaming, etc. Interestingly, there is a counterpart to the DMN, which has been described under different names in the literature. Here, it will be called the “extrinsic mode network” (EMN) and represents a network for extrinsically focused attention (61).

One reason why rs-fMRI became such a popular tool in the field of neuroimaging is that it may allow studying cognitive functions even in the absence of a task, which would be an intriguing possibility for doing clinical fMRI, especially in cases where patients are severely affected like after a stroke or traumatic brain injury. Clinical applications of rs-fMRI are based on the assumption of certain interindividual and intraindividual stability of resting-state networks in healthy individuals to draw conclusions from observed deviations in patients. In order to increase comparability and to limit variability in data acquisition between studies, first sets of guidelines for standardized protocols have been developed (e.g., like the “Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative” (ADNI); http://adni.loni.usc.edu), and other initiatives are following their example and have started similar undertakings.

However, one major disadvantage of rs-fMRI is that rs-fMRI studies still vary in their acquisition methods and whether they are conducted on a 1.5T, 3T, or 7T MR. The typical approach in rs-fMRI is to do an fMRI scan of several minutes duration with a repetition time (TR) of typically 1–3 s. But between studies and labs, there are already at least three different types of instructions, asking the research subjects to either close their eyes, keep them open, or to fixate on a fixation cross. Although the differences between these three possible instructions are moderate, they are still measurable (64). Interestingly, the most reliable results for most but not all examined networks were achieved when subjects fixated on a fixation cross. It is, however, difficult to control how well an individual followed that instruction as eye-tracking devices or eye cameras are typically not installed inside of an MR scanner and especially not in clinical MR scanners. Furthermore, different TR times may also cause varying results, since periodic signals like heart rate variability or respiration rate might affect results differently (65, 66). Another factor that varies between different studies and also influences the results is the duration of the resting-state examination that roughly varies between a few minutes and up to 12 min and more. The reliability of specific rs-fMRI seems to improve with scan durations, and acceptable good reliability for both intrasession and intersession rs-fMRI might be around 12 min (67).

Furthermore, there are also still no standards of how rs-fMRI data should be analysed. Previous studies have applied a wide spectrum of rs-fMRI analysis strategies, with varying levels of reliability (46, 68). But progress has been made in standardizing some of the procedures for achieving across-site comparability (69). It is, however, beyond the scope of this article to review all the different methods, but, just as an example, it has been shown that different methods do have different reliability, like measures of the static functional connectivity networks against the temporal dynamics of these networks (70).

In addition, rs-fMRI studies are based on the assumption of the inherent stability of the underlying resting-state networks across time and individuals. In other words, one assumes a low intrasubject and intersubject variability with high sensitivity to clinical deviations. This assumption has, however, never been thoroughly tested and might not be justifiable. There are only sparse and inconsistent reports that resting-states are indeed resting-traits (71), while the majority of reports point out that intraindividual variation can depend on environmental and psychological effects (7274). Another source of variability is the time of the day and time of the year. In an effortful longitudinal study of a single subject over 3.5 years, Choe et al. could show that there were systematic variations with a “significant linear trend, annual periodicity, and persistence” (75). Others have found that resting-state activity varies with the circadian rhythm (76), sleep duration (22), prior events (72), or mood (73). But also the metabolic state of hunger against satiety has a measurable effect on various resting-state measures (7779)—and the list of factors influencing rs-fMRI and/or the BOLD signal could be continued endlessly.

In summary, while acquisition methods and analysis strategies can be standardized, it will become challenging to control for additional endogenous and exogenous factors in a daily clinical routine. Although all mentioned factors might only have a moderate effect on resting-state measures, in the light of clinical applications, they may be in the same range that differentiates between patients and healthy controls. It is therefore questionable whether rs-fMRI will ever make it into a clinical tool. One might speculate, whether the reliability of task-related fMRI, with concrete tasks that requires focusing the attention, might be more superior and more suitable for clinical application (36, 45, 80).

Table 1 gives a schematic overview on reliability and current clinical applications of task-related and resting-state fMRI, separated for both single subject and group-level studies.

Next-Generation Data Analyses

Are Meta-Analyses the Solution?

In the light of increasing computational power, cloud computing, and open-access databases with thousands of datasets, meta-analyses became increasingly popular. Meta-analyses are a suitable tool for examining general network structures for a given cognitive task, and which areas, on average, show deviating effects in large patient populations. They may become important cornerstones for building new and more fine-grained models for various disorders.

