Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Happiness Studies 1/2021

Open Access 13.02.2020 | Research Paper

The Molecular Genetics of Life Satisfaction: Extending Findings from a Recent Genome-Wide Association Study and Examining the Role of the Serotonin Transporter

verfasst von: Bernd Lachmann, Anna Doebler, Cornelia Sindermann, Rayna Sariyska, Andrew Cooper, Heidrun Haas, Christian Montag

Erschienen in: Journal of Happiness Studies | Ausgabe 1/2021

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

In a recent genome-wide association study (GWAS), three polymorphisms (rs3756290, RAPGEF6; rs2075677, CSE1L; rs4958581, NMUR2) were suggested as potentially being related to subjective-well-being and life satisfaction. Additionally, associations between the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism (serotonin transporter) and subjective well-being have been reported in other previous studies. In the current study, we therefore sought to further investigate the findings of the GWAS and examine the association between 5-HTTLPR and subjective well-being. A total of 1174 participants (821 females) were recruited and asked to provide information on their demographics, life satisfaction, and positive affect. All participants provided a genetic sample. We found associations between one SNP derived from the GWAS (rs4958581, NMUR2) and life satisfaction. We also replicated findings involving 5-HTTLPR and life satisfaction, but only for the housing, leisure and family life satisfaction variables, and not for overall life satisfaction or positive affect. Our study underlines that research investigating complex traits in the field of behavioral genetics is challenging due to their (a) pleiotropic and (b) polygenic effects, resulting in tiny effect sizes of each marker investigated. The current study also highlights the importance of investigating genetic markers of distinct areas of life satisfaction.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10902-020-00231-x) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

1 Introduction

Over the last several decades, the constructs of life satisfaction and subjective well-being (SWB) (Diener 1984) have been examined using many different theoretical and methodological approaches, and in relation to a host of demographic, psychological and environmental variables. For example, previous studies have focused on demographic variables like gender (Batz-Barbarich et al. 2018) and age (e.g. Moksnes and Espnes 2013; Ulloa et al. 2013) in relation to life satisfaction. Here, the findings for age have been inconsistent: In a review of the literature, (Ulloa et al. 2013) suggested that the observed relationship between age and SWB was either U-shaped, inverted U-shaped, or linear. The results for gender have also been mixed; for example, a meta-analysis reported differences for job satisfaction, with women showing lower job satisfaction than men, but no gender differences for overall life satisfaction (Batz-Barbarich et al. 2018). Cultural influences on life satisfaction have also attracted broad interest, particularly focusing on the differences between collectivistic and individualistic cultures (e.g. Kim et al. 2018; Park et al. 2004; Schimmack et al. 2002; Tov and Diener 2013), and highlighting differences in levels of life satisfaction across nations (Diener and Suh 2000).
Many studies have focused on the role that environmental and life variables play in relation to SWB, for example, the role of specific life events (Luhmann et al. 2012b), the impact of life style (Burrell et al. 2006), the effects of marital status (Shapiro and Keyes 2008), to name just a few. This previous research has also examined SWB in relation to behaviors and experiences that are typical of modern industrial societies, including, for example, commuting (Lachmann et al. 2017) and the use of digital technologies (Samaha and Hawi 2016). Internet use disorder and smartphone use disorder have been examined in conjunction with life satisfaction: Some studies have examined the personality characteristics underpinning these two disorders (Lachmann et al. 2019; Montag et al. 2015; Peterka-Bonetta et al. 2019), while others have sought to examine more specific constructs like empathy or optimism (Lachmann et al. 2018b; Renaud et al. 2018).
Previous research has also focused on individual differences in life satisfaction and SWB; for example, the association between overall life satisfaction and personality (e.g. Fowler et al. 2018; Kim et al. 2018; Zalewska et al. 2018), as well as the association between specific variables of life satisfaction (e.g. family, leisure, income) and overall life satisfaction (e.g. Heller et al. 2004; Kahneman and Deaton 2010; Kuykendall et al. 2015). These approaches can be described as, respectively, top-down (i.e. personality is associated with overall life satisfaction) and bottom-up approaches (i.e. specific life satisfaction variables are related to overall life satisfaction). In some cases, both approaches have been combined into what might be called an integrative approach, postulating the simultaneous applicability of both top-down and bottom-up approaches (e.g. Brief et al. 1993; David et al. 1997; Feist et al. 1995; Headey et al. 1991; Lachmann et al. 2018a).
As part of the effort to better understand individual differences in life satisfaction, research has also increasingly sought to utilize newer methods and techniques to help understand the genetic influences on life satisfaction. The study of the influence of nature versus nurture on individual differences in SWB usually begins with estimating the heritability of the given phenotype (Plomin and Colledge 2001). The research approach used to answer this type of question is the twin/family study design, mostly utilizing an ACE [i.e. additive genetic variance (A), shared environmental factors (C), non-shared environmental factors (E) or ACED (i.e. the same as ACE, plus non-additive genetic variance (D)] model to discriminate between genetic and environmental influences (Polderman et al. 2015; e.g. Sauce and Matzel 2018). Bartels (2015) summarized 25 years of research on the heritability of life satisfaction and SWB in a meta-analysis, making a distinction between SWB and life satisfaction, although these two terms are often used interchangeably in the literature (Layard 2010). For SWB, they found an estimated heritability of 36% based on a total sample size of n = 55,974. The lowest estimated heritability was 23% (Weiss et al. 2008), and the highest was 59% (Tellegen et al. 1988). Similar results were found for life satisfaction, with an estimated heritability of 32% based on a sample size of n = 47,750 participants, with the lowest estimated heritability at 18% (Nes et al. 2008) and the highest at 47% (Bartels and Boomsma 2009). In sum, these findings demonstrate that genetic factors contribute to the characteristics of life satisfaction and SWB by explaining approximately 34% of the variance (Bartels 2015).
Beyond exploring the heritability of life satisfaction, several studies have investigated whether specific genetic variants are associated with life satisfaction and SWB. The focus in these studies has been largely, but not solely, on gene loci associated with neurotransmitters like dopamine, oxytocin, and serotonin (Farhud et al. 2014). These studies use what can be termed the candidate gene approach (Montag and Reuter 2014), typically where pharmacological studies have suggested a particular neurotransmitter may play a role in relation to a defined phenotype. This knowledge is then used to search for genetic variants on genes linked to this neurotransmitter. For example, Chen et al. (2013) established a link between monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) and happiness in women, but not men. MAOA deaminates, among other things, serotonin and dopamine, but the efficiency of the polymorphism is dependent upon a variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR), resulting in genotypes with low activity of MAOA (MAOA-L) and high-activity of MAOA (MAOA-H) (Sabol et al. 1998). Female carriers of the MAOA-L genotype showed significantly higher happiness compared to carriers of the MAOA-H genotype (Chen et al. 2013). Another study investigated the association between SWB and the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism (Liu et al. 2017). COMT Val158Met is a Single Nucleotide Polymorphism (SNP) on the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase gene involved in the inactivation of catecholamines (e.g. dopamine), in which the VAL/VAL genotype shows a 40% increase in enzyme activity compared to the MET/MET genotype (Lachman et al. 1996). Liu et al. (2017) reported greater SWB for individuals with a higher number of VAL alleles.
A review of the literature by IsHak et al. (2011) investigated the relationship between the neuropeptide oxytocin and SWB (IsHak et al. 2011). Oxytocin is involved in a wide spectrum of positive social behaviors (e.g. intimacy and bonding), facilitates social interaction, and is therefore assumed to be related to SWB (Farhud et al. 2014). IsHak et al. concluded that oxytocin would be of potential use in enhancing SWB. On the other hand, a recent genetic study by Conner et al. (2018) reported a null finding on the association between SWB and an oxytocin-related genetic variation (rs53576), suggesting that this locus might not be as widely involved in SWB as initially thought (Conner et al. 2018).
In addition to the above, the effect of the serotonergic system on SWB has also been investigated, since serotonin is associated with, among other things, emotional behavior, pain, and depression (Haase et al. 2013). The functional polymorphism focused on here is a various number tandem repeat (VNTR) variation in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), consisting of a short and a long variant. The long variant of 5-HTTLPR shows an approximately three times higher expression rate than the shorter version, resulting in the release of more serotonin transporters in the synaptic cleft (Canli and Lesch 2007). Additionally, the effect of 5-HTTLPR can be further influenced by a SNP in the promotor region (rs25531), as demonstrated in a recent study (Hooten et al. 2017). De Neve (2011) reported in a case-control study significantly higher levels of SWB for carriers of the longer variant of the serotonin transporter gene (De Neve 2011). However, the replication of these initial findings in independent samples could be only partially accomplished (De Neve et al. 2012).
To identify further promising candidate genes in the context of SWB/life satisfaction, neuroticism, and depressive symptoms (Okbay et al. 2016), a genome wide association study (GWAS) was conducted recently. These traits are commonly also referred to as the well-being spectrum (Baselmans et al. 2019), and they show a high genetic overlap which suggests the presence of a specific interaction between those traits. Okbay et al. (2016) detected three polymorphisms that were possibly associated with SWB, namely RAPGEF6 (rs3756290), CSE1L (rs2075677), and NMUR2 (rs4958581), although not much is currently known about their mode of operation. In this regard, it is important to mention that the phenotype measure they used in this study was either life satisfaction or positive affect, and in some cohorts a mixture of both, so they could utilize as large a sample as possible (Okbay et al. 2016). However, with this course of action the effects of these polymorphisms might be difficult to interpret (Røysamb and Nes 2018). This also influences further research because both life satisfaction and positive affect must then be included prospectively when investigating the associations between the polymorphisms detected in the GWAS and SWB/life satisfaction.
Given the state of the literature summarized above, the findings examining the genetics of life satisfaction/SWB are obviously mixed and difficult to interpret. It is clear that the replication and further examination of previously shown and assumed associations between specific gene loci and life satisfaction/SWB is warranted (Conner et al. 2018).
The present study seeks to add more clarity to the literature by re-examining the relationship between specific polymorphisms and SWB. We will focus on the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism since several studies have suggested it could play an important role relating to mental well-being (e.g. Canli and Lesch 2007; De Neve 2011; De Neve et al. 2012; Murakami et al. 1999; Risch et al. 2009). Further to this, we seek to extend the findings from the GWAS of Okbay et al. (2016) by conceptually replicating their findings in relation to SWB. In contrast to the classic pharmacologically based candidate gene approach, we stress that the approach to the research used in the present study might be termed the genome wide scan-based candidate gene approach. This approach might be helpful in addressing the replication crisis in psychology (and in the field of molecular genetic association studies, more specifically). Furthermore, we were interested in examining the extent to which demographic variables might influence the results. Therefore, all analyses were conducted in both the complete original sample (controlling for demographic variables) and alternatively in a balanced sample, matching all male participants from the original sample with females, considering age and education. So the use of statistical control measures can be placed at a minimum. From a methodological point of view, it might be beneficial to further analyze the potential interaction of those demographic variables (i.e. gender, age, and education) and SWB (Hutchison et al. 2004) using this alternative design. The results from the balanced sample are reported in the supplement. For all analyses, positive affect and overall life satisfaction were included, given both measures were used to represent life satisfaction in the GWAS referred to above (Okbay et al. 2016). We also followed the approach of the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) by including specific life satisfaction variables in our analyses, in addition to overall life satisfaction (Siedler et al. 2008).

