Skip to main content
Log in

Subtle linguistic cues influence perceived blame and financial liability

  • Brief Reports
  • Published:
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When bad things happen, how do we decide who is to blame and how much they should be punished? In the present studies, we examined whether subtly different linguistic descriptions of accidents influence how much people blame and punish those involved. In three studies, participants judged how much people involved in particular accidents should be blamed and how much they should have to pay for the resulting damage. The language used to describe the accidents differed subtly across conditions: Either agentive (transitive) or nonagentive (intransitive) verb forms were used. Agentive descriptions led participants to attribute more blame and request higher financial penalties than did nonagentive descriptions. Further, linguistic framing influenced judgments, even when participants reasoned about a well-known event, such as the “wardrobe malfunction” of Super Bowl 2004. Importantly, this effect of language held, even when people were able to see a video of the event. These results demonstrate that even when people have rich established knowledge and visual information about events, linguistic framing can shape event construal, with important real-world consequences. Subtle differences in linguistic descriptions can change how people construe what happened, attribute blame, and dole out punishment. Supplemental results and analyses may be downloaded from http://pbr.psychonomic-journals .org/content/supplemental.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

References

  • Brown, R., & Fish, D. (1983). The psychological causality implicit in language. Cognition, 14, 237–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fausey, C. M., & Boroditsky, L. (in press). Whodunnit? Cross-linguistic differences in eyewitness memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

  • Fausey, C. M., Long, B. L., Inamori, A., & Boroditsky, L. (in press). Constructing agency: The role of language. Frontiers in Cultural Psychology.

  • Filipović, L. (2007). Language as a witness: Insights from cognitive linguistics. International Journal of Speech, Language & the Law, 14, 245–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garvey, C., Caramazza, A., & Yates, J. (1975). Factors influencing assignments of pronoun antecedents. Cognition, 3, 227–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jansson v. Bowen, No. 34-2008-00017351 (Sacramento Super. Ct. Aug. 7, 2008).

  • Kasof, L., & Lee, J. Y. (1993). Implicit causality as implicit salience.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 65, 877–891. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.877

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., & Lowe, C. A. (1979). On the use of single sentence descriptions of behavior in attribution research. Social Behavior & Personality, 7, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, L., Frederick, S., & Ariely, D. (2006). Try it, you’ll like it: The influence of expectation, consumption, and revelation on preferences for beer. Psychological Science, 17, 1054–1058. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01829.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I. P. (1987). Associative effects of information framing. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 25, 85–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I. P., & Gaeth, G. J. (1988). How consumers are affected by the framing of attribute information before and after consuming the product. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 374–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 4, 19–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 13, 585–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maass, A., Karasawa, M., Politi, F., & Suga, S. (2006). Do verbs and adjectives play different roles in different cultures? A cross-linguistic analysis of person representation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 90, 734–750. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.734

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Old Bailey Proceedings Online (2009). Retrieved November 3, 2009, from www.oldbaileyonline.org.

  • Pickering, M. J., & Majid, A. (2007). What are implicit causality and consequentiality? Language & Cognitive Processes, 22, 780–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pryor, J. B., & Kriss, M. (1977). The cognitive dynamics of salience in the attribution process. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 35, 49–55. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.1.49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid, J., & Fiedler, K. (1998). The backbone of closing speeches: The impact of prosecution versus defense language on judicial attributions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1140–1172. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01672.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semin, G. R., Rubini, M., & Fiedler, K. (1995). The answer is in the question: The effect of verb causality upon locus of explanation. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 834–842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sher, S., & McKenzie, C. R. M. (2006). Information leakage from logically equivalent frames. Cognition, 101, 467–494. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., Carmon, Z., & Ariely, D. (2005). Placebo effects of marketing actions: Consumers may get what they pay for. Journal of Marketing Research, 42, 383–393. doi:10.1509/jmkr.2005.42.4.383

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Timberlake, J. (February 1, 2004). “Statement From Justin Timberlake,” PR Newswire.

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, P. A. (2003). Effects of wording and stimulus format on the use of contingency information in causal judgment. Memory & Cognition, 31, 231–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Caitlin M. Fausey.

Additional information

The present research was supported by an NSF Graduate Research Fellowship to C.M.F. and NSF Grant No. 0608514 to L.B.

Electronic supplementary material

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fausey, C.M., Boroditsky, L. Subtle linguistic cues influence perceived blame and financial liability. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 17, 644–650 (2010). https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.5.644

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.5.644

Keywords

Navigation