skip to main content
article

Discrimination and identification of finger joint-angle position using active motion

Published:01 July 2007Publication History
Skip Abstract Section

Abstract

The authors report six experiments on the human ability to discriminate and identify finger joint-angle positions using active motion. The PIP (proximal interphalangeal) joint of the index finger was examined in Exps. 1--3 and the MCP (metacarpophalangeal) joint in Exps. 4--6. In Exp. 1, the just noticeable difference (JND) of PIP joint-angle position was measured when the MCP joint was either fully extended or halfway bent. In Exp. 2, the JND of PIP joint-angle position as a function of PIP joint-angle reference position was measured when the PIP joint was almost fully extended, halfway bent, or almost fully flexed. In Exp. 3, the information transfer of PIP joint-angle position was estimated with the MCP joint in a fully extended position. In Exps. 4--6, the JND and the information transfer of MCP joint-angle position were studied with a similar experimental design. The results show that the JNDs of the PIP joint-angle position were roughly constant (2.5°−2.7°) independent of the PIP joint-angle reference position or the MCP joint-angle position used (Exps. 1 and 2). The JNDs of the MCP joint-angle position, however, increased with the flexion of both the PIP and MCP joints and ranged from 1.7° to 2.7° (Exps. 4 and 5). The information transfer of the PIP and MCP joint-angle position were similar, indicating 3--4 perfectly identifiable joint-angle positions for both joints (Exps. 3 and 6). The results provide the basic data needed for estimating, for example, the resolution of fingertip position during active free motion. They are compared to the results from previous studies on joint position, length, and thickness perception.

