skip to main content
10.1145/1240624.1240723acmconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication PageschiConference Proceedingsconference-collections
Article

A predictive model of menu performance

Published:29 April 2007Publication History

ABSTRACT

Menus are a primary control in current interfaces, but there has been relatively little theoretical work to model their performance. We propose a model of menu performance that goes beyond previous work by incorporating components for Fitts' Law pointing time, visual search time when novice, Hick-Hyman Law decision time when expert, and for the transition from novice to expert behaviour. The model is able to predict performance for many different menu designs, including adaptive split menus, items with different frequencies and sizes, and multi-level menus. We tested the model by comparing predictions for four menu designs (traditional menus, recency and frequency based split menus, and an adaptive 'morphing' design) with empirical measures. The empirical data matched the predictions extremely well, suggesting that the model can be used to explore a wide range of menu possibilities before implementation.

References

  1. Accot, J. and Zhai, S. Beyond Fitts' Law: Models for Trajectory-Based HCI Tasks. Proc. CHI'97, 295--302. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  2. Ahlström, D. Modeling and Improving Selection in Cascading Pull-Down Menus Using Fitts' Law, the Steering Law and Force Fields. Proc. CHI'05, 61--70. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  3. Anderson, J., Matessa, M. and Lebiere, C. ACT-R: A theory of higher level cognition and its relation to visual attention. JHCI, 12 (4). 1997. 439--462. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  4. Bederson, B. Fisheye Menus. Proc. UIST'00, 217--225. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  5. Byrne, M. ACT-R/PM and menu selection: Applying a cognitive architecture to HCI. IJHCS, 55. 2001. 41--84.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  6. Byrne, M., Anderson, J., Douglass, S. and Matessa, M. Eye Tracking the Visual Search of Click-Down Menus. Proc. CHI'99, 402--409. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  7. Card, S. Visual search of computer command menus. in Bouma, H. and Bouwhuis, D. eds. Attention and Performance X, LEA, London, 1984, 97--108.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  8. Card, S., Moran, T. and Newell, A. The Psychology of Human-Computer Interaction. LEA, 1983. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  9. Ellis, S. and Hitchcock, R. Emergence of Zipf's Law: Spontaneous Encoding Optimization by Users of a Command Language. IEEE Tran. Man & Cybernetics, 16, 1986. 423--427. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  10. Findlater,L. and McGrenere,J. A comparison of static, adaptive, & adaptable menus. Proc. CHI'04, 89--96. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  11. Fitts, P. The Information Capacity of the Human Motor System in Controlling the Amplitude of Movement. J. Experimental Psych., 47. 1954. 381--391.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  12. Fitzmaurice, G., Khan, A., Pieke, R., Buxton, M. and Kurtenbach,G. Tracking menus. Proc. UIST'03, 71--79. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  13. Greenberg, S. and Witten, I. Supporting Command Reuse. IJMMS, 39. 1993. 353--390. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  14. Gutwin, C. Improving Focus Targeting in Interactive Fisheye Views. Proc. CHI'02, 267--274. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  15. Hansen, S., Kraut, R. and Farber, J. Interface Design and Multivariate Analyis of Unix commands. ACM TOIS, 2 (1). 1984. 42--57. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  16. Hick, W. On the rate of gain of information. J. Experimental Psychology, 4. 1952. 11--36.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  17. Hoffmann, E. and Lim, J. Concurrent manual-decision tasks. Ergonomics, 40 (3). 1997. 293--318.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  18. Hornof, A. and Kieras, D. Cognitive Modeling Reveals Menu Search is Both Random and Systematic. Proc. CHI'97, 107--114. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  19. Howes, A., Payne, S. and Woodward, A. The trouble with shortcuts. Proc. CHI'00, 267--268. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  20. Hyman, R. Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. J. Experimental Psych., 45. 1953. 188--196.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  21. John, B. and Kieras, D. The GOMS family of user interface analysis techniques: comparison and contrast. ACM TOCHI, 3 (4). 1996. 320--351. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  22. Kaptelinin, V. Item Recognition in Menu Selection: The Effect of Practice. Proc. InterCHI'93., 183--184. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  23. Kurtenbach, G., Sellen, A. and Buxton, W. An empirical evaluation of some articulatory & cognitive aspects of marking menus. JHCI, 8 (1). 1993. 1--23.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  24. Landauer, T. and Nachbar, D. Selection from alphabetic and numeric menu trees using a touch screen: breadth, depth and width. Proc. CHI'85, 73--78. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  25. Lane, D., Napier, H., Batsell, R. and Naman, J. Predicting the skilled use of hierarchical menus with the keystroke-level model. JHCI, 8 (2). 1993. 185--192.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  26. Lee, E. and McGregor, J. Minimizing user search time in menu retrieval systems. Human Factors, 27 (2). 1985. 157--162.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  27. MacKenzie, I. and Zhang, S. An empirical investigation of novice experience with soft keyboards. Behaviour & Info. Tech., 20. 2001. 411--418.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  28. Miller, G. The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on our Capacity for Processing Information. Psychological Review, 63. 1956. 81--97.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  29. Nilsen, E. Perceptual-motor control in human computer interaction. Report 37, The Cognitive Sci. & Machine Intelligence Lab., Univ. Michegan, 1991.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  30. Sears, A., Jacko, J., Chu, J. and Moro, F. The role of visual search in the design of effective soft keyboards. Behavior and Info. Tech., 20. 2001. 159--156.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  31. Sears, A. and Shneiderman, B. Split menus: Effectively using selection frequency to organize menus. ACM ToCHI., 1 (1). 2004. 27--51. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  32. Seow, S. Information Theoretic Models of HCI: A Comparison of the Hick-Hyman Law and Fitts' Law. JHCI, 20 (3). 2005. 315--352. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  33. Shannon, C. and Weaver, W. The mathematical theory of communications. Urbana: Univ. Illinois Press, 1949. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  34. Somberg, B. A comparison of rule-based and positionally constant arrangements of computer menu items. Proc. CHI+GI '87, 255--260. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  35. Soukoreff, R. and MacKenzie, I. Theoretical upper & lower bounds on typing speed using a stylus and soft keyboard. Behavior & Info. Tech, 14. 1995. 370--379.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  36. Zipf, G. Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort: An Introduction to Human Ecology. Addison-Wesley, Reading, Mass., 1949.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar

Index Terms

  1. A predictive model of menu performance

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Sign in
    • Published in

      cover image ACM Conferences
      CHI '07: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems
      April 2007
      1654 pages
      ISBN:9781595935939
      DOI:10.1145/1240624

      Copyright © 2007 ACM

      Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

      Publisher

      Association for Computing Machinery

      New York, NY, United States

      Publication History

      • Published: 29 April 2007

      Permissions

      Request permissions about this article.

      Request Permissions

      Check for updates

      Qualifiers

      • Article

      Acceptance Rates

      CHI '07 Paper Acceptance Rate182of840submissions,22%Overall Acceptance Rate6,199of26,314submissions,24%

    PDF Format

    View or Download as a PDF file.

    PDF

    eReader

    View online with eReader.

    eReader