But, like any emerging new method, the methods behind meta-analyses of large datasets are still under development and standards needs to be established (81). This implies that meta-analyses are not necessarily comparable and may suffer from the publication bias (82). This has been a known issue for decades since it has been noted that meta-analyses and randomized control studies may show different results (83), but methods are under development that control for potential biases (84). Furthermore, pure meta-analyses may not be the most sufficient way to go, since they often provide us only with very general common-sense solutions, that do not go much beyond to those functional lesion maps that already have been drawn in the first half of the last century (85, 86). As a side note, already then, the posterior cingulate cortex has been associated with self-awareness, which is nowadays called the “default mode network.” Or, another example, there is a striking functional and structural similarity between Kleist maps, based on brain lesions and injuries, and the meta-analysis of neuroimaging data on language functions (87).

In summary, meta-analyses are important contributors in revising and updating our understanding of the structural and functional organization of cognitive functions, and how structure and function interact. Focusing either only on lesions or only on fMRI results may not be sufficient enough for building new and more integrative and holistic theories of brain functions and sources of brain disorders. One way of achieving this is, for example, joining structural lesion maps with results from functional imaging within one multivariate analysis (8890).

Is Machine Learning the Solution?

Over the most recent years, meta-analyses have been supplemented with machine- and deep-learning methods that can extract (partly hidden) information out of the data and may be able to detect a pattern that is not observable otherwise. The main characteristic that differentiates deep learning from other classifier approaches, e.g., for identifying subpopulations in a multimodal data space, is that features are learned automatically and do need a feature selection as a preceding step, which removes subjectivity and substantially improves accuracy (91). Deep learning has shown superior performance in detecting cross-modality relations and has attracted a substantial amount of attention among researchers from various fields. Furthermore, it has been nominated as one of the “10 breakthrough technologies” by MIT Technology Reviews (https://tinyurl.com/zx82sg5). Another advantage of deep-learning methods are their depth and breadth in model building, which may uncover hidden relations between factors that are of relevance for future clinical applications of fMRI in psychiatry (9294).

However, one potential problem with machine-learning approaches might be, however, the problem of overfitting which may compromise generalization of the results (95). Overfitting means that the algorithm finds a solution that perfectly parameterises the given dataset but may fail to classify new data correctly. One reason for overfitting is the use of too-small sample sizes as training data (96). But the field of machine learning has been hit by the replication crisis, as well (97). This is most like caused by insufficiently shared code and (training) data. Accordingly, the use of machine- and deep-learning methods is only justifiable in combination with large-scale open-access databases and open-source software.

In summary, machine-learning approaches are a promising move toward new discoveries of hidden relationships. Once reliable patterns have been identified and validated across different databases, one could expect that these approaches will bring us much closer to clinical applications of single-subject fMRI, as they may allow identifying fingerprints for certain disorders. But, it is still a long way to go until we will see clinical fMRI for diagnostic purposes, since, despite promising progress, the most recent developments are still in their infancies.

Discussion: the Next Steps in Translational Neuroscience

Translation neuroscience is a rapidly expanding field. If one takes the term literally, it means translating one concept from one domain or scientific discipline into another. In the field of neuroscience, it actually could be thought both ways, either translating a clinical concept into something that is measurable with neuroscientific methods and, the other way around, translating results from neuroscience into clinical praxis. The first way would lead to a better understanding of neurobiological underpinnings of a disorder, while the other way focuses more on the benefits of the patients. However, the issues raised above have to be taken into account for any translational research, whether it is for explorative purpose or diagnostics. The replication crisis might have triggered a new way of think and further attempts to exploring underlying mechanisms. Especially the recent years has seen an increasing interest in exploring all kind of endogenous and exogenous factors that might influence not only brain functions but also the mechanisms that generate the BOLD signal. Some of them have been discussed above, but the list of influencing factors is far from being complete. Moreover, it should highlight that there are indeed several processes that can affect the physiological and vascular basis of the BOLD signal but not necessarily the underlying neuronal mechanisms and activations, like the current blood pressure. Other factors, by contrast, might have a systematic effect on brain functions but do not have anything to do with the neurological or psychiatric disorder that should be examined, like the current phase of the circadian rhythm or the time point of the last meal. Or other factors are purely technical, like temperature, technical noise of the MR system. The influence of these factors might be boosted by the combination with nonstandardized fMRI acquisitions, different instructions (e.g., in rs-fMRI eyes open, eyes closed), nonstandardized analysis methods, less suitable algorithms. While there is an increasing number of reports recommending larger-sample sizes (n > 100) for improving reliability (98), other attempts are needed to improve single-subject fMRI (99), which are compulsory for pushing forward clinical fMRI.

