Skip to main content
Log in

The use of heuristics in intuitive mathematical judgment

  • Brief Reports
  • Published:
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Anecdotal evidence points to the use of beauty as an indication of truth in mathematical problem solving. In the two experiments of the present study, we examined the use of heuristics and tested the assumption that participants use symmetry as a cue for correctness in an arithmetic verification task. We manipulated the symmetry of sets of dot pattern addition equations. Speeded decisions about the correctness of these equations led to higher endorsements for both correct and incorrect equations when the addend and sum dot patterns were symmetrical. Therefore, this effect is not due to the fact that symmetry facilitates calculation or estimation. We found systematic evidence for the use of heuristics in solving mathematical tasks, and we discuss how these findings relate to a processing-fluency account of intuition in mathematical judgment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

References

  • Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2006). Predicting short-term stock fluctuations by using processing fluency. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 9369–9372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Zeev, T. (1996). When erroneous mathematical thinking is just as “correct”: The oxymoron of rational errors. In R. J. Sternberg & T. Ben-Zeev (Eds.), The nature of mathematical thinking (pp. 55–79). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolte, A., & Goschke, T. (2005). On the speed of intuition: Intuitive judgments of semantic coherence under different response deadlines. Memory & Cognition, 33, 1248–1255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, K. S., Regehr, G., Balthazard, C., & Parker, K. (1990). Intuition in the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 72–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. I. D. (Ed.) (2005). Handbook of mathematical cognition. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. I. D., & Austin, S. (2003). Effects of response time deadlines on adults’ strategy choices for simple addition. Memory & Cognition, 30, 988–994.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandrasekhar, S. (1987). Truth and beauty: Aesthetics and motivations in science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, K. C. (1998). The universe in a teacup: The mathematics of truth and beauty. New York: Harcourt, Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draine, S. C., & Greenwald, A. G. (1998). Replicable unconscious semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 286–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Yeo, R. A. (1994). Facial attractiveness, developmental stability, and fluctuating asymmetry. Ethology & Sociobiology, 15, 73–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.) (2002). Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273, 1699–1702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hadamard, J. (1954). The psychology of invention in the mathematical Field. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalick, S. M., Zebrowitz, L. A., Langlois, J. H., & Johnson, R. M. (1998). Does human facial attractiveness honestly advertise health? Longitudinal data on an evolutionary question. Psychological Science, 9, 8–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, A. Y., & Labroo, A. A. (2004). The effect of conceptual and perceptual fluency on brand evaluation. Journal of Marketing Research, 41, 151–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McColm, G. (2007). A metaphor for mathematics education. Notices of the American Mathematical Society, 54, 499–502.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGlone, M. S., & Tofighbakhsh, J. (2000). Birds of a feather flock conjointly (?): Rhyme as reason in aphorisms. Psychological Science, 11, 424–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, S. E. (1991). Goodness, Gestalt, groups, and Garner: Local symmetry subgroups as a theory of figural goodness. In G. R. Lockhead & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), The perception of structure (pp. 23–39). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parks, C. M., & Toth, J. P. (2006). Fluency, familiarity, aging, and the illusion of truth. Aging, Neuropsychology, & Cognition, 13, 225–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353–363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R. (2002). Reasons for the preference for symmetry. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 25, 415–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. Consciousness & Cognition, 8, 338–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality & Social Psychology Review, 8, 364–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychological Science, 9, 45–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G., Proffitt, F., Grady, J. M., & Sumich, A. (1998). Facial symmetry and the perception of beauty. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 659–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Royer, F. (1981). Detection of symmetry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 7, 1186–1210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silver, E. A. (1986). Using conceptual and procedural knowledge: A focus on relationships. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge (pp. 181–198). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, I. (2007). Why beauty is truth: The history of symmetry. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (in press). The analysis of intuition: Processing fluency and affect in judgments of semantic coherence. Cognition & Emotion.

  • Unkelbach, C. (2007). Reversing the truth effect: Learning the interpretation of processing fluency in judgments of truth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 33, 219–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanLehn, K. (1986). Arithmetic procedures are induced from examples. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge (pp. 133–179). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A. (1993). Illusions of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 1235–1253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Halberstadt, J., Fazendeiro, T., & Catty, S. (2006). Prototypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychological Science, 17, 799–806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rolf Reber.

Additional information

This research was funded by the Norwegian Research Council, Grant 166252, to R.R.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Reber, R., Brun, M. & Mitterndorfer, K. The use of heuristics in intuitive mathematical judgment. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15, 1174–1178 (2008). https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.6.1174

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.6.1174

Keywords

Navigation