Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Learning & Behavior 1/2013

01.03.2013

The effects of response cost and species-typical behaviors on a daily time–place learning task

verfasst von: Scott H. Deibel, Christina M. Thorpe

Erschienen in: Learning & Behavior | Ausgabe 1/2013

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Two theories that have been hypothesized to mediate acquisition in daily time–place learning (TPL) tasks were investigated in a free operant daily TPL task: the response cost hypothesis and the species-typical behavior hypothesis. One lever at the end of one of the choice arms of a T-maze provided food in the morning, and 6 h later, a lever in the other choice arm provided food. Four groups were used to assess the effect of two possible sources of response cost: physical effort of the task and costs associated with foraging ecology. One group was used to assess the effect of explicitly allowing for species-typical behaviors. If only first arm choice data were considered, there was little evidence of learning. However, both first press and percentage of presses on the correct lever prior to the first reinforcement revealed evidence of TPL in most rats tested. Unexpectedly, the high response cost groups for both of the proposed sources did not perform better than the low response cost groups. The groups that allowed animals to display species-typical behaviors performed the worst. Skip session probe trials confirmed that the majority of the rats that acquired the task were using a circadian timing strategy. The results from the present study suggest that learning in free operant daily TPL tasks might not be dependent on response cost.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
It was very rare for a rat not to have been run in both the morning and afternoon. This occurred an average of less than one session per rat, and there were no differences in the number of rats per group that were missing sessions.
 
2
Carr andWilkie (1997b, 1999) used shorter time-out periods, less than 1 min, in their daily TPL tasks. However, in interval TPL tasks, they used time-outs that were 2 min in length (e.g., Thorpe, Petrovic, &Wilkie, 2002). It is possible that the 2-min time-out used in the present study was too long and that, if a shorter time-out period had been used, the time-out groups’ performance might have been more similar to that of the groups without a time-out.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Aragona, B. J., Curtis, J. T., Davidson, A. J., Wang, Z., & Stephan, F. K. (2002). Behavioral and neurochemical investigation of circadian time–place learning in the rat. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 17, 330–344.PubMedCrossRef Aragona, B. J., Curtis, J. T., Davidson, A. J., Wang, Z., & Stephan, F. K. (2002). Behavioral and neurochemical investigation of circadian time–place learning in the rat. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 17, 330–344.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Biebach, H., Gordijn, M., & Krebs, J. R. (1989). Time–place learning by garden warblers, Sylvia borin. Animal Behavior, 37, 353–360.CrossRef Biebach, H., Gordijn, M., & Krebs, J. R. (1989). Time–place learning by garden warblers, Sylvia borin. Animal Behavior, 37, 353–360.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Boulos, Z., & Logothetis, D. E. (1990). Rats anticipate and discriminate between two daily feeding times. Physiology and Behavior, 48, 523–529.PubMedCrossRef Boulos, Z., & Logothetis, D. E. (1990). Rats anticipate and discriminate between two daily feeding times. Physiology and Behavior, 48, 523–529.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carr, J. A. R., Tan, A. O., Thorpe, C. M., & Wilkie, D. M. (2001). Further evidence of joint time–place control of rats’ behaviour: Further evidence from an “open hopper” test. Behavioural Processes, 53, 147–153.PubMedCrossRef Carr, J. A. R., Tan, A. O., Thorpe, C. M., & Wilkie, D. M. (2001). Further evidence of joint time–place control of rats’ behaviour: Further evidence from an “open hopper” test. Behavioural Processes, 53, 147–153.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carr, J. A. R., & Wilkie, D. M. (1997a). Ordinal, phase, and interval timing. In C. M. Bradshaw & E. Szabadi (Eds.), Time and behavior: Psychological and neurobiological analyses (pp. 265–327). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Carr, J. A. R., & Wilkie, D. M. (1997a). Ordinal, phase, and interval timing. In C. M. Bradshaw & E. Szabadi (Eds.), Time and behavior: Psychological and neurobiological analyses (pp. 265–327). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Zurück zum Zitat Carr, J. A. R., & Wilkie, D. M. (1997b). Rats use an ordinal timer in a daily time–place learning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 23, 232–247.PubMedCrossRef Carr, J. A. R., & Wilkie, D. M. (1997b). Rats use an ordinal timer in a daily time–place learning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 23, 232–247.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carr, J. A. R., & Wilkie, D. M. (1999). Rats are reluctant to use circadian timing in a daily time–place task. Behavioural Processes, 44, 287–299.CrossRef Carr, J. A. R., & Wilkie, D. M. (1999). Rats are reluctant to use circadian timing in a daily time–place task. Behavioural Processes, 44, 287–299.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Daan, S., & Koene, P. (1981). On the timing of foraging flights by oystercatchers, Haematopusostralegus, on tidal mudflats. Netherlands Journal of Sea Research, 15, 1–22.CrossRef Daan, S., & Koene, P. (1981). On the timing of foraging flights by oystercatchers, Haematopusostralegus, on tidal mudflats. Netherlands Journal of Sea Research, 15, 1–22.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Lukoyanov, N. V., Pereira, P. A., Mesquita, R. M., & Andrade, J. P. (2002). Restricted feeding facilitates time–place learning in adult rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 134, 283–290.PubMedCrossRef Lukoyanov, N. V., Pereira, P. A., Mesquita, R. M., & Andrade, J. P. (2002). Restricted feeding facilitates time–place learning in adult rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 134, 283–290.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Means, L. W., Arolfo, M. P., Ginn, S. R., Pence, J. D., & Watson, N. P. (2000). Rats more readily acquire a time-of-day go no-go discrimination than a time-of-day choice discrimination. Behavioural Processes, 52, 11–20.PubMedCrossRef Means, L. W., Arolfo, M. P., Ginn, S. R., Pence, J. D., & Watson, N. P. (2000). Rats more readily acquire a time-of-day go no-go discrimination than a time-of-day choice discrimination. Behavioural Processes, 52, 11–20.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Means, L. W., Ginn, S. R., Arolfo, M. P., & Pence, J. D. (2000). Breakfast in the nook and dinner in the dining room: Time-of-day discrimination in rats. Behavioural Processes, 49, 21–33.PubMedCrossRef Means, L. W., Ginn, S. R., Arolfo, M. P., & Pence, J. D. (2000). Breakfast in the nook and dinner in the dining room: Time-of-day discrimination in rats. Behavioural Processes, 49, 21–33.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mistlberger, R. E., de Groot, M. H. M., Bossert, J. M., & Marchant, E. G. (1996). Discrimination of circadian phase in intact and suprachiasmatic nuclei-ablated rats. Brain Research, 739, 12–18.PubMedCrossRef Mistlberger, R. E., de Groot, M. H. M., Bossert, J. M., & Marchant, E. G. (1996). Discrimination of circadian phase in intact and suprachiasmatic nuclei-ablated rats. Brain Research, 739, 12–18.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pizzo, M. J., & Crystal, J. D. (2002). Representation of time in time–place learning. Animal Learning and Behavior, 30, 387–393.PubMedCrossRef Pizzo, M. J., & Crystal, J. D. (2002). Representation of time in time–place learning. Animal Learning and Behavior, 30, 387–393.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pizzo, M. J., & Crystal, J. D. (2006). The influence of temporal spacing on time–place discrimination. Learning & Behavior, 34, 131–143.CrossRef Pizzo, M. J., & Crystal, J. D. (2006). The influence of temporal spacing on time–place discrimination. Learning & Behavior, 34, 131–143.