Concerning the aspect of larger sample-sizes, this is mandatory for basic research of neuronal correlates of neurological and psychiatric disorders. Those studies are needed for building models and testing hypothesis of the source of a disorder and its progression. This needs a clear conceptualization of what neurobiological or cognitive components may cause the disorder, and how they can be measured with, for example, neuroimaging. While in some disorders, this might be a rather trivial endeavour, psychiatric disorders are often lacking such a definite relationship. However, machine-learning and big-data approaches may help uncovering hidden relationships and are promising strategies in current research applications (94). An often seen problem within fMRI studies is the huge overlap of results across different cognitive tasks and domains. As pointed out by Hugdahl and coworkers, the fronto-parietal attention network, aka EMN, is virtually activated every time the attention is focused on an extrinsic task (61). Such an unspecific but the fMRI results dominating activation pattern is difficult to interpret in the light of psychiatric disorders where problems may arise in one particular cognitive domain but not in others. Group studies and meta-analyses may provide the sufficient power to study also subtle effects within the EMN that may relate to psychiatric disorders, but the back translation into diagnostic attempts are difficult to achieve. However, one might also have to rethink the concept of certain disorders. Many psychiatric disorders are nowadays formulated and specified as spectrum disorders, forming a gradient from healthy to severely affected with perhaps varying probabilities of certain comorbidities, with unclear aetiologies. Accordingly, it is less likely that one is able to identify one single spot in the brain or one single deviation in a biomarker that causes this particular set of symptoms. Furthermore, there are often unclear and not directly related functional-structural relationships. For example, functional differences seen in rs-fMRI data from patients with depression (100) may not directly correspond to deviations seen in structural data (101). Accordingly, the cognitive and neurobiological models of psychiatric disorders may take into account that the spectrum of a disorder has multiple sources and that structural and functional causes may or may not depend on each other. Unfortunately and despite uncountable neuroimaging studies and meta-analyses, neuroimaging results have yet not been used for redefining and specifying diagnostic categories, as, for example, specified by the DSM5 (4).

Concerning the translation from neuroimaging results in clinical praxis, i.e., using fMRI for diagnostics, this is an even more difficult attempt. As outlined earlier, clinical fMRI is routinely used only for presurgical mapping but not for diagnostic purposes per se, and in particular not for diagnostic purposes in psychiatric disorders. The fMRI signal is too easily affected by many different endogenous and exogenous factors that are difficult to control. Even with standardized acquisition and analysis protocols, substantial and clinically irrelevant variations in individual fMRI results will be still present. Moreover, these variations are most likely at the same magnitude as the deviation from the mean that one would expect in a patient. This problem is further amplified by the fact that many disorders are spectrum disorders with gradual deviations. However, advanced machine-learning approaches that have been applied to large databases achieved, for example, for the classification of autism spectrum disorders already accuracies of 70% (102) to 90% (103). Interestingly, both studies used the same dataset but different algorithms, indicating that the selection of the algorithms can bias the results. Furthermore, there has not been reached a consensus yet, which algorithms are superior or recommended for fMRI data in general or for the classification of specific disorders in particular. But further methodological progress and better standardizations can be expected in the near future.

Besides using fMRI for diagnostics and classification of patients, there have also been attempts at using fMRI for the development of drugs and validating of drug effects (104). However, also this field of translational neuroscience still suffers from the replication crisis, publication bias, and the lack of standard acquisition and analysis methods. Hence, there is currently only very limited applicability of fMRI for this purpose, as well.