2 Materials and Methods

2.1 Participants

A total of n = 1174 participants (821 females) completed an online survey and gave socio-demographic information, including age, gender, and education. The mean age in this sample was M = 23.48 (SD = 7.11), ranging from 18 to 82 years, with 93.1 percent of the participants in the age range between 18 and 29 years. Overall, 6.6% had a secondary school leaving certificate, 72.9% held a Baccalaureate-Diploma, and 20.5% had a university degree. The participants were recruited from Ulm University in Germany so most of the participants were university students. All participants are part of the Ulm Gene Brain Behavior project, from which other studies have been published (e.g. Montag et al. 2017; Sindermann et al. 2018). The local ethics committee of Ulm University approved the study, and every participant had to provide their electronic and written consent to participate.

2.2 Measures

The items used to measure life satisfaction were retrieved from a specific section of the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) which covers one’s current life situation. Individuals were asked to report on their degree of life satisfaction in the areas of health, job, income, housing, leisure time, family, and their overall satisfaction with life. Furthermore, data on mood were collected (also derived from SOEP) to use this information in the analyses as covariate. A Likert-scale was used to answer this item, ranging from 0 (“altogether bad”) to 10 (“altogether good”). The SOEP recommends administering the item concerning overall life satisfaction as the final item to avoid possible interference with the specific life satisfaction variables (Siedler et al. 2008). It should be noted that overall life satisfaction is not a simple composite of the specific life satisfaction variables (Erdogan et al. 2012); all life satisfaction variables are distinct, but they also overlap to some extent (e.g. a person more satisfied with their leisure is also more likely to have a higher score on overall satisfaction). The items of life satisfaction were answered using a Likert-scale, ranging from 0 (“completely dissatisfied”) to 10 (“completely satisfied”).
The Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) was administered to collect data on positive affect (Watson et al. 1988) taking into account the last 6 month. Ten adjectives describing either negative affect (e.g. upset, ashamed, distressed) or positive affect (e.g. strong, inspired, active) formed the two-dimensional questionnaire, with items rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = ‘very slightly or not at all’ to 5 = ‘extremely’. The items from each dimension add up to one summed score for both positive affect and negative affect. Watson et al. (1988) provided comprehensive information concerning the psychometric properties of this scale. It has been used very widely in previous research. Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample was α = .86 for positive affect (α = .87 for the balanced sample), and α = .85 for negative affect (α = .86 for the balanced sample). In the current study we only used the positive affect scale, together with the variables of life satisfaction, to stay close to the original approach of the Okbay et al. (2016) study. It should be noted that the association between negative affect and the polymorphisms of interest in this study yielded no significant results. The findings concerning negative affect and the correlations between negative affect and the variables of life satisfaction as well as positive affect can be found in the supplement.

2.3 Procedure

The participants were asked to complete the online survey. After indicating their willingness to take part in the study and the provision of a valid email address, they received a personalized link to access the online survey. Upon receipt of the link, participants could complete the survey using the digital device of their choice. Completed surveys were transmitted via an SSL encrypted link to our secure data server for further processing and analyses. After this, an appointment in our laboratory was scheduled so the participants could provide a genetic sample. Genetic samples were collected using a non-invasive method, (i.e. extraction of buccal cells using cotton swabs) to safeguard the participants from any harm.

2.4 Genotyping

DNA was automatically extracted from buccal cells using a MagNA Pure 96 instrument and commercial extraction kits (MagNA Pure 96 DNA and Viral NA Small Volume Kit; Roche Diagnostics, Mannheim, Germany) in our own laboratory (Department of Molecular Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany).
Genotypes for the Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) rs3756290 (RAPGEF6), rs2075677 (CSE1L), and rs4958581 (NMUR2) were identified using the MALDI-TOFF Mass Spectrometer technique on a MassARRAY and iPLEX system (Sequenom, San Diego, California, USA) by the Varionostic GmbH (Ulm, Germany).
Genotyping the 5-HTTLPR 44-bp variable-number tandem repeat (VNTR) polymorphism including SNP rs25531 was undertaken in the laboratory of the Department of Molecular Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany. We included SNP rs25531 in the analyses (triallelic approach) since it has been shown that carriers of the long version of 5-HTTLPR showed a similar low expression rate to carriers of the short version if a base exchange from A to G occurred in SNP rs25531 (Hu et al. 2006). To indicate the triallelic approach in the analyses a dash was attached (S′, L′). The 5-HTTLPR was amplified by polymerase chain reaction (PCR) using a peqSTAR (VWR (Avantor), Darmstadt, Germany). The protocol consisted of a 5-min denaturation phase at 95 °C. Thereafter, 35 cycles of 30 s denaturation (95 °C), 30 s annealing (64.9 °C), and 60 s extension (72 °C) were undertaken. The final elongation was performed for 5 min (72 °C). For the PCR the following primer sequences (TIB MOLBIOL, Berlin, Germany) were used:
  • Forward: 5′-TCCTCCgCTTTggCgCCTCTTCC-3′;
  • Backward: 5′-TgggggTTgCAggggAgATCCTg-3′.
The PCR-product of rs25531 SNP was genotyped using MSP1 (restriction enzyme; New England Biolabs) for digestion and incubated at 37 °C for 90 min. Thereafter, samples were loaded for 2 h at 175 V on to a 1.8% agarose gel in a TBE solution. After completion of the gel electrophoresis, the results were documented by means of a gel documentation system and classified by two independent raters.

