skip to main content
research-article

Parallel prototyping leads to better design results, more divergence, and increased self-efficacy

Published:29 December 2010Publication History
Skip Abstract Section

Abstract

Iteration can help people improve ideas. It can also give rise to fixation, continuously refining one option without considering others. Does creating and receiving feedback on multiple prototypes in parallel, as opposed to serially, affect learning, self-efficacy, and design exploration? An experiment manipulated whether independent novice designers created graphic Web advertisements in parallel or in series. Serial participants received descriptive critique directly after each prototype. Parallel participants created multiple prototypes before receiving feedback. As measured by click-through data and expert ratings, ads created in the Parallel condition significantly outperformed those from the Serial condition. Moreover, independent raters found Parallel prototypes to be more diverse. Parallel participants also reported a larger increase in task-specific self-confidence. This article outlines a theoretical foundation for why parallel prototyping produces better design results and discusses the implications for design education.

References

  1. Alexander, C., Ishikawa, S., and Silverstein, M. 1977. A Pattern Language: Towns, Buildings, Construction. Oxford University Press.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  2. Arkes, H. R. and Blumer, C. 1985. The psychology of sunk cost. Organiz. Behav. Hum. Decision Proc. 35, 124--140.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  3. Ball, L. J., Evans, JStB, Dennis, I. D., and Ormerod, T. C. 1997. Problem-solving strategies and expertise in engineering design. Think. Reason. 3, 247.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  4. Ball, L. J. and Ormerod, T. C. 1995. Structured and opportunistic processing in design: A critical discussion. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Stud. 43, 131--151. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  5. Bandura, A. 1997. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control 1st Ed. Worth Publishers.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  6. de Bono, E. 1999. Six Thinking Hats 2nd Ed. Back Bay Books.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  7. Boroditsky, L. 2007. Comparison and the development of knowledge. Cognition 102, 118--128.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  8. Buxton, B. 2007. Sketching User Experiences: Getting the Design Right and the Right Design. Morgan Kaufmann. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  9. Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., and Higgens, D. M. 2005. Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the creative achievement questionnaire. Creativity Resear. J. 17, 37--50.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  10. Colhoun, J., Gentner, D., and Loewenstein, J. 2008. Learning abstract principles through principle-case comparison. In Proceedings of Cognitive Science Society. 1659--1664.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  11. Cross, N. 2006. Designerly Ways of Knowing 1st Ed. Springer.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  12. Cross, N. 2004. Expertise in design: An overview. Design Studies 25, 427--441.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  13. Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1991. Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. Harper Perennial.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  14. Dodgson, P. G. and Wood, J. V. 1998. Self-esteem and the cognitive accessibility of strengths and weaknesses after failure. J. Personality Social Psy. 75, :178--197.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  15. Dorst, K. and Cross, N. 2001. Creativity in the design process: Co-evolution of problem-solution. Design Studies 22, 425--437.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  16. Dow, S. P., Heddleston, K., and Klemmer, S. R. 2009. The efficacy of prototyping under time constraints. In Proceeding of the 7th ACM Conference on Creativity and Cognition. 165--174. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  17. Duncker, K. 1945. On Problem-Solving. Greenwood Press Reprint.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  18. Dweck, C. 2007. Mindset: The New Psychology of Success. Ballantine Books.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  19. Dym, C. L, Agogino, A. M., Frey, D. D., and Leifer, L. J. 2005. Engineering design thinking, teaching, and learning. J. Engin. Educ. 94, 103--120.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  20. Fredrickson, B. L. 2001. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psych. 56, 218--226.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  21. Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., and Thompson, L. 2003. Learning and transfer: A general role for analogical encoding. J. Educ. Psych. 95, 408, 393.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  22. Gentner, D. and Markman, A. B. 1997. Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. Amer. Psych. 52, 45--56.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  23. Gick, M. L. and Keith J. H. 1983. Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psych. 15, 1--38.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  24. Goel, V. and Pirolli, P. 1992. The structure of design problem spaces. Cognitive Science 16, 395--429.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  25. Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., and Kuhl, P. K. 2001. The Scientist in the Crib: What Early Learning Tells Us About the Mind. Harper Paperbacks.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  26. Granville, V., Krivánek, M., and Rasson, J. P. 1994. Simulated annealing: A proof of convergence. IEEE Trans. Patt. Anal. Mach. Intell. 16, 652--656. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  27. Hall, T. S. 2008. Improving self-efficacy in problem solving: Learning from errors and feedback. PhD thesis, University of North Carolina, Greensboro.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  28. Hartmann, B., Klemmer, S. R., Bernstein, M., Abdulla, L., Burr, B., Robinson-Mosher, and Gee, J. 2006. ‘Reflective physical prototyping through integrated design, test, and analysis. In Proceedings of the 19th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology. ACM, 299--308. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  29. Herring, S. R., Chang, C.-C., Krantzler, J., and Balley, B. P. 2009. Getting inspired!: Understanding how and why examples are used in creative design practice. In Proceedings of the 27th International Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. ACM, 87--96. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  30. Janis, I. L. 1982. Groupthink: Psychological Studies of Policy Decisions and Fiascoes 2nd Ed. Wadsworth Publishing.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  31. Jansson, D. and Smith, S. 1991. Design fixation. Design Studies 12, 3--11.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  32. Kelley, T. 2002. The Art of Innovation. Broadway Business Books.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  33. Kershaw, T. C. and Ohlsson, S. 2004. Multiple causes of difficulty in insight: The case of the nine-dot problem. J. Exper. Psych.; Learn., Memory, Cognition 30, 3--13.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  34. Kolodner, J. 1993. Case-Based Reasoning. Morgan Kaufmann. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  35. Kosara, R. 2007. Visualization criticism—the missing link between information visualization and art. In Proceedings of the Conference on Information Visualization. IEEE Computer Society, 631--636. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  36. Landis, J. R. and Koch, G. G. 1977. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159--174.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  37. Laseau, P. 1988. Graphic Thinking for Architects and Designers 2nd Ed. John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  38. Lee, B., Srivastava, S., Kumar, R., Brafman, R., and Klemmer, S. R. 2000. Designing with interactive example galleries. In Proceedings of the ACM SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  39. Mele, A. R. 2005. Motivation and Agency. Oxford University Press.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  40. Newell, A. 1972. Human Problem Solving. Prentice Hall. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  41. Nielsen, J. and Faber, J. M. 1996. Improving system usability through parallel design. Computer 29, 29--35. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  42. Osborn, A. F. 1963. Applied Imagination: Principles and Procedures of Creative Problem Solving. Charles Scribner’s Sons.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  43. Piaget, J. 2001. The Psychology of Intelligence. Routledge.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  44. Rittel, H. and Webber, M. 1973. Dilemmas in a general theory of planning. Policy Sci. 4, 169, 155.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  45. Schmidt, R. A., Young, D. E., Swinnen, S., and Shapiro, D. C. 1989. Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition: Support for the guidance hypothesis. J. Exper. Psych.: Learn., Memory, Cognition 15, 352--359.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  46. Schon, D. A. 1990. Educating the Reflective Practitioner: Toward a New Design for Teaching and Learning in the Professions 1st Ed. Jossey-Bass.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  47. Schon, D. A. 1995. The Reflective Practitioner: How Professionals Think in Action. Ashgate Publishing.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  48. Schooler, L. J., and Anderson, J. R. 1990. The disruptive potential of immediate feedback. In Proceedings of the 12th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  49. Schrage, M. 1999. Serious Play: How the World's Best Companies Simulate to Innovate. Harvard Business School Press.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  50. Shrout, P. E. and Fleiss, J. L. 1979. Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psych. Bull. 86, 420--428.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  51. Simon, H. A. 1996. The Sciences of the Artificial 3rd Ed. The MIT Press. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  52. Smith, S., Kohn, N., and Shah, J. 2008. What you see is what you get: Effects of provocative stimuli on creative invention. In Proceedings of the NSF International Workshop on Studying Design Creativity.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  53. Sterne, J. 2002. Web Metrics: Proven Methods for Measuring Web Site Success 1st Ed. Wiley. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  54. Thompson, L., Gentner, D., and Loewenstein, J. 2000. Avoiding missed opportunities in managerial life: Analogical training more powerful than individual case training. Organiz. Behav. Hum. Decision Process. 82, 60--75.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  55. Tohidi, M., Buxton, W., Baecker, R., and Sellen, A. 2006. Getting the right design and the design right. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. 1243--1252. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  56. Torrance, E. P. 1974. Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Personnel Press, Ginn and Co., Xerox Education Co.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar

Index Terms

  1. Parallel prototyping leads to better design results, more divergence, and increased self-efficacy

          Recommendations

          Reviews

          Alyx Macfadyen

          This paper is essentially about designing visual material for persuasive advertising. The processes of iteration and prototyping are at the heart of software and systems engineering and design. As such, they seem somewhat out of context for producing advertising material designed to convince consumers to buy a product or service. Several hypotheses underpin this study, and are perhaps reasonable even when applied to this somewhat lightweight study. Click-through analytics, for example, are the stated and accepted metric for better design results, but it remains contentious to deploy them as the sole measure for "higher quality design." The authors cite research in cognitive studies, motivation, and user agency as validating factors for their study. Nevertheless, it is important to point out that the purpose of the works cited was to improve user experience rather than to influence the purchase of a product or service. The results of this study suggest that prototyping and iteration did return positive results. This is true, however, of all mentoring, testing, and extra time taken in the design process. The sequence of methods reported is certainly interesting, and those methods that support the confidence and self-efficacy of learners are important for success in industry. Design skills for Web portals, system interfaces, and online decision support systems, as well as in professional practices, are crucial for usability. I would like to have seen more focus here on design teaching that places an emphasis on contributing to the service and supporting its users and purpose. Targeted consumers quickly learn to recognize and avoid advertisements. Thus, designers must continue to develop new methods, ploys, and tricks to ensure that they obtain the click-throughs that are the measure of design success. Online Computing Reviews Service

          Access critical reviews of Computing literature here

          Become a reviewer for Computing Reviews.

          Comments

          Login options

          Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

          Sign in

          Full Access

          • Published in

            cover image ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction
            ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction  Volume 17, Issue 4
            December 2010
            149 pages
            ISSN:1073-0516
            EISSN:1557-7325
            DOI:10.1145/1879831
            Issue’s Table of Contents

            Copyright © 2010 ACM

            Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

            Publisher

            Association for Computing Machinery

            New York, NY, United States

            Publication History

            • Published: 29 December 2010
            • Revised: 1 August 2010
            • Accepted: 1 August 2010
            • Received: 1 May 2010
            Published in tochi Volume 17, Issue 4

            Permissions

            Request permissions about this article.

            Request Permissions

            Check for updates

            Qualifiers

            • research-article
            • Research
            • Refereed

          PDF Format

          View or Download as a PDF file.

          PDF

          eReader

          View online with eReader.

          eReader