Conclusion

In conclusion, developments and progress have been and will be made in all domains, covered by this article. The replication crisis has pushed the development of new strategies, like the ReproNim initiative, that will help to standardize acquisition and analysis pipelines. Furthermore, the increased computational power and the continually growing number of available open-access databases with large sample-sizes and longitudinal data will allow the generation of “norm”-databases that can describe the distribution and interindividual variability of cognitive functions and network structures. Longitudinal data that are increasingly available will also give a better picture of disease progression. Machine-learning approaches will become better and more reliable in identifying disorders from multiple sources. All these approaches may lead to redefinitions of symptoms and may give a clearer picture of the causes of various spectrum disorders. Whether this will finally lead to clinical fMRI as a diagnostic tool is difficult to predict, since the variability of the BOLD signal is still an unresolved issue. In light of that, rs-fMRI currently does not appear as a tool that shows sufficient reliability and stability within and between subjects. The most reliable way of conducting rs-fMRI might be in combination with a fixation task and at least 12-min scan duration. By contrast, task-related fMRI that require focused attention of the subject might have better reliability and hence predictive value on the single-0subject level but would require a careful selection of clinically relevant paradigms. Here, better theoretical models have to be developed for translating clinical concepts into meaningful fMRI paradigms. Furthermore, it would be beneficial to acquire the data (in particular rs-fMRI) approximately at the same time of the day, and a sleeping and diet protocol could explain further, but irrelevant variability. Meta-analyses, in turn, might help in identifying precisely clinical concepts. In essence, after almost three decades, fMRI has generated substantial new insights into neurological and psychiatric disorders. It has produced a vast amount of data and triggered the development of new methods both for data acquisition and data analysis. Although the reliability of fMRI is still limited and hinders its use for diagnostic purposes in a daily clinical routine, the field of translational neuroscience is continuously moving toward more standardized, more reliable, and more clinical relevant applications of fMRI.

In essence, it is not unlikely to expect that clinical fMRI will at one point go beyond its current presurgical application and toward more diagnostic applications. This will be achieved by improved and standardized methods, better understanding of the neurovascular-coupling mechanisms, and revised models of psychiatric disorders.

Author Contributions

The author KS did the literature research and wrote the paper without any further assistance.

Funding

KS is funded through a grant by the Research Council of Norway (276044/ “When default is not default: Solutions to the replication crisis and beyond”).

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling editor declared a past co-authorship with the author KS.

References

1. Insel TR. A bridge to somewhere. Trans Psychiatry (2011) 1:e2–2. doi: 10.1038/tp.2011.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Marková IS. Translational neuroscience and psychiatry: a conceptual analysis. J Eval Clin Pract (2018) 24:791–6. doi: 10.1111/jep.12914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bandettini PA. Twenty years of functional MRI: the science and the stories. NeuroImage (2012) 62:575–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Rosen BR, Savoy RL. fMRI at 20: has it changed the world? NeuroImage (2012) 62:1316–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Maxwell SE, Lau MY, Howard GS. Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? Am Psychol (2015) 70:487–98. doi: 10.1037/a0039400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Open Science Collaboration. PSYCHOLOGY. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science (2015) 349:aac4716–aac4716. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Stroebe W, Strack F. The alleged crisis and the illusion of exact replication. Perspect Psychol Sci (2014) 9:59–71. doi: 10.1177/1745691613514450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. van Bavel JJ, Mende-Siedlecki P, Brady WJ, Reinero DA. Contextual sensitivity in scientific reproducibility. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA (2016), 113:6454–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1521897113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Strack F. From data to truth in psychological science. A personal perspective. Front Psychol (2017) 8:181–3. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kennedy DN. (2019). Everything matters: The ReproNim perspective on reproducible neuroimaging. fninf-13-00001.tex, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2019.00001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Turner BO, Paul EJ, Miller MB, Barbey AK. Small sample sizes reduce the replicability of task-based fMRI studies. Commun Biol (2018a) 1:62–10. doi: 10.1038/s42003-018-0073-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Matthews PM, Honey GD, Bullmore ET. Applications of fMRI in translational medicine and clinical practice. Nat Rev Neurosci (2006) 7:732–44. doi: 10.1038/nrn1929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. O’Connor EE, Zeffiro TA. Why is clinical fMRI in a resting state? Front Neurol (2019) 10:420. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Buxton RB, Wong EC, Frank LR. Dynamics of blood flow and oxygenation changes during brain activation: the balloon model. Magn Reson Med (1998) 39:855–64. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910390602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Buxton RB. Dynamic models of BOLD contrast. NeuroImage (2012) 62:953–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Buxton RB, Uludağ K, Dubowitz DJ, Liu TT. Modeling the hemodynamic response to brain activation. NeuroImage (2004) 23(Suppl 1):S220–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Friston KJ, Kahan J, Biswal B, Razi A. A DCM for resting state fMRI. NeuroImage (2014) 94:396–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Stephan KE, Weiskopf N, Drysdale PM, Robinson PA, Friston KJ. Comparing hemodynamic models with DCM. NeuroImage (2007) 38:387–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Whitworth JA, Williamson PM, Mangos G, Kelly JJ. Cardiovascular consequences of cortisol excess. Vasc Health Risk Manag (2005) 1:291–9. doi: 10.2147/vhrm.2005.1.4.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Muller M, van der Graaf Y, Visseren FL, Mali WPTM, Geerlings MI. Hypertension and longitudinal changes in cerebral blood flow: the SMART-MR study. Ann Neurol (2012) 71:825–33. doi: 10.1002/ana.23554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Alosco ML, Brickman AM, Spitznagel MB, Narkhede A, Griffith EY, Raz N, et al. Higher BMI is associated with reduced brain volume in heart failure. BMC Obes (2014) 1:4. doi: 10.1186/2052-9538-1-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Curtis BJ, Williams PG, Jones CR, Anderson JS. Sleep duration and resting fMRI functional connectivity: examination of short sleepers with and without perceived daytime dysfunction. Brain Behav (2016) 6:e00576–13. doi: 10.1002/brb3.576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Herting MM, Gautam P, Chen Z, Mezher A, Vetter NC. Test-retest reliability of longitudinal task-based fMRI_ Implications for developmental studies. Dev Cognit Neurosci (2018) 33:17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Muthukumaraswamy SD, Evans CJ, Edden RAE, Wise RG, Singh KD. Individual variability in the shape and amplitude of the BOLD-HRF correlates with endogenous GABAergic inhibition. Hum Brain Mapp (2012) 33:455–65. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Falkenberg LE, Westerhausen R, Specht K, Hugdahl K. Resting-state glutamate level in the anterior cingulate predicts blood-oxygen level-dependent response to cognitive control. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA (2012) 109:5069–73. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115628109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Falkenberg LE, Westerhausen R, Craven AR, Johnsen E, Kroken RA, L Berg E-M, et al. Impact of glutamate levels on neuronal response and cognitive abilities in schizophrenia. NeuroImage Clin (2014) 4:576–84. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bennett CM, Miller MB. How reliable are the results from functional magnetic resonance imaging? Ann New York Acad Sci (2010) 1191:133–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05446.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Di X, Wolfer M, Kühn S, Zhang Z, Biswal BB. Estimations of the weather effects on brain functions using functional MRI – a cautionary tale. bioRxiv (2019) 5:443–24. doi: 10.1101/646695