2.5 Statistical Analysis

SPSS version 22.0 for Windows (IBM SPSS Statistics, IBM, New York, NY, USA) was used to conduct the statistical analyses. All analyses were performed for the complete original sample, as well as the balanced sample (as described above) to assess any potential influences of these demographic variables on the results using two different approaches. Normality of the distribution for all variables was inspected visually and using the procedure described by Miles and Shevlin (2001). Gender differences across life satisfaction/positive affect were examined using ANOVAs. The association of age and mood on life satisfaction and positive affect variables were examined using Pearson correlations. The associations between overall life satisfaction and the specific life satisfaction variables with the respective genetic variants were examined using ANCOVAs, with gender as a second fixed factor, and age and mood as covariates (please see the results section for further details). The level of significance (Bonferroni correction) for all analyses was set at .05/8 = .00625 (life satisfaction variables plus positive affect).

3 Results

The inspection of the data revealed no outliers and a minimal amount of missing data where some participants did not provide information for all variables of interest (please see Table 1 for the exact numbers of participants). A normal distribution for all variables in the complete sample (n = 1174) and the balanced sample (n = 524) was observed (results concerning the balanced sample can be found in the supplement).

3.1 Analyses in the Complete Sample

The life satisfaction variables and the positive affect variables were analyzed for gender specific effects. Significant differences were found for the variables health (F(1, 1172) = 9.83, p = .002) and family (F(1, 1171) = 10.32, p = .001), with females showing lower scores for health (MFemale = 7.37, SD = 2.11; MMale = 7.78, SD = 1.75) and higher scores for family (MFemale = 7.59, SD = 2.41; MMale = 7.09, SD = 2.47). No significant differences across gender were found for mood (t(1, 1170) = 1.84, p = .066), however mood was significantly and positively associated with overall life satisfaction (r = .41, p < .001), all specific life satisfaction variables [ranging between r = .18, p < .001 (income) and r = .41, p < .001 (leisure)], and positive affect (r = .31, p < .001). Age was correlated with the life satisfaction variables job (r = .10, p = .001) and income (r = .18, p < .001). Given the observed associations between mood, gender, and age with the life satisfaction variables and positive affect, further analyses were conducted using gender as a second fixed factor (besides genotype), and with age and mood as covariates. The descriptive statistics for the life satisfaction variables and positive affect (complete sample) are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for all life satisfaction (LS) variables, positive affect (PA), and mood in the complete sample (N = 1174)
Variables
N
Mean
SD
Min (scale)
Max (scale)
Skew
LS-Health
1173
7.50
2.02
0
10
− 1.16
LS-Job
1173
5.11
3.69
0
10
− .39
LS-Income
1173
4.41
3.11
0
10
.02
LS-Housing
1168
7.23
2.45
0
10
− 1.03
LS-Leisure
1173
7.16
2.06
0
10
− .95
LS-Family
1172
7.44
2.44
0
10
− 1.14
OLS
1168
7.92
1.80
0
10
− 1.42
PA
1170
34.61
5.92
10
50
− .40
Mood
1172
6.52
1.71
0
10
− .67
OLS overall life satisfaction
The genotype frequencies and Hardy–Weinberg equilibria for the SNPs under investigation were as follows: for RAPGEF6 (CC: n = 583, CT: n = 410, TT: n = 56; HWE: Χ2 = 2.18, df = 1, p = .140), NMUR2 (CC: n = 132, CT: n = 479, TT: n = 438; HWE: Χ2 = .01, df = 1, p = .953), CSE1L (AA: n = 613, AG: n = 375, GG: n = 59; HWE: Χ2 = .03, df = 1, p = .868), and 5-HTTLPR including rs25531 (triallelic approach) (L′L′: n = 253, L′S′: n = 435, S′S′: n = 193; HWE: Χ2 = .05, df = 1, p = .815). Due to different reasons (e.g. genotyping failure, MALDI TOF dropout) we have varying genotype frequencies. In the following paragraphs we will use the name of the genes which includes the respective SNP.
We should note here that our analyses of the association between genes and life satisfaction/positive affect showed few statistically significant results, but for the sake of completeness we have reported all of our findings without regard to their significance. For clarity, however, the statistically significant findings have been highlighted in Figs. 1, 2, 3 and 4.

3.2 Associations for RAPGEF6, NMUR2, CSE1L and Life Satisfaction/Positive Affect in the Complete Sample

For RAPGEF6, there was no significant association with the life satisfaction variables (overall life satisfaction (F(2, 1035) = .44, p = .642; health: F(2, 1035) = .55, p = .577; job: F(2, 1035) = .84, p = .432; income: F(2, 1035) = 1.56, p = .211; housing: F(2, 1035) = 1.68, p = .188; leisure: F(2, 1035) = .60, p = .548; family: F(2, 1035) = 3.57, p = .028;), nor with positive affect (F(2, 1035) = .45, p = .640) on genotype level (CC vs. CT vs. TT).
Testing the same variables when combining participants carrying at least one T-allele (T+: TT, TC) against participants who were homozygous for the C-allele (T−: CC) also yielded no significant results (overall life satisfaction (F(1, 1037) = .46, p = .498; health: F(1, 1037) = 1.10, p = .295; job: F(1, 1037) = .67, p = .413; income: F(1, 1037) = .20, p = .656; housing: F(1, 1037) = .03, p = .955; leisure: F(1, 1037) = .19, p = .666; family: F(1, 1037) = 4.04, p = .045; positive affect: F(1, 1037) = .54, p = .465). Doing the same analyses for participants carrying at least one C-allele (C+: CC, TC) against participants who were homozygous for the T-allele (C−: TT) provided again no significant results.
The interaction between gender and RAPGEF6 was not significant, independent of genotype grouping.
For NMUR2, the results were as follows: overall life satisfaction F(2, 1035) = 4.96, p = .007; health: F(2, 1035) = 1.13, p = .325; job: F(2, 1035) = .63, p = .533; income: F(2, 1035) = .89, p = .413; housing: F(2, 1035) = .75, p = .473; leisure: F(2, 1035) = .21, p = .808; family: F(2, 1035) = 7.68, p < .001; positive affect: F(2, 1035) = .52, p = .594 (tested on genotype level CC vs. CT vs. TT). After a Bonferroni correction (.05/8 = .00625), the variable family showed a significant association with NMUR2. Post-hoc analyses (Games-Howell) revealed a significant difference between the TT and CT genotypes (p = .003; − .53, 95% CI [− .91, − .14]) relating to the variable family, showing lower scores for the TT genotype (M = 7.24, SD = 2.61) compared to the CT genotype (M = 7.76, SD = 2.20). For details, please see Fig. 1.
On allele level (C+: CC, TC vs. C−: TT carriers), the following results were observed: overall life satisfaction: F(1, 1037) = 3.98, p = .046; health: F(1, 1037) = 2.26, p = .133; job: F(1, 1037) = .32, p = .571; income: F(1, 1037) = .63, p = .428; housing: F(1, 1037) = .73, p = .393; leisure: F(1, 1037) = .40, p = .528; family: F(1, 1037) = 11.48, p = .001; positive affect: F(1, 1037) = .01, p = .968. Again, the variables family and NMUR2 (Bonferroni corrected) were significantly associated with the C− group (M = 7.23, SD = 2.63), showing lower scores compared to the C+ group (M = 7.64, SD = 2.28). Please refer to Fig. 2 for an illustration of this. The analyses for T+ carriers versus T− carriers yielded no significant result.
The interaction between gender and NMUR2 was not significant, independent of genotype grouping.
For CSE1L, there were no significant results in relation to the life satisfaction variables and positive affect: overall life satisfaction F(2, 1034) = .55, p = .576; health: F(2, 1034) = .44, p = .641; job: F(2, 1034) = 1.02, p = .360; income: F(2, 1034) = .73, p = .482; housing: F(2, 1034) = 1.64, p = .195; leisure: F(2, 1034) = .66, p = .518; family: F(2, 1034) = .11, p = .892; positive affect: F(2, 1034) = 2.22, p = .109 (tested on genotype level AA vs. AG vs. GG).
Comparing the G+ group with the G− group also showed no significant results (after Bonferroni correction): overall life satisfaction: F(1, 1036) = 1.08, p = .298; health: F(1, 1036) = .59, p = .441; job: F(1, 1036) = .01, p = .954; income: F(1, 1036) = .40, p = .527; housing: F(1, 1036) = 3.34, p = .068; leisure: F(1, 1036) = 1.11, p = .292; family: F(1, 1036) = .03, p = .868; positive affect: F(1, 1036) = 4.28, p = .039. Also, no significant results have been found when comparing A+ carriers with A− carriers.
The interaction between gender and CSE1L was not significant, independent of genotype grouping.