References

  1. Biggs, J., Horch, K., and Clark, F. J. 1999. Extrinsic muscles of the hand signal fingertip location more precisely than they signal the angles of individual finger joints. Experimental Brain Research 125, 221--230.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  2. Braida, L. D. and Durlach, N. I. 1970. Intensity perception II. Resolution in one-interval paradigms. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 51, 2, 483--502.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  3. Choi, S. and Tan, H. Z. 2005. Perceived instability of virtual haptic texture. II. Effect of collision detection algorithm. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 14, 4, 463--481. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  4. Clark, F. J. 1992. How accurately can we perceive the position of our limbs? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15, 4, 725--726.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  5. Clark, F. J. and Horch, K. W. 1986. Kinesthesia. In Handbook of Perception and Human Performance: Sensory Processes and Perception, K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, and J. P. Thomas, Eds., Vol. 1. Wiley, New York. 13/11--13/62.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  6. Clark, F. J., Horch, K. W., Bach, S. M., and Larson, G. F. 1979. Contribution of cutaneous and joint receptors to static knee-position sense in man. J. Neurophysiol. 42, 877--888.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  7. Clark, F. J., Burgess, R. C., and Chapin, J. W. 1986. Proprioception with the proximal interphalangeal joint of the index finger: Evidence for a movement sense without a static-position sense. Brain 109, 1195--1208.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  8. Clark, F. J., Burgess, R. C., Chapin, J. W., and Lipscomb, W. T. 1985. Role of intramuscular receptors in the awareness of limb position. Journal of Neurophysiology 54, 6, 1529--1540.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  9. Clark, F. J., Larwood, K. J., Davis, M. E., and Deffenbacher, K. A. 1995. A metric for assessing acuity in positioning joints and limbs. Experimental Brain Research 107, 73--79.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  10. De Domenico, G. and McCloskey, D. I. 1987. Accuracy of voluntary movements at the thumb and elbow joints. Experimental Brain Research 65, 471--478.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  11. Durlach, N. I. and Braida, L. D. 1969. Intensity perception I. Preliminary theory of intensity resolution. J. Acoustical Society of America 46, 2, 372--383.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  12. Durlach, N. I., Delhorne, L. A., Wong, A., Ko, W. Y., Rabinowitz, W. M., and Hollerbach, J. 1989a. Manual discrimination and identification of length by the finger-span method. Perception & Psychophysics 46, 1, 29--38.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  13. Durlach, N. I., Tan, H. Z., MacMillan, N. A., Rabinowitz, W. M., and Braida, L. D. 1989b. Resolution in one dimension with random variations in background dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics 46, 3, 293--296.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  14. Erickson, R. P. 1974. Parallel “population” neural coding in feature extraction. In The neurosciences, 3rd Study Program, F. O. Schmitt and F. G. Worden, Eds.), MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  15. Ferrell, W. R. and Milne, S. E. 1989. Factors affecting the accuracy of position matching at the proximal interphalangeal joint in human subjects. Journal of Physiology 411, 575--583.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  16. Ferrell, W. R. and Smith, A. 1988. Position sense at the proximal interphalangeal joint of the human index finger. Journal of Physiology 399, 49--61.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  17. Garner, W. R. 1962. Uncertainty and Structure as Psychological Concepts. Wiley, New York.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  18. Hall, L. A. and McCloskey, D. I. 1983. Detections of movements imposed on finger, elbow and shoulder joints. Journal of Physiology 335, 519--533.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  19. Ho, C. and Srinivasan, M. A. 1997. Human Haptic Discrimination of Thickness. Touch Lab Report 6, RLE TR-608, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA (http://touchlab.mit.edu/publications/1997_009.pdf).Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  20. Houtsma, A. J. M. 1983. Extimation of mutual information from limited experimental data. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 74, 1626--1629.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  21. Horch, K. W., Clark, F. J., and Burgess, P. R. 1975. Awareness of knee joint angle under static conditions. Journal of Neurophysiology 38, 1436--1447.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  22. John, K. T., Goodwin, A. W., and Darian-Smith, I. 1989. Tactile discrimination of thickness. Experimental Brain Research 78, 62--68.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  23. Laidlaw, R. W. and Hamilton, M. A. 1937. A study of thresholds in appreciation of passive movement among normal control subjects. Bulletin of the Neurological Institute of New York 6, 268--273.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  24. Macmillan, N. A. and Creelman, C. D. 2004. Detection Theory: A User's Guide, 2nd ed. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  25. Miller, G. A. 1954. Note on the bias of information estimates. In Information Theory in Psychology, H. Quastler Ed. 95--100.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  26. Monster, A. W., Herman, R., and Altland, N. R. 1973. Effect of the peripheral and central “sensory” component in the calibration of position. In New Developments in Electromyography and Clinical Neurophysiology, J. E. Desmedt Ed., Vol. 3. Karger, Basel.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  27. Paillard, J. and Brouchon, M. 1968. Active and passive movements in the calibration of position sense. In The Neuropsychology of Spatially Oriented Behavior, S. J. Freedman Ed. Dorsey, Homewood, Il.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  28. Pang, X. D., Tan, H. Z., and Durlach, N. I. 1991. Manual discrimination of force using active finger motion. Perception & Psychophysics 49, 6, 531--540.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  29. Tan, H. Z. 1997. Identification of sphere size using the PHANToM&trademark;: Towards a set of building blocks for rendering haptic environment. In Proceedings of the 6th International Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, vol. 61. 197--203.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  30. Tan, H. Z., Srinivasan, M. A., Eberman, B., and Cheng, B. 1994. Human factors for the design of force-reflecting haptic interfaces. In Proceedings of the 3rd International Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, Vol. 55--1. American Society of Mechanical Engineers, Chicago, IL. 353--359.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  31. Tan, H. Z., Durlach, N. I., Beauregard, G. L., and Srinivasan, M. A. 1995. Manual discrimination of compliance using active pinch grasp: The roles of force and work cues. Perception and Psychophysics 57, 4, 495--510.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  32. van Beers, R. J., Sittig, A. C., and Denier van der Gon, J. J. 1998. The precision of proprioceptive position sense. Experimental Brain Research 122, 4, 367--377.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref

Index Terms

  1. Discrimination and identification of finger joint-angle position using active motion

        Recommendations

        Comments

        Login options

        Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

        Sign in

        Full Access

        PDF Format

        View or Download as a PDF file.

        PDF

        eReader

        View online with eReader.

        eReader