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Black DF, Little JT, Johnson DR. Neuroanatomical considerations in preoperative functional brain mapping. Top Magn Reson Imaging (2019) 28:213–24. doi: 10.1097/RMR.0000000000000213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Jezzard P, Song AW. Technical foundations and pitfalls of clinical fMRI. NeuroImage (1996) 4:S63–75. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Specht K, Scheffler M, Reinartz J, Reul J. Experiences and applicability of presurgical real-time fMRI. Riv. di Neuroradiologia (2003a) 16:1092–6. doi: 10.1177/197140090301600615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Knecht S, Dräger B, Deppe M, Bobe L, Lohmann H, Floel A, et al. Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain (2000) 123:2512–8. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.12.2512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fernández G, de Greiff A, von J, Reuber M, Lun S, Klaver P, et al. Language mapping in less than 15 minutes: real-time functional MRI during routine clinical investigation. NeuroImage (2001) 14:585–94. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Rutten GJM, Ramsey NF, van Rijen PC, Alpherts WC, van Veelen CWM. FMRI-determined language lateralization in patients with unilateral or mixed language dominance according to the Wada test. NeuroImage (2002) 17:447–60. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Spreer J, Arnold S, Quiske A, Wohlfarth R, Ziyeh S, Altenüller D, et al. Determination of hemisphere dominance for language: comparison of frontal and temporal fMRI activation with intracarotid amytal testing. Neuroradiology (2002) 44:467–74. doi: 10.1007/s00234-002-0782-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Fernandez G, Specht K, Weis S, Tendolkar I, Reuber M, Fell J, et al. Intrasubject reproducibility of presurgical language lateralization and mapping using fMRI. Neurology (2003) 60:969–75. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000049934.34209.2e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Jansen A, Menke R, Sommer J, Förster AF, Bruchmann S, Hempleman J, et al. The assessment of hemispheric lateralization in functional MRI–robustness and reproducibility. NeuroImage (2006) 33:204–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bradshaw AR, Bishop DVM, Woodhead ZVJ. Methodological considerations in assessment of language lateralisation with fMRI: a systematic review. PeerJ (2017) 5:e3557. doi: 10.7717/peerj.3557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Janecek JK, Swanson SJ, Sabsevitz DS, Hammeke TA, Raghavan M, E Rozman M, et al. Language lateralization by fMRI and Wada testing in 229 patients with epilepsy: rates and predictors of discordance. Epilepsia (2013) 54:314–22. doi: 10.1111/epi.12068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Buchbinder BR. Functional magnetic resonance imaging. Handb Clin Neurol (2016) 135:61–92. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53485-9.00004-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Duncan JS, Winston GP, Koepp MJ, Ourselin S. Brain imaging in the assessment for epilepsy surgery. Lancet Neurol (2016) 15:420–33. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00383-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Castellano A, Cirillo S, Bello L, Riva M, Falini A. Functional MRI for Surgery of Gliomas. Curr Treat Options Neurol (2017) 19:34. doi: 10.1007/s11940-017-0469-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Tyndall AJ, Reinhardt J, Tronnier V, Mariani L, Stippich C. Presurgical motor, somatosensory and language fMRI: Technical feasibility and limitations in 491 patients over 13 years. Eur Radiol (2017) 27:267–78. doi: 10.1007/s00330-016-4369-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Cabrera OS, Lehericy S, Masson V, Samson S, Dupont S. Adapting a memory fMRI research protocol in clinical routine: Feasibility and results. Epilepsy Behav (2018) 81:49–54. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2017.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Specht K, Willmes K, Shah NJ, Jäncke L. Assessment of reliability in functional imaging studies. J Magn Reson Imaging (2003b) 17:463–71. doi: 10.1002/jmri.10277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Holiga Š., Sambataro F, Luzy C, Greig G, Sarkar N, Renken RJ, et al. Test-retest reliability of task-based and resting-state blood oxygen level dependence and cerebral blood flow measures. PloS One (2018) 13:e0206583. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Fröhner JH, Teckentrup V, Smolka MN, Kroemer NB. Addressing the reliability fallacy in fMRI: Similar group effects may arise from unreliable individual effects. NeuroImage (2019) 195:174–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Paek EJ, Murray LL, Newman SD, Kim D-J. Test-retest reliability in an fMRI study of naming in dementia. Brain Lang (2019) 191:31–45. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Stip E, Letourneau G. Psychotic symptoms as a continuum between normality and pathology. Can J Psychiatry (2009) 54:140–51. doi: 10.1177/070674370905400302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Madre M, Canales-Rodríguez EJ, Ortiz-Gil J, Murru A, Torrent C, Bramon E, et al. Neuropsychological and neuroimaging underpinnings of schizoaffective disorder: a systematic review. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2016) 134:16–33. doi: 10.1111/acps.12564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Koenigs M, Baskin-Sommers A, Zeier J, Newman JP. Investigating the neural correlates of psychopathy: a critical review. Mol Psychiatry (2011) 16:792–9. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Biswal BB, Mennes M, Zuo X-N, Gohel S, Kelly C, Smith SM, et al. Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci (2010) 107:4734–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911855107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Biswal BB. Resting state fMRI: a personal history. NeuroImage (2012) 62:938–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Fornito A, Bullmore ET. What can spontaneous fluctuations of the blood oxygenation-level-dependent signal tell us about psychiatric disorders? Curr Opin In Psychiatry (2010) 23:239–49. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328337d78d