3.3 Associations Between 5-HTTLPR and Life Satisfaction/Positive Affect in the Complete Sample

The findings for the association between 5-HTTLPR (including rs25531) and life satisfaction and positive affect were as follows (tested on genotype level: L′L′ vs. L′S′ vs. S′S′): overall life satisfaction: F(2, 870) = 2.65, p = .071; health: F(2, 870) = .01, p = .991; job: F(2, 870) = .37, p = .692; income: F(2, 870) = .33, p = .719; housing: F(2, 870) = 4.36, p = .013; leisure: F(2, 870) = 3.39, p = .034; family: F(2, 870) = 5.04, p = .007; positive affect: F(2, 870) = .18, p = .839.
Testing these associations for S′+ carriers (S′S′, S′L′) versus S′− carriers (L′L′) showed the following results: overall life satisfaction: F(1, 872) = .33, p = .568; health: F(1, 872) = .02, p = .903; job: F(1, 872) = .70, p = .404; income: F(1, 872) = .01, p = .917; housing: F(1, 872) = 8.71, p = .003; leisure: F(1, 872) = 5.09, p = .024; family: F(1, 872) = 3.42, p = .065; positive affect: F(1, 872) = .21, p = .645. This time the variable housing and 5-HTTLPR (Bonferroni corrected) were significantly associated, with the S′+ group (M = 7.19, SD = 2.42) showing lower scores compared to the S′− group (M = 7.63, SD = 2.28). The result is illustrated in Fig. 3.
When comparing S′S′ carriers with L′L′ carriers (extreme groups), the following results were observed: overall life satisfaction: F(1, 439) = 2.99, p = .084; health: F(1, 439) = .02, p = .892; job: F(1, 439) = .27, p = .606; income: F(1, 439) = .33, p = .568; housing: F(1, 439) = 5.60, p = .018; leisure: F(1, 439) = 6.35, p = .012; family: F(1, 439) = 8.80, p = .003; positive affect: F(1, 439) = .36, p = .551. The variable family and 5-HTTLPR (Bonferroni corrected) were associated, with the S′S′ group (M = 7.10, SD = 2.60) showing lower scores compared to the L′L′− group (M = 7.67, SD = 2.27).
The interaction between gender and 5-HTTLPR was not significant, independent of genotype grouping. The results are depicted in Fig. 4.

4 Discussion

The present study investigated the association between three genetic variants (rs3756290, RAPGEF6; rs2075677, CSE1L; rs4958581, NMUR2) and life satisfaction derived from a recent GWAS (Okbay et al. 2016). The study also re-examined the association between 5-HTTLPR and life satisfaction (De Neve 2011; De Neve et al. 2012). Further to this, for methodological reasons, all analyses were replicated in a sample balanced for gender, age, and education, following a different methodological approach since there is some evidence in the literature of interactions between those three demographic variables and genetics (e.g. Belsky et al. 2016; Jiang et al. 2017; Miettinen et al. 2018; Wu et al. 2016). The results for the balanced sample are summarized in the supplement.
Our findings in this study showed that the only significant association detected in the complete sample for the SNPs found in the GWAS (Okbay et al. 2016) was between rs4958581, NMUR2 and the life satisfaction variable family. For the same analyses in the balanced sample, no significant associations were shown at all. The results for 5-HTTLPR were mixed: In the complete sample, the life satisfaction variables housing [S′+ (S′S′, S′L′) vs. S′− (L′L′)] and family (S′S′ vs. L′L′) were associated with 5-HTTLPR, with L′L′ carriers showing higher scores on life satisfaction. In the balanced sample, we found associations between the life satisfaction variables housing and leisure with 5-HTTLPR [S′+ (S′S′, S′L′) vs. S′− (L′L′)], with L′L′ carriers showing higher scores on life satisfaction.
In line with the previous literature, in the present study carriers of the L′L′ genotype showed higher scores on life satisfaction compared to those carrying at least one S′ (S′+) allele (De Neve 2011; De Neve et al. 2012), but only for some specific life satisfaction variables, and not for overall life satisfaction or positive affect. A positive influence of the long variant of 5-HTTLPR on various phenotypical characteristics is reported in the literature (e.g. Fox et al. 2009), while also showing the potential role of the short variant of 5-HTTLPR as a risk allele, for example, regarding depression (Caspi et al. 2003) or anxiety (Lesch et al. 1996). Moreover, in several studies and meta-analyses, the moderating role of 5-HTTLPR is highlighted, for example, in terms of moderating emotional behavior and changes in marital satisfaction (Haase et al. 2013), stress and depression (Karg et al. 2011), or coping strategies and emotion regulation (Plieger et al. 2017). A recent meta-analysis, however, concluded that the 5-HTTLPR by stress interaction on depression must be of a modest effect size and only observable in limited situations (Culverhouse et al. 2018). In other cases, the following relationship has been reported, in which the short variant of 5-HTTLPR was associated with positive emotional expressions (Haase et al. 2015), or with nostalgia proneness (Luo et al. 2019). The previous research seems to suggest 5-HTTLPR plays an important role in both the positive and negative regulation of behavior; further attention on its role seems warranted at this point. Taking this into account, our findings showing that only some of the specific life satisfaction variables were associated with 5-HTTLPR are in line with the current state of research.
It cannot be ruled out that inclusion of SNP rs25531 in our analyses (triallelic approach) had some influence on potentially replicating the findings of de Neve (2011; De Neve et al. 2012), the latter of which were undertaken without consideration of SNP rs25531. It is now widely agreed, however, that it is best to use a triallelic approach, since it is known that the expression rates of 5-HTTLPR change depending on SNP rs25531, thus altering the functional effect (Hooten et al. 2017; Hu et al. 2006).
Of note, given the mixed findings shown in the previous literature, in addition to the analyses reported in the results, although not the focus of the current study, we also explored associations between overall life satisfaction and several polymorphisms with theoretical associations with life satisfaction (the data used for the analyses were derived from the Ulm Gene Brain Behavior project; please see the method section for further information). We report these (non-) findings for the reason of completeness without further discussion. No significant results for overall life satisfaction were found in relation to either OXTR (rs53576), COMT Val158Met or MAOA (VNTR). In other words, only the null findings from Conner et al. (2018) were replicated. Also, we did not find any significant results for BDNF VAL66 MET (Brain-derived neurotrophic factor; i.e. playing a role in neuronal growth and long-term memory), DRD2/ANKK1 Taq Ia and DRD4 (VNTR) (both dopamine receptors) in relation to overall life satisfaction (please see Table S4).
Analyzing the SNPs derived from the GWAS (Okbay et al. 2016) showed one significant result in the complete sample, relating to SNP rs4958581 (NMUR2). The only significant association was found between rs4958581 and the life satisfaction variable family; in this case, carriers of the TT genotype showed lower scores compared to C+ carriers. It should be noted that our analyses on genotype level showed a significant difference between the CT and TT genotypes, but not between CT and CC genotypes which could be because of the smaller sample size of the CC (n = 132) group compared to the CT and TT groups (CT: n = 479, TT: n = 438). A GWAS is a method used to detect potential correlations between a specific trait and SNP. To accomplish this goal, millions of genetic markers are tested, with typically a large number of participants included in this type of study (Røysamb and Nes 2018). The GWAS approach is not hypothesis-driven and is assumption-free, relying solely on the discovery of statistically highly significant associations between a trait and a specific gene locus. Since there is typically no theoretical basis underpinning the findings of a GWAS, the findings from these studies need to be further investigated in studies such as that reported here. The findings from a GWAS can form a useful starting point for further studies (we have termed this the genome wide scan-based candidate gene approach further above). Over the last few years, the GWAS methodology has produced many promising discoveries in the field of behavioral genetics, of relevance for the understanding of various diseases, and potentially helping the development of new treatments and therapeutic approaches (Visscher et al. 2017). Nevertheless, it can often be difficult to interpret the results of a GWAS since the detected effects of specific SNPs are very small, and because of the influence of pleiotropic and polygenic effects (Reimers et al. 2019; Røysamb and Nes 2018).
In relation to the GWAS by Okbay et al. (2016), it should be noted that the phenotype measures used were life satisfaction, positive affect, and in some cases a mixture of both measures. Life satisfaction and positive affect were used to measure SWB, but these measures potentially reflect different constructs, namely cognitive and affective SWB (e.g. Luhmann et al. 2012a). Even though both constructs overlap, they are also somewhat distinct (Weiss et al. 2016). The combination of both measures to gather data on SWB complicates the interpretation of the findings from this GWAS (Okbay et al. 2016), and could have affected our findings as well. We used separate measures for life satisfaction and positive affect.
The analyses in the current study were conducted separately across two samples; firstly, on the complete sample, and secondly, on a balanced sample to attempt to control for stratification effects (Hutchison et al. 2004). Given the findings from these analyses, it remains unclear if this strategy was successful or not. Conducting the analyses in a balanced sample in this study allowed stronger statistical control of some of the variables, but at the same time it led to a loss of power in the sample. This may have led to an inability to find specific effects in the balanced sample; this was the case for the findings from the analyses of the SNPs derived from the GWAS, and for all but one finding in relation to 5-HTTLPR. In one case, we observed a novel association in the balanced sample concerning the life satisfaction variable leisure and 5-HTTLPR, and in a further case one finding was present in both samples (that between the life satisfaction variable housing and 5-HTTLPR). In general, however, the associations found in the complete sample were not observed in the balanced sample, which could point to a statistical power problem when using the balanced sample.