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Reebs, S. G. (1993). A test of time–place learning in a cichlid fish. Behavioural Processes, 30, 273–282.CrossRef Reebs, S. G. (1993). A test of time–place learning in a cichlid fish. Behavioural Processes, 30, 273–282.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Reebs, S. G. (1996). Time–place learning in golden shiners (Pisces: cyprinidae). Behavioural Processes, 36, 253–262.CrossRef Reebs, S. G. (1996). Time–place learning in golden shiners (Pisces: cyprinidae). Behavioural Processes, 36, 253–262.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Reebs, S. G. (1999). Time–place learning based on food but not on predation risk in a fish, the inanga (Galaxias maculates). Ethology, 105, 361–371.CrossRef Reebs, S. G. (1999). Time–place learning based on food but not on predation risk in a fish, the inanga (Galaxias maculates). Ethology, 105, 361–371.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Saksida, L. M., & Wilkie, D. M. (1994). Time-of-day discrimination by pigeons, Columba livia. Animal Learning and Behavior, 22, 143–154.CrossRef Saksida, L. M., & Wilkie, D. M. (1994). Time-of-day discrimination by pigeons, Columba livia. Animal Learning and Behavior, 22, 143–154.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schatz, B., Beugnon, G., & Lachaud, J. P. (1994). Time–place learning by an invertebrate, the ant Ectatommaruidum. Animal Behavior, 48, 236–238.CrossRef Schatz, B., Beugnon, G., & Lachaud, J. P. (1994). Time–place learning by an invertebrate, the ant Ectatommaruidum. Animal Behavior, 48, 236–238.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Skinner, D. M., Etchegary, C. M., Ekert-Maret, E. C., Baker, C. J., Harley, C. W., Evans, J. H., & Martin, G. M. (2003). An analysis of response, direction and place learning in an open field and T maze. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 29, 3–13.PubMedCrossRef Skinner, D. M., Etchegary, C. M., Ekert-Maret, E. C., Baker, C. J., Harley, C. W., Evans, J. H., & Martin, G. M. (2003). An analysis of response, direction and place learning in an open field and T maze. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 29, 3–13.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Thorpe, C. M., Bates, M. E., & Wilkie, D. M. (2003). Rats have trouble associating all three parts of the time–place–event memory code. Behavioural Processes, 63, 95–110.PubMedCrossRef Thorpe, C. M., Bates, M. E., & Wilkie, D. M. (2003). Rats have trouble associating all three parts of the time–place–event memory code. Behavioural Processes, 63, 95–110.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Thorpe, C. M., Floresco, S. B., Carr, J. A. R., & Wilkie, D. M. (2002). Alterations in time–place learning induced by lesions to the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Behavioural Processes, 59, 87–100.PubMedCrossRef Thorpe, C. M., Floresco, S. B., Carr, J. A. R., & Wilkie, D. M. (2002). Alterations in time–place learning induced by lesions to the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Behavioural Processes, 59, 87–100.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Thorpe, C. M., Jacova, C., & Wilkie, D. M. (2004). Some pitfalls in measuring memory in animals. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 711–718.PubMedCrossRef Thorpe, C. M., Jacova, C., & Wilkie, D. M. (2004). Some pitfalls in measuring memory in animals. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 711–718.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Thorpe, C. M., Petrovic, V., & Wilkie, D. M. (2002). How rats process spatiotemporal information in the face of distraction. Behavioural Processes, 58, 79–90.PubMedCrossRef Thorpe, C. M., Petrovic, V., & Wilkie, D. M. (2002). How rats process spatiotemporal information in the face of distraction. Behavioural Processes, 58, 79–90.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Thorpe, C. M., & Wilkie, D. M. (2006). Properties of time–place learning. In T. R. Zentall & E. A. Wasserman (Eds.), Comparative cognition: Experimental explorations of animal intelligence (pp. 229–245). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Thorpe, C. M., & Wilkie, D. M. (2006). Properties of time–place learning. In T. R. Zentall & E. A. Wasserman (Eds.), Comparative cognition: Experimental explorations of animal intelligence (pp. 229–245). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Zee, E. A., Havekes, R., Barf, R. P., Hut, R. A., Nijholt, I. M., Jacobs, E. H., & Gerkema, M. P. (2008). Circadian time–place learning in mice depends on Cry genes. Current Biology, 18, 844–848.PubMedCrossRef Van der Zee, E. A., Havekes, R., Barf, R. P., Hut, R. A., Nijholt, I. M., Jacobs, E. H., & Gerkema, M. P. (2008). Circadian time–place learning in mice depends on Cry genes. Current Biology, 18, 844–848.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat White, W., & Timberlake, W. (1990). [A failure to find time–place learning in rats on a three-arm radial maze]. Unpublished raw data White, W., & Timberlake, W. (1990). [A failure to find time–place learning in rats on a three-arm radial maze]. Unpublished raw data
Zurück zum Zitat Widman, D. R., Gordon, D., & Timberlake, W. (2000). Response cost and time–place discrimination by rats in maze tasks. Animal Learning & Behavior, 28, 298–309.CrossRef Widman, D. R., Gordon, D., & Timberlake, W. (2000). Response cost and time–place discrimination by rats in maze tasks. Animal Learning & Behavior, 28, 298–309.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Widman, D. R., Sermania, C. M., & Genismore, K. E. (2004). Evidence for time–place learning in the Morris water maze without food restriction but with increased response cost. Behavioural Processes, 67, 183–193.PubMedCrossRef Widman, D. R., Sermania, C. M., & Genismore, K. E. (2004). Evidence for time–place learning in the Morris water maze without food restriction but with increased response cost. Behavioural Processes, 67, 183–193.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The effects of response cost and species-typical behaviors on a daily time–place learning task
verfasst von
Scott H. Deibel
Christina M. Thorpe
Publikationsdatum
01.03.2013
Verlag
Springer-Verlag
Erschienen in
Learning & Behavior / Ausgabe 1/2013
Print ISSN: 1543-4494
Elektronische ISSN: 1543-4508
DOI
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-012-0076-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2013

Learning & Behavior 1/2013 Zur Ausgabe