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Beckmann CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond. B Biol Sci (2005) 360:1001–13. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, et al. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA (2006) 103:13848–53. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Raichle ME. The brain’s dark energy. Science (2006) 314:1249–50. doi: 10.1126/science.1134405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Allen EA, Erhardt EB, Damaraju E, Gruner W, Segall JM, Silva RF, et al. A baseline for the multivariate comparison of resting-state networks. Front Syst Neurosci (2011) 5:2. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Snyder AZ, Raichle ME. A brief history of the resting state: the Washington University perspective. NeuroImage (2012) 62:902–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Allen EA, Damaraju E, Plis SM, Erhardt EB, Eichele T, Calhoun VD. Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cereb Cortex (2014) 24:663–76. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hugdahl K, Raichle ME, Mitra A, Specht K. On the existence of a generalized non-specific task-dependent network. Front Hum Neurosci (2015) 9:1–15. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Kiviniemi V, Vire T, Remes J, Elseoud AA, Starck T, Tervonen O, et al. A sliding time-window ICA reveals spatial variability of the default mode network in time. Brain Connect (2011) 1:339–47. doi: 10.1089/brain.20110036.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Raichle ME, Snyder AZ. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. NeuroImage (2007) 37:1083–90– discussion 1097–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Patriat R, Molloy EK, Meier TB, Kirk GR, Nair VA, Meyerand ME, et al. The effect of resting condition on resting-state fMRI reliability and consistency: A comparison between resting with eyes open, closed, and fixated. NeuroImage (2013) 78:463–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Chang C, Metzger CD, Glover GH, Duyn JH, Heinze H-J, Walter M. Association between heart rate variability and fluctuations in resting-state functional connectivity. NeuroImage (2013) 68:93–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Huotari N, Raitamaa L, Helakari H, Kananen J, Raatikainen V, Rasila A, et al. Sampling rate effects on resting state fMRI metrics. Front Neurosci (2019) 13:279. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Birn RM, Molloy EK, Patriat R, Parker T, Meier TB, Kirk GR, et al. The effect of scan length on the reliability of resting-state fMRI connectivity estimates. NeuroImage (2013) 83:550–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Braun U, Plichta MM, Esslinger C, Sauer C, Haddad L, Grimm O, et al. Test-retest reliability of resting-state connectivity network characteristics using fMRI and graph theoretical measures. NeuroImage (2012) 59:1404–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Yan C-G, Craddock RC, Zuo X-N, Zang Y-F, Milham MP. Standardizing the intrinsic brain: towards robust measurement of inter-individual variation in 1000 functional connectomes. NeuroImage (2013) 80:246–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Zhang C, Baum SA, Adduru VR, Biswal BB, Michael AM. Test-retest reliability of dynamic functional connectivity in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage (2018) 183:907–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Hjelmervik H, Hausmann M, Osnes B, Westerhausen R, Specht K. Resting States are resting traits - an fMRI study of sex differences and menstrual cycle effects in resting state cognitive control networks. PloS One (2014) 9:e103492. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Waites AB, Stanislavsky A, Abbott DF, Jackson GD. Effect of prior cognitive state on resting state networks measured with functional connectivity. Hum Brain Mapp (2004) 24:59–68. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Harrison BJ, Pujol J, Ortiz H, Fornito A, Pantelis C, Yücel M. Modulation of brain resting-state networks by sad mood induction. PloS One (2008) 3:e1794. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Blautzik J, Vetter C, Peres I, Gutyrchik E, Keeser D, Berman A, et al. Classifying fMRI-derived resting-state connectivity patterns according to their daily rhythmicity. NeuroImage (2013) 71:298–306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Choe AS, Jones CK, Joel SE, Muschelli J, Belegu V, Caffo BS, et al. Reproducibility and temporal structure in weekly resting-state fMRI over a period of 3.5 years. PloS One (2015) 10:e0140134. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Hodkinson DJ, Daly OOA, Zunszain PA, Pariante CM, Lazurenko V, Zelaya FO, et al. Circadian and homeostatic modulation of functional connectivity and regional cerebral blood flow in humans under normal entrained conditions. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2014) 34:1493–9. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2014.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Al-Zubaidi A, Heldmann M, Mertins A, Jauch-Chara K, Münte TF. Influences of hunger, satiety and oral glucose on functional brain connectivity: a multimethod resting-state fMRI study. Neuroscience (2018) 382:80–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.04.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Al-Zubaidi A, Heldmann M, Mertins A, Brabant G, Nolde JM, Jauch-Chara K, et al. Impact of hunger, satiety, and oral glucose on the association between insulin and resting-state human brain activity. Front Hum Neurosci (2019a) 13:162. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Al-Zubaidi A, Mertins A, Heldmann M, Jauch-Chara K, Münte TF. Machine learning based classification of resting-state fMRI features exemplified by metabolic state (Hunger/Satiety). Front Hum Neurosci (2019b) 13:164. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Plichta MM, Schwarz AJ, Grimm O, Morgen K, Mier D, Haddad L, et al. Test-retest reliability of evoked BOLD signals from a cognitive-emotive fMRI test battery. NeuroImage (2012) 60:1746–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Müller VI, Cieslik EC, Laird AR, Fox PT, Radua J, Mataix-Cols D, et al. Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2018) 84:151–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Jennings RG, Van Horn JD. Publication bias in neuroimaging research: implications for meta-analyses. Neuroinform (2011) 10:67–80. doi: 10.1007/s12021-011-9125-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. LeLorier J, Gregoire G, Benhaddad A, Lapierre J, Derderian F. Discrepancies between meta-analyses and subsequent large randomized, controlled trials. N Engl J Med (1997) 337:536–42. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199708213370806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Acar F, Seurinck R, Eickhoff SB, Moerkerke B. Assessing robustness against potential publication bias in activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analyses for fMRI. PloS One (2018) 13:e0208177–23. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Kleist K. Bericht über die Gehirnpathologie in ihrer Bedeutung für Neurologie und Psychiatrie. Z f d g Neur u Psych (1937) 158:159–93. doi: 10.1007/BF02870728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Neumärker KJ, Bartsch AJ, Karl Kleist. A pioneer of neuropsychiatry. Hist Psychiatry (2003) 14:411–58. doi: 10.1177/0957154X03144001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Price CJ. A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. NeuroImage (2012) 62:816–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Specht K, Zahn R, Willmes K, Weis S, Holtel C, Krause BJ, et al. Joint independent component analysis of structural and functional images reveals complex patterns of functional reorganisation in stroke aphasia. NeuroImage (2009) 47:2057–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Abel S, Weiller C, Huber W, Willmes K, Specht K. Therapy-induced brain reorganization patterns in aphasia. Brain (2015) 138:1097–112. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Robson H, Specht K, Beaumont H, Parkes LM, Sage K, Lambon Ralph MA, et al. Arterial spin labelling shows functional depression of non-lesion tissue in chronic Wernicke’s aphasia. Cortex (2017) 92:249–60. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Plis SM, Hjelm DR, Salakhutdinov R, Allen EA, Bockholt HJ, Long JD, et al. Deep learning for neuroimaging: a validation study. Front Neurosci (2014) 8:229. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Glaser JI, Benjamin AS, Farhoodi R, Kording KP. The roles of supervised machine learning in systems neuroscience. Prog Neurobiol (2019) 175:126–37. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2019.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Rolnick D, Dyer EL. Generative models and abstractions for large-scale neuroanatomy datasets. Curr Opin In Neurobiol (2019) 55:112–20. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2019.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Rutledge RB, Chekroud AM, Huys QJ. Machine learning and big data in psychiatry: toward clinical applications. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2019) 55:152–9. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2019.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Davatzikos C. Machine learning in neuroimaging: Progress and challenges. NeuroImage (2019) 197:652–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Wen D, Wei Z, Zhou Y, Li G, Zhang X, Han W. Deep learning methods to process fMRI data and their application in the diagnosis of cognitive impairment: a brief overview and our opinion. Front Neuroinform (2018) 12:9788–5. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2018.00023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Hutson M. Artificial intelligence faces reproducibility crisis. Science (2018) 359:725–6. doi: 10.1126/science.359.6377.725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Turner BO, Paul EJ, Miller MB, Barbey AK. Small sample sizes reduce the replicability of task-based fMRI studies. Commun Biol (2018b) 1:62. doi: 10.1038/s42003-018-0073-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. de Bertoldi F, Finos L, Maieron M, Weis L, Campanella M, Ius T, et al. Improving the reliability of single-subject fMRI by weighting intra-run variability. NeuroImage (2015) 114:287–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Kandilarova S, Stoyanov D, Kostianev S, Specht K. Altered resting state effective connectivity of anterior insula in depression. Front Psychiatry (2018) 9:8–7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Kandilarova S, Stoyanov D, Sirakov N, Maes M, Specht K. Reduced grey matter volume in frontal and temporal areas in depression: contributions from voxel-based morphometry study. Acta Neuropsychiatr (2019) 59:1–6. doi: 10.1017/neu.2019.20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Heinsfeld AS, Franco AR, Craddock RC, Buchweitz A, Meneguzzi F. Identification of autism spectrum disorder using deep learning and the ABIDE dataset. NeuroImage Clin (2017) 17:16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Kazeminejad A, Sotero RC. Topological properties of resting-state fMRI functional networks improve machine learning-based autism classification. Front Neurosci (2019) 12:736–10. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.01018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Carmichael O, Schwarz AJ, Chatham CH, Scott D, Turner JA, Upadhyay J, et al. The role of fMRI in drug development. Drug Discovery Today (2018) 23:333–48. doi: 10.1016/j.drudis.2017.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: fMRI—functional magnetic resonance imaging, BOLD (blood oxygenation level dependent) signal, reliability, clinical fMRI, psychiatry

Citation: Specht K (2020) Current Challenges in Translational and Clinical fMRI and Future Directions. Front. Psychiatry 10:924. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00924

Received: 17 September 2019; Accepted: 20 November 2019;
Published: 08 January 2020.

Edited by:

Drozdstoy Stoyanov, Plovdiv Medical University, Bulgaria

Reviewed by:

Stefka Georgieva Mantarova-Valkova, Military Medical Academy, Bulgaria
Iliyana Pacheva, Plovdiv Medical University, Bulgaria

Copyright © 2020 Specht. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Karsten Specht, Karsten.specht@uib.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.