4.1 Strength and Limitations

One limitation of the present study is the sample size, especially when conducting the analyses in the balanced sample. Even though the complete sample consisted of more than 1000 participants, the balanced sample was only about half that size, which led to a loss of statistical power. Furthermore, there was an imbalance in the distribution of females and males in the complete sample. The strategy to counteract this imbalance by conducting a second set of analyses in a balanced sample was only partially successful given the loss of statistical power described above. On the other hand, we should note that the findings for the life satisfaction variable housing and 5-HTTLPR were replicated in the balanced sample, despite the decrease in statistical power. This suggests that the association between 5-HTTLPR and the life satisfaction variable housing might be particularly robust.
We should also note that 93,1 percent of the participants were between 18 and 29 years old and students. This should be kept in mind when considering the degree to which the results in this study generalize to other samples in future studies.
We were able to replicate previously established findings with respect to 5-HTTLPR and SWB in our sample. Moreover, we found support for the association between rs4958581 (NMUR2) and SWB. Finally, the inclusion of various life satisfaction variables provides additional insights into the structure and functioning of life satisfaction generally (Schimmack et al. 2004) and, more specifically, may be also useful for future studies examining cross-cultural aspects of life satisfaction.

5 Conclusion

In the current study, we found significant associations between rs4958581 (NMUR2), a relevant SNP identified in a previous GWAS, and the life satisfaction variable family. We also found associations between 5-HTTLPR and the specific life satisfaction variables of housing, leisure, and family, but not for overall life satisfaction or positive affect. Our findings reflect the somewhat heterogeneous pattern of previous research findings regarding the link between 5-HTTLPR and SWB, in which for most of the studies the L′ allele was associated with higher life satisfaction, but not in all cases (e.g. De Neve et al. 2012). For the findings from the GWAS (Okbay et al. 2016), our analyses show a first independent evidence of potential associations with SWB, in addition to those noted in the GWAS. In more general terms, it is very challenging to find and validate significant effects in the field of behavioral genetics because of pleiotropic and polygenic effects, epistasis effects, and gene by environment interactions. Therefore, much more work is needed to further consolidate our understanding of the genetic basis of SWB.

Acknowledgements

Open Access funding provided by Projekt DEAL. The position of CM was funded by a Heisenberg grant awarded to him by the German Research Foundation (MO 2363/3-2). This work was also funded by a DFG grant awarded to AD (DO2035/1-1) and CM (MO 2363/10-1).

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

There are no actual or potential conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels, M. (2015). Genetics of wellbeing and its components satisfaction with life, happiness, and quality of life: A review and meta-analysis of heritability studies. Behavior Genetics, 45(2), 137–156. Bartels, M. (2015). Genetics of wellbeing and its components satisfaction with life, happiness, and quality of life: A review and meta-analysis of heritability studies. Behavior Genetics, 45(2), 137–156.
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels, M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009). Born to be happy? The etiology of subjective well-being. Behavior Genetics, 39(6), 605. Bartels, M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009). Born to be happy? The etiology of subjective well-being. Behavior Genetics, 39(6), 605.
Zurück zum Zitat Batz-Barbarich, C., Tay, L., Kuykendall, L., & Cheung, H. K. (2018). A meta-analysis of gender differences in subjective well-being: Estimating effect sizes and associations with gender inequality. Psychological Science, 29(9), 1491–1503. Batz-Barbarich, C., Tay, L., Kuykendall, L., & Cheung, H. K. (2018). A meta-analysis of gender differences in subjective well-being: Estimating effect sizes and associations with gender inequality. Psychological Science, 29(9), 1491–1503.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky, D. W., Moffitt, T. E., Corcoran, D. L., Domingue, B., Harrington, H., Hogan, S., et al. (2016). The genetics of success: How single-nucleotide polymorphisms associated with educational attainment relate to life-course development. Psychological Science, 27(7), 957–972. Belsky, D. W., Moffitt, T. E., Corcoran, D. L., Domingue, B., Harrington, H., Hogan, S., et al. (2016). The genetics of success: How single-nucleotide polymorphisms associated with educational attainment relate to life-course development. Psychological Science, 27(7), 957–972.
Zurück zum Zitat Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., George, J. M., & Link, K. E. (1993). Integrating bottom-up and top-down theories of subjective well-being: The case of health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 646–653. Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., George, J. M., & Link, K. E. (1993). Integrating bottom-up and top-down theories of subjective well-being: The case of health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 646–653.
Zurück zum Zitat Burrell, L. M., Adams, G. A., Durand, D. B., & Castro, C. A. (2006). The impact of military lifestyle demands on well-being, army, and family outcomes. Armed Forces & Society, 33(1), 43–58. Burrell, L. M., Adams, G. A., Durand, D. B., & Castro, C. A. (2006). The impact of military lifestyle demands on well-being, army, and family outcomes. Armed Forces & Society, 33(1), 43–58.
Zurück zum Zitat Canli, T., & Lesch, K. (2007). Long story short: The serotonin transporter in emotion regulation and social cognition. Nature Neuroscience, 10(9), 1103–1109. Canli, T., & Lesch, K. (2007). Long story short: The serotonin transporter in emotion regulation and social cognition. Nature Neuroscience, 10(9), 1103–1109.
Zurück zum Zitat Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301(5631), 386–389. Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301(5631), 386–389.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen, H., Pine, D. S., Ernst, M., Gorodetsky, E., Kasen, S., Gordon, K., et al. (2013). The MAOA gene predicts happiness in women. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 40, 122–125. Chen, H., Pine, D. S., Ernst, M., Gorodetsky, E., Kasen, S., Gordon, K., et al. (2013). The MAOA gene predicts happiness in women. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 40, 122–125.
Zurück zum Zitat Conner, T. S., McFarlane, K. G., Choukri, M., Riordan, B. C., Flett, J. A., Phipps-Green, A. J., et al. (2018). The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) variant rs53576 is not related to emotional traits or states in young adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2548. Conner, T. S., McFarlane, K. G., Choukri, M., Riordan, B. C., Flett, J. A., Phipps-Green, A. J., et al. (2018). The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) variant rs53576 is not related to emotional traits or states in young adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2548.
Zurück zum Zitat Culverhouse, R. C., Saccone, N. L., Horton, A. C., Ma, Y., Anstey, K. J., Banaschewski, T., et al. (2018). Collaborative meta-analysis finds no evidence of a strong interaction between stress and 5-HTTLPR genotype contributing to the development of depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(1), 133–142. Culverhouse, R. C., Saccone, N. L., Horton, A. C., Ma, Y., Anstey, K. J., Banaschewski, T., et al. (2018). Collaborative meta-analysis finds no evidence of a strong interaction between stress and 5-HTTLPR genotype contributing to the development of depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(1), 133–142.
Zurück zum Zitat David, J. P., Green, P. J., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). Differential roles of neuroticism, extraversion, and event desirability for mood in daily life: An integrative model of top-down and bottom-up influences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 149–159. David, J. P., Green, P. J., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). Differential roles of neuroticism, extraversion, and event desirability for mood in daily life: An integrative model of top-down and bottom-up influences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 149–159.
Zurück zum Zitat De Neve, J. (2011). Functional polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) in the serotonin transporter gene is associated with subjective well-being: Evidence from a US nationally representative sample. Journal of Human Genetics, 56(6), 456–459. De Neve, J. (2011). Functional polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) in the serotonin transporter gene is associated with subjective well-being: Evidence from a US nationally representative sample. Journal of Human Genetics, 56(6), 456–459.
Zurück zum Zitat Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 542–575. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 542–575.
Zurück zum Zitat Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (2000). Measuring subjective well-being to compare the quality of life of cultures. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 3–12). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (2000). Measuring subjective well-being to compare the quality of life of cultures. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 3–12). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Erdogan, B., Bauer, T. N., Truxillo, D. M., & Mansfield, L. R. (2012). Whistle while you work a review of the life satisfaction literature. Journal of Management, 38(4), 1038–1083. Erdogan, B., Bauer, T. N., Truxillo, D. M., & Mansfield, L. R. (2012). Whistle while you work a review of the life satisfaction literature. Journal of Management, 38(4), 1038–1083.
Zurück zum Zitat Farhud, D. D., Malmir, M., & Khanahmadi, M. (2014). Happiness & health: The biological factors-systematic review article. Iranian Journal of Public Health, 43(11), 1468–1477. Farhud, D. D., Malmir, M., & Khanahmadi, M. (2014). Happiness & health: The biological factors-systematic review article. Iranian Journal of Public Health, 43(11), 1468–1477.
Zurück zum Zitat Feist, G. J., Bodner, T. E., Jacobs, J. F., Miles, M., & Tan, V. (1995). Integrating top-down and bottom-up structural models of subjective well-being: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(1), 138–150. Feist, G. J., Bodner, T. E., Jacobs, J. F., Miles, M., & Tan, V. (1995). Integrating top-down and bottom-up structural models of subjective well-being: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(1), 138–150.
Zurück zum Zitat Fowler, S. A., Davis, L. L., Both, L. E., & Best, L. A. (2018). Personality and perfectionism as predictors of life satisfaction: The unique contribution of having high standards for others. Facets, 3(1), 227–241. Fowler, S. A., Davis, L. L., Both, L. E., & Best, L. A. (2018). Personality and perfectionism as predictors of life satisfaction: The unique contribution of having high standards for others. Facets, 3(1), 227–241.
Zurück zum Zitat Fox, E., Ridgewell, A., & Ashwin, C. (2009). Looking on the bright side: Biased attention and the human serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1663), 1747–1751. Fox, E., Ridgewell, A., & Ashwin, C. (2009). Looking on the bright side: Biased attention and the human serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1663), 1747–1751.
Zurück zum Zitat Haase, C. M., Beermann, U., Saslow, L. R., Shiota, M. N., Saturn, S. R., Lwi, S. J., et al. (2015). Short alleles, bigger smiles? The effect of 5-HTTLPR on positive emotional expressions. Emotion, 15(4), 438–448. Haase, C. M., Beermann, U., Saslow, L. R., Shiota, M. N., Saturn, S. R., Lwi, S. J., et al. (2015). Short alleles, bigger smiles? The effect of 5-HTTLPR on positive emotional expressions. Emotion, 15(4), 438–448.
Zurück zum Zitat Haase, C. M., Saslow, L. R., Bloch, L., Saturn, S. R., Casey, J. J., Seider, B. H., et al. (2013). The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene moderates the association between emotional behavior and changes in marital satisfaction over time. Emotion, 13(6), 1068–1079. Haase, C. M., Saslow, L. R., Bloch, L., Saturn, S. R., Casey, J. J., Seider, B. H., et al. (2013). The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene moderates the association between emotional behavior and changes in marital satisfaction over time. Emotion, 13(6), 1068–1079.
Zurück zum Zitat Headey, B., Veenhoven, R., & Wearing, A. (1991). Top-down versus bottom-up theories of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24(1), 81–100. Headey, B., Veenhoven, R., & Wearing, A. (1991). Top-down versus bottom-up theories of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24(1), 81–100.
Zurück zum Zitat Heller, D., Watson, D., & Ilies, R. (2004). The role of person versus situation in life satisfaction: A critical examination. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4), 574–600. Heller, D., Watson, D., & Ilies, R. (2004). The role of person versus situation in life satisfaction: A critical examination. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4), 574–600.
Zurück zum Zitat Hooten, W. M., Townsend, C. O., & Sletten, C. D. (2017). The triallelic serotonin transporter gene polymorphism is associated with depressive symptoms in adults with chronic pain. Journal of Pain Research, 10, 1071–1078. Hooten, W. M., Townsend, C. O., & Sletten, C. D. (2017). The triallelic serotonin transporter gene polymorphism is associated with depressive symptoms in adults with chronic pain. Journal of Pain Research, 10, 1071–1078.
Zurück zum Zitat Hu, X., Lipsky, R. H., Zhu, G., Akhtar, L. A., Taubman, J., Greenberg, B. D., et al. (2006). Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 78(5), 815–826. Hu, X., Lipsky, R. H., Zhu, G., Akhtar, L. A., Taubman, J., Greenberg, B. D., et al. (2006). Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 78(5), 815–826.
Zurück zum Zitat Hutchison, K. E., Stallings, M., McGeary, J., & Bryan, A. (2004). Population stratification in the candidate gene study: Fatal threat or red herring? Psychological Bulletin, 130(1), 66–79. Hutchison, K. E., Stallings, M., McGeary, J., & Bryan, A. (2004). Population stratification in the candidate gene study: Fatal threat or red herring? Psychological Bulletin, 130(1), 66–79.
Zurück zum Zitat IsHak, W. W., Kahloon, M., & Fakhry, H. (2011). Oxytocin role in enhancing well-being: A literature review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 130(1–2), 1–9. IsHak, W. W., Kahloon, M., & Fakhry, H. (2011). Oxytocin role in enhancing well-being: A literature review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 130(1–2), 1–9.
Zurück zum Zitat Jiang, R., Babyak, M. A., Brummett, B. H., Siegler, I. C., Kuhn, C. M., & Williams, R. B. (2017). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Val66Met polymorphism interacts with gender to influence cortisol responses to mental stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 79, 13–19. Jiang, R., Babyak, M. A., Brummett, B. H., Siegler, I. C., Kuhn, C. M., & Williams, R. B. (2017). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Val66Met polymorphism interacts with gender to influence cortisol responses to mental stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 79, 13–19.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(38), 16489–16493. Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(38), 16489–16493.
Zurück zum Zitat Karg, K., Burmeister, M., Shedden, K., & Sen, S. (2011). The serotonin transporter promoter variant (5-HTTLPR), stress, and depression meta-analysis revisited: Evidence of genetic moderation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(5), 444–454. Karg, K., Burmeister, M., Shedden, K., & Sen, S. (2011). The serotonin transporter promoter variant (5-HTTLPR), stress, and depression meta-analysis revisited: Evidence of genetic moderation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(5), 444–454.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim, H., Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Tsutsui, Y. (2018). Extraversion and life satisfaction: A cross-cultural examination of student and nationally representative samples. Journal of Personality, 86(4), 604–618. Kim, H., Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Tsutsui, Y. (2018). Extraversion and life satisfaction: A cross-cultural examination of student and nationally representative samples. Journal of Personality, 86(4), 604–618.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuykendall, L., Tay, L., & Ng, V. (2015). Leisure engagement and subjective well-being: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 141(2), 364–403. Kuykendall, L., Tay, L., & Ng, V. (2015). Leisure engagement and subjective well-being: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 141(2), 364–403.
Zurück zum Zitat Lachman, H. M., Papolos, D. F., Saito, T., Yu, Y. M., Szumlanski, C. L., & Weinshilboum, R. M. (1996). Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: Description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics, 6(3), 243–250. Lachman, H. M., Papolos, D. F., Saito, T., Yu, Y. M., Szumlanski, C. L., & Weinshilboum, R. M. (1996). Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: Description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics, 6(3), 243–250.
Zurück zum Zitat Lachmann, B., Sariyska, R., Kannen, C., Błaszkiewicz, K., Trendafilov, B., Andone, I., et al. (2018a). Contributing to overall life satisfaction: Personality traits versus life satisfaction variables Revisited—Is replication impossible? Behavioral sciences, 8(1), 1. Lachmann, B., Sariyska, R., Kannen, C., Błaszkiewicz, K., Trendafilov, B., Andone, I., et al. (2018a). Contributing to overall life satisfaction: Personality traits versus life satisfaction variables Revisited—Is replication impossible? Behavioral sciences, 8(1), 1.
Zurück zum Zitat Lachmann, B., Sariyska, R., Kannen, C., Stavrou, M., & Montag, C. (2017). Commuting, life-satisfaction and internet addiction. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(10), 1176. Lachmann, B., Sariyska, R., Kannen, C., Stavrou, M., & Montag, C. (2017). Commuting, life-satisfaction and internet addiction. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(10), 1176.
Zurück zum Zitat Lachmann, B., Sindermann, C., Sariyska, R. Y., Luo, R., Melchers, M. C., Becker, B., et al. (2018b). The role of empathy and life satisfaction in internet and smartphone use disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 398. Lachmann, B., Sindermann, C., Sariyska, R. Y., Luo, R., Melchers, M. C., Becker, B., et al. (2018b). The role of empathy and life satisfaction in internet and smartphone use disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 398.
Zurück zum Zitat Layard, R. (2010). Measuring subjective well-being. Science, 327(5965), 534–535. Layard, R. (2010). Measuring subjective well-being. Science, 327(5965), 534–535.
Zurück zum Zitat Lesch, K. P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S. Z., Greenberg, B. D., Petri, S., et al. (1996). Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science, 274(5292), 1527–1531. Lesch, K. P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S. Z., Greenberg, B. D., Petri, S., et al. (1996). Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science, 274(5292), 1527–1531.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu, J., Gong, P., Gao, X., & Zhou, X. (2017). The association between well-being and the COMT gene: Dispositional gratitude and forgiveness as mediators. Journal of Affective Disorders, 214, 115–121. Liu, J., Gong, P., Gao, X., & Zhou, X. (2017). The association between well-being and the COMT gene: Dispositional gratitude and forgiveness as mediators. Journal of Affective Disorders, 214, 115–121.
Zurück zum Zitat Luhmann, M., Hawkley, L. C., Eid, M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2012a). Time frames and the distinction between affective and cognitive well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(4), 431–441. Luhmann, M., Hawkley, L. C., Eid, M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2012a). Time frames and the distinction between affective and cognitive well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(4), 431–441.
Zurück zum Zitat Luhmann, M., Hofmann, W., Eid, M., & Lucas, R. E. (2012b). Subjective well-being and adaptation to life events: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 592–615. Luhmann, M., Hofmann, W., Eid, M., & Lucas, R. E. (2012b). Subjective well-being and adaptation to life events: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 592–615.
Zurück zum Zitat Luo, Y. L., Welker, K. M., Way, B., DeWall, N., Bushman, B. J., Wildschut, T., et al. (2019). 5-HTTLPR polymorphism is associated with nostalgia proneness: The role of neuroticism. Social Neuroscience, 14(2), 183–190. Luo, Y. L., Welker, K. M., Way, B., DeWall, N., Bushman, B. J., Wildschut, T., et al. (2019). 5-HTTLPR polymorphism is associated with nostalgia proneness: The role of neuroticism. Social Neuroscience, 14(2), 183–190.
Zurück zum Zitat Miettinen, M. E., Smart, M. C., Kinnunen, L., Keinänen-Kiukaanniemi, S., Moilanen, L., Puolijoki, H., et al. (2018). The effect of age and gender on the genetic regulation of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D-the FIN-D2D population-based study. The Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, 178, 229–233. Miettinen, M. E., Smart, M. C., Kinnunen, L., Keinänen-Kiukaanniemi, S., Moilanen, L., Puolijoki, H., et al. (2018). The effect of age and gender on the genetic regulation of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D-the FIN-D2D population-based study. The Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, 178, 229–233.
Zurück zum Zitat Miles, J., & Shevlin, M. (2001). Applying regression and correlation: A guide for students and researchers. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Miles, J., & Shevlin, M. (2001). Applying regression and correlation: A guide for students and researchers. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Moksnes, U. K., & Espnes, G. A. (2013). Self-esteem and life satisfaction in adolescents—Gender and age as potential moderators. Quality of Life Research, 22(10), 2921–2928. Moksnes, U. K., & Espnes, G. A. (2013). Self-esteem and life satisfaction in adolescents—Gender and age as potential moderators. Quality of Life Research, 22(10), 2921–2928.
Zurück zum Zitat Montag, C., Błaszkiewicz, K., Lachmann, B., Sariyska, R., Andone, I., Trendafilov, B., et al. (2015). Recorded behavior as a valuable resource for diagnostics in mobile phone addiction: Evidence from psychoinformatics. Behavioral sciences, 5(4), 434–442. Montag, C., Błaszkiewicz, K., Lachmann, B., Sariyska, R., Andone, I., Trendafilov, B., et al. (2015). Recorded behavior as a valuable resource for diagnostics in mobile phone addiction: Evidence from psychoinformatics. Behavioral sciences, 5(4), 434–442.
Zurück zum Zitat Montag, C., & Reuter, M. (2014). Disentangling the molecular genetic basis of personality: From monoamines to neuropeptides. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 43, 228–239. Montag, C., & Reuter, M. (2014). Disentangling the molecular genetic basis of personality: From monoamines to neuropeptides. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 43, 228–239.
Zurück zum Zitat Montag, C., Sindermann, C., Melchers, M., Jung, S., Luo, R., Becker, B., et al. (2017). A functional polymorphism of the OXTR gene is associated with autistic traits in caucasian and asian populations. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 174(8), 808–816. Montag, C., Sindermann, C., Melchers, M., Jung, S., Luo, R., Becker, B., et al. (2017). A functional polymorphism of the OXTR gene is associated with autistic traits in caucasian and asian populations. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 174(8), 808–816.
Zurück zum Zitat Murakami, F., Shimomura, T., Kotani, K., Ikawa, S., Nanba, E., & Adachi, K. (1999). Anxiety traits associated with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region in the Japanese. Journal of Human Genetics, 44(1), 15–17. Murakami, F., Shimomura, T., Kotani, K., Ikawa, S., Nanba, E., & Adachi, K. (1999). Anxiety traits associated with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region in the Japanese. Journal of Human Genetics, 44(1), 15–17.
Zurück zum Zitat Nes, R., Czajkowski, N., Roysamb, E., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., & Tambs, K. (2008). Well-being and ill-being: Shared environments, shared genes? The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(4), 253–265. Nes, R., Czajkowski, N., Roysamb, E., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., & Tambs, K. (2008). Well-being and ill-being: Shared environments, shared genes? The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(4), 253–265.
Zurück zum Zitat Okbay, A., Baselmans, B. M., De Neve, J., Turley, P., Nivard, M. G., Fontana, M. A., et al. (2016). Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nature Genetics, 48(6), 624–633. Okbay, A., Baselmans, B. M., De Neve, J., Turley, P., Nivard, M. G., Fontana, M. A., et al. (2016). Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nature Genetics, 48(6), 624–633.
Zurück zum Zitat Park, N., Huebner, E. S., Laughlin, J. E., Valois, R. F., & Gilman, R. (2004). A cross-cultural comparison of the dimensions of child and adolescent life satisfaction reports. In A. Dannerbeck, F. Casas, M. Sadurni, & G. Coenders (Eds.), Quality-of-life research on children and adolescents (pp. 61–79). Dordrecht: Springer. Park, N., Huebner, E. S., Laughlin, J. E., Valois, R. F., & Gilman, R. (2004). A cross-cultural comparison of the dimensions of child and adolescent life satisfaction reports. In A. Dannerbeck, F. Casas, M. Sadurni, & G. Coenders (Eds.), Quality-of-life research on children and adolescents (pp. 61–79). Dordrecht: Springer.
Zurück zum Zitat Peterka-Bonetta, J., Sindermann, C., & Montag, C. (2019). Personality associations with smartphone and internet use disorder: A comparison study including links to impulsivity and social anxiety. Frontiers in Public Health, 7, 127. Peterka-Bonetta, J., Sindermann, C., & Montag, C. (2019). Personality associations with smartphone and internet use disorder: A comparison study including links to impulsivity and social anxiety. Frontiers in Public Health, 7, 127.
Zurück zum Zitat Plieger, T., Melchers, M., Vetterlein, A., Görtz, J., Kuhn, S., Ruppel, M., et al. (2017). The serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and coping strategies influence successful emotion regulation in an acute stress situation: Physiological evidence. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 114, 31–37. Plieger, T., Melchers, M., Vetterlein, A., Görtz, J., Kuhn, S., Ruppel, M., et al. (2017). The serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and coping strategies influence successful emotion regulation in an acute stress situation: Physiological evidence. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 114, 31–37.
Zurück zum Zitat Plomin, R., & Colledge, E. (2001). Genetics and psychology: Beyond heritability. European Psychologist, 6(4), 229–240. Plomin, R., & Colledge, E. (2001). Genetics and psychology: Beyond heritability. European Psychologist, 6(4), 229–240.
Zurück zum Zitat Polderman, T. J., Benyamin, B., De Leeuw, C. A., Sullivan, P. F., Van Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M., et al. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics, 47(7), 702–709. Polderman, T. J., Benyamin, B., De Leeuw, C. A., Sullivan, P. F., Van Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M., et al. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics, 47(7), 702–709.
Zurück zum Zitat Reimers, M. A., Craver, C., Dozmorov, M., Bacanu, S., & Kendler, K. S. (2019). The coherence problem: Finding meaning in GWAS complexity. Behavior Genetics, 49(2), 187–195. Reimers, M. A., Craver, C., Dozmorov, M., Bacanu, S., & Kendler, K. S. (2019). The coherence problem: Finding meaning in GWAS complexity. Behavior Genetics, 49(2), 187–195.
Zurück zum Zitat Renaud, J., Wrosch, C., & Scheier, M. F. (2018). Optimism. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), The SAGE encyclopedia of lifespan human development (pp. 1550–1552). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. ISBN 9781506307657. Renaud, J., Wrosch, C., & Scheier, M. F. (2018). Optimism. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), The SAGE encyclopedia of lifespan human development (pp. 1550–1552). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. ISBN 9781506307657.
Zurück zum Zitat Risch, N., Herrell, R., Lehner, T., Liang, K., Eaves, L., Hoh, J., et al. (2009). Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta-analysis. JAMA, 301(23), 2462–2471. Risch, N., Herrell, R., Lehner, T., Liang, K., Eaves, L., Hoh, J., et al. (2009). Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta-analysis. JAMA, 301(23), 2462–2471.
Zurück zum Zitat Røysamb, E., & Nes, R. (2018). The genetics of wellbeing. Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT: DEF Publishers. Røysamb, E., & Nes, R. (2018). The genetics of wellbeing. Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT: DEF Publishers.
Zurück zum Zitat Sabol, S. Z., Hu, S., & Hamer, D. (1998). A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Human Genetics, 103(3), 273–279. Sabol, S. Z., Hu, S., & Hamer, D. (1998). A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Human Genetics, 103(3), 273–279.
Zurück zum Zitat Samaha, M., & Hawi, N. S. (2016). Relationships among smartphone addiction, stress, academic performance, and satisfaction with life. Computers in Human Behavior, 57, 321–325. Samaha, M., & Hawi, N. S. (2016). Relationships among smartphone addiction, stress, academic performance, and satisfaction with life. Computers in Human Behavior, 57, 321–325.
Zurück zum Zitat Sauce, B., & Matzel, L. D. (2018). The paradox of intelligence: Heritability and malleability coexist in hidden gene-environment interplay. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 26–47. Sauce, B., & Matzel, L. D. (2018). The paradox of intelligence: Heritability and malleability coexist in hidden gene-environment interplay. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 26–47.
Zurück zum Zitat Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (2004). Personality and life satisfaction: A facet-level analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(8), 1062–1075. Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (2004). Personality and life satisfaction: A facet-level analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(8), 1062–1075.
Zurück zum Zitat Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(4), 582–593. Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(4), 582–593.
Zurück zum Zitat Shapiro, A., & Keyes, C. L. M. (2008). Marital status and social well-being: Are the married always better off? Social Indicators Research, 88(2), 329–346. Shapiro, A., & Keyes, C. L. M. (2008). Marital status and social well-being: Are the married always better off? Social Indicators Research, 88(2), 329–346.
Zurück zum Zitat Sindermann, C., Luo, R., Zhang, Y., Kendrick, K. M., Becker, B., & Montag, C. (2018). The COMT Val158Met polymorphism and reaction to a transgression: Findings of genetic associations in both chinese and german samples. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 148. Sindermann, C., Luo, R., Zhang, Y., Kendrick, K. M., Becker, B., & Montag, C. (2018). The COMT Val158Met polymorphism and reaction to a transgression: Findings of genetic associations in both chinese and german samples. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 148.
Zurück zum Zitat Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1031–1039. Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1031–1039.
Zurück zum Zitat Tov, W., & Diener, E. (2013). Culture and subjective well-being. Available at SSRN 2199219. Tov, W., & Diener, E. (2013). Culture and subjective well-being. Available at SSRN 2199219.
Zurück zum Zitat Ulloa, B. F. L., Møller, V., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2013). How does subjective well-being evolve with age? A literature review. Journal of Population Ageing, 6(3), 227–246. Ulloa, B. F. L., Møller, V., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2013). How does subjective well-being evolve with age? A literature review. Journal of Population Ageing, 6(3), 227–246.
Zurück zum Zitat Visscher, P. M., Wray, N. R., Zhang, Q., Sklar, P., McCarthy, M. I., Brown, M. A., et al. (2017). 10 years of GWAS discovery: Biology, function, and translation. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 101(1), 5–22. Visscher, P. M., Wray, N. R., Zhang, Q., Sklar, P., McCarthy, M. I., Brown, M. A., et al. (2017). 10 years of GWAS discovery: Biology, function, and translation. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 101(1), 5–22.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.
Zurück zum Zitat Weiss, A., Baselmans, B. M., Hofer, E., Yang, J., Okbay, A., Lind, P. A., et al. (2016). Personality polygenes, positive affect, and life satisfaction. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 19(5), 407–417. Weiss, A., Baselmans, B. M., Hofer, E., Yang, J., Okbay, A., Lind, P. A., et al. (2016). Personality polygenes, positive affect, and life satisfaction. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 19(5), 407–417.
Zurück zum Zitat Weiss, A., Bates, T. C., & Luciano, M. (2008). Happiness is a personal (ity) thing: The genetics of personality and well-being in a representative sample. Psychological Science, 19(3), 205–210. Weiss, A., Bates, T. C., & Luciano, M. (2008). Happiness is a personal (ity) thing: The genetics of personality and well-being in a representative sample. Psychological Science, 19(3), 205–210.
Zurück zum Zitat Wu, K., Chen, C., Moyzis, R. K., Greenberger, E., & Yu, Z. (2016). Gender interacts with opioid receptor polymorphism A118G and serotonin receptor polymorphism − 1438 A/G on speed-dating success. Human Nature, 27(3), 244–260. Wu, K., Chen, C., Moyzis, R. K., Greenberger, E., & Yu, Z. (2016). Gender interacts with opioid receptor polymorphism A118G and serotonin receptor polymorphism − 1438 A/G on speed-dating success. Human Nature, 27(3), 244–260.
Zurück zum Zitat Zalewska, A. M., Nezlek, J., & Zieba, M. (2018). Integrated approach to personality and well-being. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 49(2), 128–130. Zalewska, A. M., Nezlek, J., & Zieba, M. (2018). Integrated approach to personality and well-being. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 49(2), 128–130.
Metadaten
Titel
The Molecular Genetics of Life Satisfaction: Extending Findings from a Recent Genome-Wide Association Study and Examining the Role of the Serotonin Transporter
verfasst von
Bernd Lachmann
Anna Doebler
Cornelia Sindermann
Rayna Sariyska
Andrew Cooper
Heidrun Haas
Christian Montag
Publikationsdatum
13.02.2020
Verlag
Springer Netherlands
Erschienen in
Journal of Happiness Studies / Ausgabe 1/2021
Print ISSN: 1389-4978
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-7780
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00231-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

Journal of Happiness Studies 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe