Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Transportation 2/2023

Open Access 03-02-2022

Combining disparate surveys across time to study satisfaction with life: the effects of study context, sampling method, and transport attributes

Authors: Xinyi Wang, F. Atiyya Shaw, Patricia L. Mokhtarian, Giovanni Circella, Kari E. Watkins

Published in: Transportation | Issue 2/2023

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Satisfaction with life is a self-reported measure of the quality of life that has become a critical societal indicator extensively used for the evaluation and comparison of a wide range of trends and policies. This study fuses five cross-sectional travel surveys conducted from 1992 to 2018 across various geographical locations in California. Using the fused sample, we develop generalized ordered logit models to examine the effects of demographic characteristics, travel-related attributes, general and transport-related attitudinal variables, and context-control variables on individuals’ self-reported measures of life satisfaction. We find that longer commute times, mobility limitations, and a tendency to see travel as a waste of time are negatively associated with life satisfaction. To enable the use of disparate cross-sectional survey data, we incorporate context-control variables into the models. We find that life satisfaction appears to be increasing as GDP per capita increases. Among employed people, the macro-scale unemployment rate positively influences their life satisfaction. Interestingly, all else equal, we find that online opinion panel respondents have lower life satisfaction relative to respondents from other sampling methods (mainly address-based sampling), a finding that should be considered in future research using these sampling methods. Overall, this study provides a unique look at life satisfaction within a transport context, while providing an example of fusing small-scale survey datasets to study longitudinal, domain-specific, influences on variables like subjective well-being.
Notes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

In 2011, the United Nations General Assembly passed a resolution recognizing happiness and well-being as a fundamental human goal, and followed this in 2013 by establishing an official International Day of Happiness. These actions attracted much attention from the international community, and especially from those within academia, generating a surge of popular news and academic pieces on well-being and its variants. However, psychologists and social scientists have been studying happiness and subjective well-being (SWB) for decades, based on large-scale longitudinal surveys. For example, Harvard Medical School’s Study of Adult Development is the longest-running study of adult life (ongoing since 1939), and focuses on well-being during adulthood (McLaughlin et al. 2010; Waldinger et al. 2007). The World Values Survey (WVS) is another well-known longitudinal study, originating in 1981, spanning almost 100 countries, and spawning numerous contributions to the SWB literature due to its open availability (Kim 2018; Sarracino 2010). Other established sources of longitudinal well-being data include the British Social Attitudes Survey (BSA; Dean and Phillips 2015), the European Social Survey (ESS; Welsch and Kuehling 2017), the U.S. General Social Survey (GSS; Ifcher and Zarghamee 2014), and the International Social Survey Program (ISSP; Levin 2014).
These large-scale longitudinal studies have allowed researchers to model the effects of general variables such as demographic characteristics, as well as selected values and behaviors, on SWB. However, because these longitudinal surveys are broad in nature, they do not facilitate the examination of SWB within specific contexts or with the help of more diverse explanatory variables. As a result, researchers within assorted fields have taken to studying SWB using cross-sectional surveys, which are more commonly available and facilitate investigation from specific perspectives (e.g., effects of health, occupation, transportation, etc. on well-being). In this study, we combine the longitudinal and cross-sectional approaches to studying well-being, creating a fused dataset that includes common variables from five travel-behavior-oriented cross-sectional surveys conducted across a 27-year period. Each survey includes an identical SWB question, as well as numerous other common variables across the individual datasets. Since these surveys were originally designed to serve travel behavior modeling purposes, the development of this fused dataset allows a unique examination of SWB within a transport context. Combining multiple travel survey datasets also enables the analysis of contextual variables that will differ across time and space, leading to more generalizable conclusions about the impact of the transportation-related attributes and attitudes on life satisfaction.
Despite the continuity of some design factors across the five cross-sectional surveys, there are inevitable inconsistencies stemming from question wording differences and evolving survey design techniques over the years. In this study, we demonstrate an approach for addressing and ameliorating such inconsistencies using a combination of survey fusion and model development techniques. As such, one contribution of this work is to provide a rigorous example of using multi-year cross-sectional survey datasets to study the longitudinal evolution of variables, in this case, SWB, over time. Accordingly, this study both: (1) provides a detailed examination of SWB from a general as well as a transportation-oriented perspective; and (2) provides an example of combining cross-sectional survey datasets for longitudinal studies.
The remainder of this paper is organized as follows: the “Literature review” section provides an overview of the SWB literature from both conceptual and empirical perspectives. The “Overview of data” section provides a short introduction to the five transportation-oriented surveys, describes the survey fusion framework, and summarizes key statistics across the fused dataset. In the “Model estimation and analysis” section, we briefly provide background on the generalized ordered logit (GOL) model, followed by the results of the GOL models. In the “Discussion” section, we discuss model findings and limitations. We conclude with a brief overview of major findings, and provide recommendations for future research.

Literature review

Conceptual construct: from subjective well-being to life satisfaction

While the concept of happiness has long fascinated philosophers, positive subjective well-being as an academic field of study saw formal and widespread development starting in the 1970s (Diener 1984). As an individualized measurement of well-being (Mokhtarian 2019), SWB serves as a reflector of critical societal metrics such as economic development, social progress, and government policy (Diener 2000). However, the definition and measurement of SWB are more complex relative to traditional social indicators like gross domestic product (GDP). Conceptually, SWB has been defined to have two main components: hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Hedonic well-being (HWB) refers to pleasure attainment and pain avoidance, while eudaimonic well-being (EWB) is based on the idea of self-actualization (Ryan and Deci 2001). In this study, we focus on a component of SWB, life satisfaction, which represents individuals’ conscious evaluation of their lives (Pavot and Diener 1993). Traditionally, life satisfaction has been considered a component of HWB; however, Huta and Ryan (2010) showed that life satisfaction may be related to both hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives of SWB.
Life satisfaction (along with other SWB components) is typically measured using either single-item or multi-item methods, each of which has differing strengths and limitations (Diener 1984). Single-item measures refer to short, clear survey items that can be implemented independently of other items. The simplicity of single-item measures requires less effort from respondents and survey developers, which makes them more suitable for inclusion in non-SWB focused surveys. In contrast, multi-item measures, such as the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS), are composed of a group/set of survey questions, thus allowing researchers to check internal consistency and/or quality of responses, and to obtain a richer, more-nuanced, measure. Recent literature has shown that single-item measures of life satisfaction perform similarly to the multi-item measures in SWLS studies (Atroszko et al. 2017; Cheung and Lucas 2014; Jovanović, 2016); and furthermore, Diener (1984), one of the SWLS developers, recommends the use of single-item scales when a brief measure of global well-being is needed, noting that they provide adequate validity and reliability for such purposes. Accordingly, we use a single item to measure respondents’ satisfaction with life, in a series of travel-behavior oriented surveys that will be analyzed in this study.

What influences SWB1? Some empirical results

Here, we summarize findings from the literature on the effects of (1) demographic characteristics, (2) travel-related attributes, (3) personality and attitudinal variables, and (4) contextual variables (e.g., GDP per capita) on SWB, focusing particularly on these four subgroups of explanatory variables as these were the ones available in the study at hand. Furthermore, many of these variables are commonly studied explanatory factors for life satisfaction in the literature, in addition to domain-specific factors such as health, occupation, and community/friendship. Although the latter have also been found to influence SWB measures, we do not explore them further given that they are not within the scope of this study.

Demographic characteristics

Behavioral researchers across disciplines often begin their modeling efforts with the inclusion of demographic characteristics, and as evidenced by a wide body of literature, SWB is no exception. Relative to other demographic characteristics, income tends to be of primary interest in SWB studies since it is an indicator of numerous other critical variables such as education (Diener and Biswas-Diener 2002), health (Marmot 2002; Pickett and Wilkinson 2015), and housing (Hansen et al. 1998). In addition, studies have shown that income has interactions with age (Deaton 2008) and household structure (Cracolici et al. 2014; Cummins 2009). As expected, these findings converge on the understanding that income has an overall positive effect on life satisfaction, with higher-income populations having higher levels of well-being, and lower-income populations seeing the greatest potential for increased levels of well-being due to their incompletely met basic living needs (Cheung and Lucas 2014; Deaton 2008; Diener and Oishi 2000; Helliwell and Putnam 2004; Pickett and Wilkinson 2015; Stevenson and Wolfers 2008). In higher-income countries such as the U.S., which is the context of this study, the age effect on SWB has a U-shaped pattern across the life cycle when cohort effects are controlled: i.e., the lowest life satisfaction occurs among the middle-aged population (Blanchflower and Oswald 2008; Deaton 2008; Helliwell and Putnam 2004; Shields et al. 2009; Welsch and Kuehling 2017).
In contrast to the stable patterns identified thus far for income and age, the relationship between SWB and education is less consistent. For example, some studies report that education positively influences SWB, with higher-educated people having higher SWB (Helliwell and Putnam 2004; Nikolaev 2018; Witter et al. 1984; Yakovlev and Leguizamon 2012), while others report negative and/or insignificant effects of education on SWB (Kim 2018; Nikolaev 2015; Shields et al. 2009). Family structure is another key demographic variable, with studies finding that being married increases life satisfaction (Diener et al. 2000; Shields and Wooden 2003), and researchers finding that in general, interactions with family members have positive effects on SWB (Hartley-Clark 2014; Helliwell and Putnam 2004).
In recent years, SWB has attracted increasing attention in the transportation domain, with De Vos et al. (2013) conceptualizing a seminal framework of ways in which travel behavior may affect SWB, namely via (1) experiences during (destination-oriented) travel, (2) activity participation enabled by travel, (3) activities during (destination-oriented) travel, (4) travel as an activity, and (5) the potential to travel. In practice, the majority of the empirical literature has focused on examining the effects of behavior related to the commute (defined as the trip from home to work/ school, and back) on SWB.
While numerous studies (Hilbrecht et al. 2014; Martin et al. 2014; Nie and Sousa-Poza 2016; Stutzer and Frey 2008) have reported that increased commute time is negatively associated with SWB, others (Dickerson et al. 2014; Lorenz 2018) find that commute time is unassociated or even positively associated with SWB. For example, Sweet and Kanaroglou (2016) find that commuting indirectly increases SWB by enabling activity participation. Regarding travel mode, active modes such as cycling and walking are positively related to SWB in general or satisfaction with travel in particular (Martin et al. 2014; Morris and Guerra 2015; St-Louis et al. 2014). In contrast, car commuters have been found to experience increased stress due to mental strain and traffic congestion (Wener and Evans 2011). However, potentially due to differences in local transit services and roadway infrastructure, there are conflicting findings regarding whether car or transit commuters are more satisfied (Eriksson et al. 2013). Nonetheless, among all modes, drivers are least likely to obtain hedonic benefits and most likely to obtain cognitive disadvantages as a result of travel-based multitasking (Shaw et al. 2019). In the same vein, transit passengers are more likely than other mode users to experience both hedonic and productive benefits from travel-based multitasking (Shaw et al. 2019). Thus, we see that the effects of mode choice on SWB can be moderated by mode attributes such as quality of the available service and opportunities for multitasking.

Personality and attitudes

While we have thus far examined external characteristics such as demographic characteristics and travel attributes, underlying traits such as personality types and attitudes also have significant impacts on SWB (DeNeve and Cooper 1998; Diener et al. 2003). Unlike manifest/external characteristics, personalities and attitudes are latent, individual measures that can have wide-ranging impacts on a broad array of responses such as dominance, sociability, emotional stability, and trust (Ajzen 2005). Personality traits may influence SWB directly, with certain traits (e.g., extraversion, neuroticism) resulting in different experiences of positive/negative affect, or influence SWB indirectly, by guiding people’s behaviors and the resulting outcomes (Soto 2015). Attitudes are latent constructs that represent a person’s perspective on specific aspects of life (Ajzen 2005) such as education, environment, and transportation, to name a few. Life satisfaction is also an attitudinal construct, which reflects one’s perspective on/assessment of life (Heller et al. 2006).
Thus, latent factors such as personality orientations and attitudes can help researchers to understand and explain SWB using internal characteristics that can potentially represent motivations and values. Numerous researchers have included personalities and attitudes in their models to better explain SWB. For example, Helliwell and Putnam (2004) show that those who believe themselves to live among trustworthy people report higher SWB. McCarthy and Habib (2018) find that community-mindedness positively influences SWB, while people who take pride in owning a car have higher SWB. Thus, we see that personalities and attitudes can aid in better understanding different levels of SWB among individuals.

Context-control variables

Empirical research by Lucas and Donnellan (2007) has shown that one third of the variance in life satisfaction exhibits complete stability over time, with another one third of the variance showing moderate stability, and the remaining instability attributable to contextual circumstances.
Many longitudinal studies have examined SWB trends over time. For example, Blanchflower and Oswald (2004) found that happiness declined through the last quarter of the twentieth century in the U.S.; however, they found almost no change in Great Britain during the same time period. More recently, Ortiz-Ospina and Roser (2018) analyzed SWB trends from the World Value Survey (WVS) and found that 49 of 69 countries have positive happiness trends over time (1984–2014). Other findings also show that SWB differs across regions. For example, Morrison and Weckroth (2018) found that metropolitan inhabitants of Finland had significantly lower average life satisfaction than their non-metropolitan counterparts. In a more nuanced report, Requena (2016) concludes that in wealthier countries, those living in rural areas have higher levels of SWB relative to those living in urbanized centers, while city dwellers in less prosperous countries have higher SWB relative to their counterparts in lower-density regions.
To study the contextual effects on SWB in a more systematic manner, many researchers include specific context-control variables in their models. Such variables might include GDP (Diener et al. 2010), unemployment rate (Di Tella et al. 2001; Ochsen 2011), social inequality (Kelley and Evans 2017), democratic governance (Frey and Stutzer 2000), geographic characteristics (e.g., sunshine hours) (Oswald and Wu 2010), culture (Oishi 2006), etc.
For longitudinal across-region SWB surveys, the best case is to use consistently designed surveys such as the WVS. However, such resources will commonly not exist if researchers want to study SWB with specific domain variables. Given that we used a fused dataset developed from five travel-behavior oriented surveys, there were differences in sampling methods across component surveys. Based on literature showing that stratified samples from online panels are not representative of the entire population regarding demographic characteristics, attitudes, and behaviors (Blasius and Brandt 2010; Fan and Yan 2010; Szolnoki and Hoffmann 2013), we introduce a context variable to control for varying sample sources.
To summarize, in this literature review section, we introduced SWB as a conceptual construct and discussed prominent measurement philosophies, providing support for our use of a single-item measure for life satisfaction. We then drew from the literature to examine the effects of demographic characteristics, travel-related attributes, personality/attitudinal constructs, and contextual variables on SWB, providing a foundation to better understand the models developed for this analysis.

Overview of data

Cross-sectional surveys

This study utilizes a fused dataset of five California-based, transport-oriented cross-sectional surveys covering a 27-year period from 1992 to 2018. These five surveys were selected for this specific analysis as they all contained the same life satisfaction question, which is the key dependent variable in this study, and were implemented within California, creating a degree of homogeneity of geographic context. One co-author of this paper was responsible for or integrally involved with the survey design, development, and implementation processes across all surveys considered for this data fusion process, while other co-authors were responsible for or integrally involved with one or more of them. Table 1 provides an overview of key characteristics of the surveys; here, we discuss these characteristics, providing additional context regarding the original goals and distinctive features of each survey.
Table 1
Overview of cross-sectional surveys that comprise the fused dataset
Year
Survey focus
Location
Sampling method
Completion channel
Response rate (%)a
Working sample sizeb (N)
1992
Telecommuting
Southern California
Convenience sampling (at a single employer)
Paper
44
601
1998
Positive travel utility
Northern California
Simple, address-based, random sampling within three diverse neighborhoods
Paper
24
1,317
2011
Travel-based multitasking
Northern California
Convenience sampling (on-site physical distribution, university staff and student emails, other online);
Simple random sampling, email addresses within study areas, address-based;
Quota sampling using an online opinion panel (Survey Analytics);
Paper, online
5c
2,415
2015
Mobility choices of millennials and Gen X
California
Quota sampling using an online opinion panel
Online
46
1,155
2018
Impacts of emerging technologies and transportation trends
California
Stratified random sampling, address-based;
Quota sampling using an online opinion panel;
Convenience sampling, recall of respondents from 2015 survey
Paper, online
7.01d
2,026
aFor initial samples, which have been filtered for the purposes of this study
bFor the purposes of this study; comprises workers who commute
cComposite across the methods for which a rate could be computed; in some cases the denominator was unknown or (e.g., in the case of links posted to a website) not applicable
dThe response rate is derived from the stratified address-based random sample (1,992 responses from 30,000 invitations) and the recall of respondents from the 2015 survey (246 responses from 1,939 invitations). The number of invitations for the online opinion panel was not provided by the panel vendor
The earliest survey, conducted in 1992, was deployed with the overarching goal of studying telecommuting, then defined as the concept of working from home or a location closer to home, thus eliminating the need to travel to and from work (i.e., commuting). Telecommuting was hailed as an exciting game-changer in the early 1990s, and seen as a strategy for reducing congestion and emissions. The survey was deployed to six of twenty-seven departments in the City of San Diego, California, and all regular employees within those departments were surveyed. This convenience sampling method was not used in any of the remaining five surveys, and resulted in a very high response rate of 44% (Mokhtarian and Salomon 1996).
The next survey in the series was conducted six years later in 1998, and was deployed with the intention of measuring the existence and impact of positive attitudes toward travel itself (particularly local daily travel), in contrast to the conventional view of travel as a disutility, undertaken purely for instrumental reasons of reaching a desired destination. This survey used address-based simple random sampling across three judgmentally-selected San Francisco Bay Area neighborhoods with diverse land-use, travel, and demographic patterns; the overall survey had a response rate of 24% (Curry 2000). Although surveys involving the same investigator that were conducted in 2003, 2006, and 2009 were also considered, the next one with enough commonalities to be included in the fused dataset of this current study occurred in 2011.
The 2011 survey focused on multitasking during commute travel (i.e., attitudes and behaviors related to travel-based multitasking). This survey used several sampling methods (see Table 1) with the goal of obtaining sizable (rather than representative) shares of all pertinent means of travel (modes). Survey distribution channels included physical distribution on public transit, employee/student email lists from organizations including a large university, a large email list of Sacramento-area commuters interested in alternatives to solo driving, email and mailing addresses purchased from commercial marketing companies, and survey links posted on transportation agency and corporation websites. Where measurable, response rates across the various sampling channels varied from 0.23% (for the email addresses purchased from a marketing corporation) to 18.2% (for the physical distribution on public transit; Neufeld and Mokhtarian 2012).
The next survey included in the dataset was conducted in 2015, and focused on the mobility choices of Generation Y (Millennials, born in 1981–1997) and Generation X (born in 1965–1980). This study used an online opinion panel, and applied a quota sampling approach using targets for gender, age, race, ethnicity, household income, and presence of children in an effort to ensure diverse representation of the population in California. This sampling method resulted in a high response rate of 46.3%, which is not surprising as the invitations for this survey were sent to members of an online opinion panel. These individuals had previously self-selected into the panel, and were available to complete surveys in exchange for incentives in the form of reward points, gift cards or airline miles (Circella et al. 2016).
The final survey included in the fused dataset was fielded in 2018, which is the second wave of the 2015 survey. The 2018 survey aimed to study the impacts of emerging technologies (e.g., ride-hailing services, autonomous vehicles) and transportation trends through a unique longitudinal approach. Specifically, part of the sampling frame of the 2018 survey came from the 2015 survey respondents. Among the re-contacted respondents, 246 people completed the 2018 survey. The rest of the 2018 survey respondents were recruited from two sources: a stratified, address-based random sample, and a quota sample from an online opinion panel (Circella et al. 2019, see Table 1 for the response rate).

Data fusion framework

The basic process we undertook to fuse the surveys discussed in the preceding section can perhaps serve as a framework for others interested in such an approach. We first developed a question “inventory” spreadsheet, which included all survey questions and possible response choices across the surveys. Next, we categorized the survey questions using a four-level hierarchical classification system, the first level of which specified a general, broad category for each survey question. General categories included attitudes, socio-demographic characteristics, travel attributes, land use characteristics, and specific survey focused questions. Next, we categorized each survey question according to its specific topic; for example, general values, environment, lifestyle, travel, time use, work, etc. The remaining categories in the hierarchy simply specified in greater detail what aspect of the topics are covered in each question. This hierarchical labeling system then allowed us to group questions that conveyed essentially identical meanings based on content and possible answer choices, while allowing for minor wording and formatting differences. In the next section, we provide descriptive statistics for the dependent variable in this study (i.e., satisfaction with life), as well as the common explanatory variables used across the surveys, and detail some of the adjustments that were made to facilitate the data fusion process.

Variables used in the study

For the purposes of this analysis, we restrict our sample to commuters only (i.e., workers who travel to and from work), to increase the homogeneity of context across all survey datasets. Furthermore, by filtering out the non-workers, we are able to retain more travel-related (specifically, commute-related) variables in the model. After removing inattentive and incomplete cases, the final fused dataset comprises 7,514 valid cases for use in this study (see Table 1 for sample size by survey). Each of the five surveys obtained life satisfaction ratings by asking respondents to rate the statement “I am generally satisfied with my life” using a five-point Likert-type response scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5).
Figure 1a presents the life satisfaction rating distribution for each survey, and illustrates that there is an increasing trend over the years with the exception of the survey conducted in 2015—which only used an online opinion panel to sample respondents, and also had the narrowest age range. This observation prompted us to compare the group of all opinion panel respondents (part of the 2011 and 2018 sample [Fig. 1b], and all of the 2015 sample) to everyone else, and we found that there was a significantly lower mean life satisfaction (t = 9.326, d.f. = 5450.8, p < 0.001) for the opinion panel group. However, because recruitment via an opinion panel (particularly in 2015; see the “Cross-sectional surveys” section and Table 1) is somewhat confounded with belonging to age groups whose SWB might be expected to be lower than average (Millennials having entered the workforce during a major recession, and Gen Xers being near the bottom of the U-shaped relationship of age to SWB as described in the “Demographic characteristics” section), we revisit this effect in the “Model estimation and analysis” section, using generalized ordered probit models that control for age and other variables. The overall mean for the fused dataset was 3.92 (out of 5).
Figure 2 presents the life satisfaction rating distribution by region. We see that among the three California regions, Northern Californians have the highest average life satisfaction, followed by respondents from “other” California regions and Southern Californians.
Table 2 summarizes descriptive statistics for the explanatory variables used in the models, which are presented in the “Model estimation and analysis”. Prior to executing the models, it was necessary to transform several variables to obtain a consistent scale across surveys. For example, the original five surveys have different household income categories to reflect income distributions at the time of survey implementation. For this modeling effort, we used the mid-point of each income category from the original surveys and converted this to the equivalent purchasing power in June 2018 (the implementation date for the last survey in the fused dataset). To do this, we used the consumer price index (CPI) from the Bureau of Labor Statistics to convert all category midpoints to “2018 dollars”, after which we classified the converted household incomes into the six income categories used in the 2018 survey, in keeping with the need for consistency across surveys. The income distribution after conversion is shown in Table 2. This is detailed here as an example of the types of consistency conversions necessary when fusing cross-sectional surveys across time.
Table 2
Descriptive statistics for variables common across surveys
Variables
1992
 
1998
 
2011
 
2015
 
2018
 
Full sample
 
ATT subsamplea
 
 
N = 601
 
N = 1,317
 
N = 2,415
 
N = 1,155
 
N = 2,026
 
N = 7,514
 
N = 4,887
 
Continuous variables
\(\overline{X }\)
s.d
\(\overline{X }\)
s.d
\(\overline{X }\)
s.d
\(\overline{X }\)
s.d
\(\overline{X }\)
s.d
\(\overline{X }\)
s.d
\(\overline{X }\)
s.d
Age (years)
39.82
9.91
43.60
12.05
44.46
12.63
34.36
8.27
44.29
13.38
42.34
12.51
41.84
12.31
Commute time (min)
25.90
13.13
29.76
20.36
41.64
31.73
28.40
23.44
28.23
22.41
32.65
25.86
35.31
27.90
GDP per capita ($1,000)b
38.27
0.00
44.57
0.00
55.56
0.00
62.68
0.00
67.70
0.00
56.56
9.53
54.19
6.43
Unemployment rate (%)c
9.20
0.00
3.94
0.00
10.04
0.00
6.10
1.43
4.19
1.23
6.72
2.81
7.47
2.75
General attitudes
              
 Like large yard
3.65
1.06
3.79
1.08
3.92
0.89
3.78
1.04
 Car is a symbol
2.24
1.05
2.30
1.10
2.90
1.12
2.43
1.12
 Don’t mind being stuck in traffic
2.01
0.99
1.98
0.97
2.43
1.05
2.10
1.01
 Travel is wasted time
2.85
1.06
2.60
1.00
2.90
1.06
2.74
1.04
 Commute is stressful
2.68
1.10
2.36
0.98
2.57
1.05
2.50
1.04
Categorical variables
N
%
N
%
N
%
N
%
N
%
N
%
N
%
Socio-economic and demographic
              
Annual household income (2018 purchasing power)
 Less than $25,000
5
0.8
30
2.3
158
6.5
67
5.8
125
6.2
385
5.1
255
5.2
 $25,000 to $49,999
108
18.0
140
10.6
347
14.4
189
16.4
311
15.3
1,095
14.6
676
13.8
 $50,000 to $74,999
0
0.0
268
20.3
493
20.4
438
37.9
355
17.5
1,554
20.7
1,199
24.5
 $75,000 to $99,999
192
31.9
245
18.6
470
19.5
165
14.3
327
16.1
1,399
18.6
880
18.0
 $100,000 to $149,999
228
37.9
222
16.9
392
16.2
156
13.5
445
22.0
1,443
19.2
770
15.8
 $150,000 or more
68
11.3
412
31.3
555
23.0
140
12.1
463
22.9
1,638
21.8
1,107
22.7
Household structure
              
 Living alone
115
19.1
326
24.8
402
16.6
185
16.0
295
14.6
1,323
17.6
913
18.7
 Living with others
486
80.9
991
75.2
2,013
83.4
970
84.0
1,731
85.4
5,191
82.4
3,974
81.3
Gender
              
 Male
287
47.8
644
48.9
960
39.8
533
46.1
1,001
49.4
3,425
45.6
2,137
43.7
 Female
314
52.2
673
51.1
1,455
60.2
622
53.9
1,025
50.6
4,089
54.4
2,750
56.3
Education
              
 Some high school
0
0.0
5
0.4
3
0.1
12
1.0
22
1.1
42
0.6
20
0.4
 Completed high school
15
2.5
70
5.3
66
2.7
106
9.2
134
6.6
391
5.2
242
5.0
 Some college
181
30.1
316
24.0
557
23.1
404
35.0
565
27.9
2,023
26.9
1,277
26.1
 Bachelor’s degree
171
28.5
450
34.2
785
32.5
436
37.7
788
38.9
2,630
35.0
1,671
34.2
 Some graduate school
84
14.0
151
11.5
255
10.6
0
0.0
0
0.0
490
6.5
406
8.3
 Completed graduate degree
150
25.0
325
24.7
749
31.0
197
17.1
517
25.5
1,938
25.8
1,271
26.0
Occupation
              
 Manager
288
21.9
398
16.5
210
18.2
8,96
18.3
 Other
1,029
78.1
2,017
83.5
945
81.8
3,991
81.7
Travel-related
              
Driver's license
              
 Do not have
5
0.8
18
1.4
83
3.4
59
5.1
51
2.5
216
2.9
160
3.3
 Have
596
99.2
1,299
98.6
2,332
96.6
1,096
94.9
1,975
97.5
7,298
97.1
4,727
96.7
Physical limitation—transit
              
 No
1,277
97.0
2,321
96.1
1,107
95.8
4,705
96.3
 Yes
40
3.0
94
3.9
48
4.2
182
3.7
Physical limitation—walk
              
 No
1,266
96.1
2,251
93.2
1,055
91.3
4,572
93.6
 Yes
51
3.9
164
6.8
100
8.7
315
6.4
Context-control
              
Region indicator
              
 Northern Californiad
0
0.0
1,317
100.0
2,415
100.0
452
39.1
773
38.1
4,957
66.0
4,184
85.6
 Southern Californiae
601
100.0
0
0.0
0
0.0
483
41.8
822
40.6
1,906
25.4
483
9.9
 Other California regions
0
0.0
0
0.0
0
0.0
220
19.1
431
21.3
651
8.7
220
4.5
Respondents
              
 From opinion panel
0
0.0
0
0.0
481
19.9
1,155
100.0
1,211
59.8
2,847
37.9
1,636
33.5
 From other sources
601
100.0
1,317
100.0
1,934
80.1
0
0.0
815
40.2
4,667
62.1
3,251
66.5
aThe ATT subsample includes the 1998, 2011, and 2015 surveys. We use the ATT subsample to develop the attitudinal model in “Full-Samle model” section. The ATT subsample has seven extra common variables (five continuous variables and two categorical variables), as shown
bContext-control variable derived from the U.S. Bureau of Economic Analysis (https://​www.​bea.​gov) (state level)
cContext-control variable derived from the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (https://​www.​bls.​gov) (county level)
dNorthern California includes the Sacramento Area Council of Governments (SACOG) and the San Francisco-based Metropolitan Transportation Commission (MTC) regional planning areas
eSouthern California includes the Los Angeles-based Southern California Association of Governments (SCAG) and San Diego Association of Governments (SANDAG) regional planning areas
To further simplify model development, we treat income and education as continuous variables in the models. The original age statistics for each survey are listed in Table 2. In the modeling portion of this paper, we use mean-centered age to provide more natural interpretations of the impact of age (by considering changes from the mean age rather than changes from 0 years old) and to introduce a quadratic term into the models (Enders 2016). Table 3 shows the slightly differing statements representing the five attitudinal variables retained in the fused dataset; respondents were asked to rate the statements using a five-point Likert-type response scale. We also note that similarly modest differences in wording exist not only in the attitudinal statements, but also in survey questions that obtain demographic and travel-related characteristics. We acknowledge that the subtle differences in wording between the surveys may influence respondents’ final responses, but note that such differences will be common consequences of fusing disparate datasets. We also exercised what we believe to be conservative judgments about the extent of differences that we considered acceptable; i.e., we eliminated questions/statements that we considered likely to result in rating differences across surveys. As a result, all retained variables are believed to convey the same meaning to respondents across surveys, although slight variations in wording are still present.
Table 3
Attitudinal variables common across surveys
Attitudinal variable
Survey
Survey statements
Like large yard
1998
I like to have a large yard at my home.
 
2011
I like the idea of living somewhere with large yards and lots of space between homes.
 
2015
I like the idea of living somewhere with large yards and lots of space between homes.
Car is a symbol
1998
To me, a car is a status symbol.
 
2011
I (would) like to own a car that impresses other people.
 
2015
To me, owning a car is a symbol of success.
Don’t mind being stuck in traffic
1998
Getting stuck in traffic doesn't bother me too much.
 
2011
Getting stuck in traffic doesn't bother me much.
 
2015
Getting stuck in traffic does not bother me that much.
Travel is wasted time
1998
Travel time is generally wasted time.
 
2011
Time spent traveling is generally wasted time.
 
2015
The time I spend commuting is generally wasted time.
Commute is stressful
1998
My commute is a real hassle.
 
2011
My commute is stressful.
 
2015
My commute is stressful.
Overall, the full sample consists of slightly more females than males (54% vs. 46%). More than 60% of respondents hold a bachelor’s degree or higher. Fewer than 20% of respondents live alone, and most of them (97%) hold a driver’s license. About two-thirds of the survey respondents come from Northern California (66%), compared to 25% Southern Californians and 9% Californians from other regions.

Model estimation and analysis

Two generalized ordered logit models for satisfaction with life are developed and presented here; we note that the first of these models uses the fused dataset across all five surveys, while the second model uses a reduced version of the fused dataset that includes three of the five surveys (1998, 2011, and 2015). The model utilizing all five surveys is described as the full-sample model. The full-sample model includes a geographically and chronologically widespread sample, with which we focus on the examination of context variables such as GDP, unemployment rate, and sampling method. The reduced model is described as the attitudinal model since it allows an examination of attitudinal variables. The full-sample model maximizes the sample size, while the second model enlarges the set of common variables to include five transport-related attitudinal variables, at the cost of losing two of the five surveys (Table 3). Besides these variables of particular interest (i.e., the contextual and attitudinal variables), we retain demographic characteristics and other travel-related variables in both models.

Generalized ordered logit model

In this study, satisfaction with life is measured using a five-point Likert-type response scale from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”. As such, ordinal logit (OL; ordered logit) models would serve this analysis well. However, the dataset used in this analysis violates the parallel lines assumption of OL models, a violation that frequently occurs in practice (Williams 2006). The parallel lines assumption requires corresponding coefficients (with the exception of the intercept) to be identical across different levels of the dependent variable.
Therefore, we use a less restrictive form of OL, the generalized ordered logit (GOL) model. GOL models relax the parallel lines restriction on the explanatory variables that violate this assumption, while keeping coefficients for the remaining explanatory variables in a parsimonious form (Williams 2016). We also considered using the multinomial logit (MNL) model, since MNL does not impose the parallel lines assumption across explanatory variables; however, MNL does not take into account the ordinal nature of the dependent variable. Furthermore, in empirical practice, the MNL specification would result in substantially more model coefficients than GOL. As a result, to balance parsimony, conceptual fidelity, and interpretability, we selected GOL models for use in the analysis presented here. The gologit2 specification in Stata/IC15.1 was used for model development (Williams 2006).
The GOL model specification can be written as follows:
$$P\left( {Y_{i} > j} \right) = g\left( {\alpha_{j} ,{\varvec{X}}_{i} {\varvec{\beta}}_{j} } \right) = \frac{{{\text{exp}}\left( {\alpha_{j} + {\varvec{X}}_{i} {\varvec{\beta}}_{j} } \right)}}{{1 + \left\{ {{\text{exp}}\left( {\alpha_{j} + {\varvec{X}}_{i} {\varvec{\beta}}_{j} } \right)} \right\}}}\quad j = 1,{ }2, \ldots ,M - 1,$$
(1)
where \({Y}_{i}\) represents the life satisfaction of respondent \(i\); \({\alpha }_{j}\) is the constant term associated with response j; \({X}_{i}\) is a vector of explanatory variables; \({\beta }_{j}\) is the corresponding vector of coefficients associated with response j (some, but not necessarily all, elements of which may be equal across some values of j); and \(M\) is the number of life satisfaction categories, which is five in this study; \({\alpha }_{j}+{X}_{i}{\beta }_{j}\) can be interpreted as the observed propensity for life satisfaction to be greater than response j. GOL models are different from OL models in that they allow \({\beta }_{j}\) to vary with \(j\); otherwise, the specifications for GOL and OL models are identical. The probability of each rating category is:
$$\begin{gathered} P\left( {Y_{i} = 1} \right) = 1 - g\left( {\alpha_{1} ,{\varvec{X}}_{i} {\varvec{\beta}}_{1} } \right) \hfill \\ P\left( {Y_{i} = j} \right) = g\left( {\alpha_{j - 1} ,{\varvec{X}}_{i} {\varvec{\beta}}_{j - 1} } \right) - g\left( {\alpha_{j} ,{\varvec{X}}_{i} {\varvec{\beta}}_{j} } \right)\quad j = 2, \ldots ,M - 1 \hfill \\ P\left( {Y_{i} = M} \right) = g\left( {\alpha_{M - 1} ,{\varvec{X}}_{i} {\varvec{\beta}}_{M - 1} } \right) \hfill \\ \end{gathered}$$
(2)
From this it can be seen that an increase in \({\alpha }_{1}+{{\varvec{X}}}_{i}{{\varvec{\beta}}}_{1}\) will unequivocally decrease the probability of the lowest life satisfaction response, and an increase in \({\alpha }_{M-1}+{{\varvec{X}}}_{i}{{\varvec{\beta}}}_{M-1}\) will unequivocally increase the probability of the highest response, but the effect of increases in \({\alpha }_{j}+{{\varvec{X}}}_{i}{{\varvec{\beta}}}_{j}\) on the middle three responses is ambiguous (Greene 2018). For this reason, we will interpret variables that have consistent effects across life satisfaction levels (i.e. for which the \({\beta }_{j}\)’s are equal across j) in terms of increases or decreases in the propensity for life satisfaction, which is unambiguous. For variables that have differing effects across life satisfaction levels (i.e. for which the \({\beta }_{j}\)’s vary across j), we will calculate the average marginal effects to illustrate their impact on each life satisfaction response level.

Full-sample model

The final GOL model developed for this analysis relaxes the parallel lines assumption for household income and the three context-control variables (i.e., GDP per capita, unemployment rate, and the opinion panel indicator), as we found that there are different effects of household income and context-control variables across life satisfaction levels. Table 4 shows the final model results, and indicates that overall, the full-sample model has an acceptable model fit with a \({\rho }^{2}\) (equally-likely base) of 0.30. We will interpret the explanatory variables found to have significant predictive power for life satisfaction ratings. In addition, Table 5 presents the marginal effects of the explanatory variables whose coefficients have been allowed to relax the parallel line assumption, namely, household income, GDP per capita, unemployment rate, and the opinion panel indicator. Specifically, we present two groups of statistics—actual probability changes and percentage changes in the probability—since each offers meaningful but different insights in view of the unbalanced shares of the five responses. For example, a large incremental change in probability could represent a small percentage change if the baseline share is large, while conversely, a small incremental change could be a large percentage of a small share.
Table 4
Full-sample models of life satisfaction
Variable
Generalized ordered logit
Generalized ordered logit without contextual variables
Coefficient
z−value
Coefficient
z−value
Socioeconomic and demographic
 
Household income (SD | D)
0.359***
4.71
0.402***
5.35
Household income (D | N)
0.326***
10.44
0.318***
10.44
Household income (N | A)
0.273***
12.84
0.268***
12.90
Household income (A | SA)
0.151***
7.28
0.150***
7.39
Living with others
0.223***
3.64
0.277***
4.60
Age (mean-centered)
−0.00210
−1.06
0.00386**
2.03
Age (mean-centered) squared
0.000763***
5.84
0.000944***
7.31
Female
0.178***
3.91
0.182***
4.02
Education
0.0743***
3.83
0.0933***
4.90
Travel-related
    
Have a driver’s license
0.358***
2.67
0.377***
2.82
Commute time
−0.00416***
−4.61
−0.00294***
−3.35
Context-control
    
GDP per capita (SD | D)
−0.0153
−0.97
GDP per capita (D | N)
0.0215***
3.51
GDP per capita (N | A)
0.0199***
4.99
GDP per capita (A | SA)
0.0487***
13.24
Unemployment rate (SD | D)
10.548**
2.39
Unemployment rate (D | N)
6.922***
4.17
Unemployment rate (N | A)
3.948***
3.61
Unemployment rate (A | SA)
−1.710
−1.16
From opinion panel (SD | D)
−0.448
−1.64
From opinion panel (D | N)
−0.525***
−4.41
From opinion panel (N | A)
−0.559***
−7.23
From opinion panel (A | SA)
−0.726***
−10.08
Thresholds
    
Threshold 1 (SD | D)
2.668***
2.59
1.873***
6.50
Threshold 2 (D | N)
−1.081***
−2.72
0.192
1.07
Threshold 3 (N | A)
−1.820***
−6.39
−0.849***
−5.12
Threshold 4 (A | SA)
−5.152***
−17.85
−2.960***
−16.97
Model summary
    
Number of cases
 
7,514
 
7,514
Log-likelihood (0)
 
−12,093.32
 
−12,093.32
Log-likelihood (thresholds)
 
−8,853.27
 
−8,853.27
Log-likelihood \((\widehat{\beta })\)
 
−8,494.77
 
−8,632.21
\({\rho }^{2}\) (equally-likely base)
 
0.2976
 
0.2862
\({\rho }^{2}\) (thresholds-only base)
 
0.0405
 
0.0250
For this sample, the shares of each response are strongly disagree (SD): 1.25%, disagree (D): 6.71%, neutral (N): 13.79%, agree (A): 54.88%, and strongly agree (SA): 23.37%
***Coefficient is statistically significant at the 0.01 level
**Coefficient is statistically significant at the 0.05 level
*Coefficient is statistically significant at the 0.1 level
Table 5
Average marginal effects of variables with relaxed parallel line assumption in the full-sample model of life satisfaction
  
Strongly disagree
Disagree
Neutral
Agree
Strongly agree
Baseline shares
 
0.0125
0.0671
0.1379
0.5488
0.2337
Household income
Probability changea
−0.00438
−0.0187
−0.0209
0.0184
0.0256
 
% change in probabilityb
−35%
−29%
−17%
3%
12%
GDP per capita
Probability changea
0.000187
−0.00171
−0.00168
−0.00502
0.00823
 
% change in probabilityb
1.5%
−2.6%
−1.3%
−0.9%
3.7%
Unemployment rate
Probability changea
−0.129
−0.362
−0.145
0.925
−0.289
 
% change in probabilityb
−1042%
−566%
−133%
174%
−131%
From opinion panel
Probability changec
0.00539
0.0332
0.0547
0.0249
−0.118
 
% change in probabilityd
44%
49%
40%
5%
−56%
aThe probability changes of each response level from a one-unit increase in the explanatory variable. For each explanatory variable, probability changes across the five response levels sum to one
bThe proportional changes in the probability of each response level given a one-unit increase in the explanatory variable
cThe probability changes associated with belonging to an opinion panel, for each response level. The probability changes across the five response levels sum to one
dThe proportional changes in probability associated with belonging to an opinion panel, for each response level
We look first at household level demographic characteristics. As expected, the model shows that increased household income levels tend to increase the propensity for satisfaction with life. Interestingly, the magnitudes of the coefficients decline as life satisfaction levels increase. This suggests that those with lower life satisfaction tend to have greater returns on their satisfaction propensity as their income levels increase. The trend is consistent with the marginal effects (percentage changes in probability). Specifically, a one-level increase in household income results in larger percentage changes in probability for lower life satisfaction levels than for higher life satisfaction levels. The second household-level variable is household structure, which shows that households with more members have an increased propensity to experience greater satisfaction with life relative to those who live alone. Consistent with numerous SWB studies showing the importance of social relationships in general, and close family ties in particular, living with other family members on net brings both practical and emotional support for the burdens of daily life.
Turning now to individual level demographic characteristics, we see that age has a U-shaped relationship with life satisfaction propensity. This indicates that individuals’ life satisfaction tends to have a declining trend during their early life stages, with the lowest life satisfaction occurring around the age of 44, on average. After this turning point, people have a greater tendency to be satisfied with their lives. The trend, which is consistent with studies in the literature (Beutel et al. 2010), is conceptually intuitive, as we can conceive that those in the middle-aged portion of life may have a greater number of stressors—career building, marriages, children, ailing parents—that may result in anxiety and decreased life satisfaction (hence the stereotypical mid-life crisis, e.g., Rosenberg et al. (1999). Figure 3 intuitively illustrates the average changes in the probabilities of the five response levels as age varies from 18 to 93. The probability of strongly agreeing with the life satisfaction statement forms a U-shaped curve across adulthood. In contrast, the probabilities of the other responses do not form such curves due to the restriction that the probabilities of the five response levels sum to one. Still, the figure indicates that people in middle age have the lowest propensity to be highly satisfied with their life and the highest propensity to be less satisfied. In terms of gender, the model indicates that, in general, females tend to have a higher propensity for life satisfaction than males (Welsch and Kuehling 2017). We see that individuals with higher levels of education tend toward greater life satisfaction. Regarding occupation, we see that those in managerial positions have an increased propensity to be more satisfied with their life relative to those in other occupations.
Regarding transport attributes, we see that increased commute time tends to have a negative effect on life satisfaction. Commuting is a recurring event, often conducted under time pressure and in less-than-pleasant circumstances, which occupies much of an individual’s “travel budget” and serves as a spatio-temporal anchor for many other activities. In the U.S., the vast majority of commuting is performed by driving an automobile, and especially for drivers but also for many others, commute time cannot be used as productively as may be desired (Shaw et al. 2019). For these reasons, among others, it is not surprising that longer commutes have a deleterious effect on travel well-being (Smith 2017) and overall life satisfaction (Hilbrecht et al. 2014).
On the other hand, we also see that having a driver’s license has a positive effect on life satisfaction propensity. As an instrument of motility (De Vos et al. 2013), license possession can increase one’s mobility by providing travel flexibility and an increased radius of potential travel. As witnessed by the literature on the mobility of the elderly, simply being able to drive can intuitively increase overall life satisfaction (Banister and Bowling 2004). Thus, we see that commuting specifically has an opposing effect to that of travel freedom generally—probably because of the lack of freedom found in much commuting.
The three contextual variables—GDP per capita, unemployment rate, and sampling method—have differing effects across life satisfaction levels. In general, people from regions with a higher GDP per capita are more likely to feel satisfied with life. Also, GDP per capita has the most substantial effects (i.e., the largest positive coefficient) on people who have a high propensity to feel satisfied with life. However, based on the marginal effects shown in Table 5, a one-unit ($1,000) increase in GDP per capita also results in a slight increase in the probability of the lowest life satisfaction level. Interestingly, the unemployment rate positively associates with life satisfaction, i.e., a higher unemployment rate is related to higher life satisfaction. One potential explanation resides in recalling the target group of this study, i.e., employed people. Considering their unemployed peers, people who do not lose their jobs when the unemployment rate is high may be more appreciative of their life than at other times. The marginal effects in Table 5 further show that a one-percentage-point increase in the unemployment rate will result in a probability increase of the medium–high level of life satisfaction (“Agree”) and probability reductions for the other life satisfaction levels.
In addition to the specific context-control variables of GDP per capita and unemployment rate, we considered including context-control indicators to capture the average impacts of certain unobserved contextual factors. First, we tested using region and year indicators as substitutes for the specific contextual variables in the full-sample model. See Table 7 in the Appendix for the model results. Compared to using specific contextual variables, the interpretability of the context-control indicators is relatively weak. For example, in the context indicator model, we see that life satisfaction has an increasing trend over the years,2 while people from southern and other California regions have a higher life satisfaction than people from northern California. However, the sources of, or reasons for, these life satisfaction differences are unknown. Further, the fit of the context indicator model is lower than that of the model using specific contextual variables. We also tested including the context-control indicators together with the specific context-control variables; however, the two indicators were insignificant when GDP per capita and the unemployment rate were also in the model.
Regarding the sampling method, online opinion panel respondents tend to have lower life satisfaction propensities relative to respondents recruited using the other sampling methods (mainly address-based sampling). One possible explanation is that the two groups of people have different purposes for survey participation. The online panel respondents are people who previously registered at some websites for survey participation with rewards, while the other sampling methods (mainly address-based sampling) recruit essentially volunteer respondents (even though most of the surveys provided some small incentives). With this in mind, we can see that the online panel respondents may have more financial pressure and possibly more ennui, while the volunteer respondents are more likely both to be in a benevolent mood when they started the survey, and to have their sense of well-being further improved by the knowledge of being helpful by completing the survey.
To further analyze the effects of contextual variables, we executed a restricted version of the model with contextual variables removed (Table 4). As we can see, coefficients for the remaining variables do not change dramatically, which suggests the model’s stability and the independent explanatory power of the two groups of variables.3 The likelihood ratio test for the removal of a block of variables decisively rejects the null hypothesis that all the contextual variables are irrelevant (\({\chi }^{2}\) = 274.88, d.f. = 12, p < 0.001), which indicates the importance of contextual influences in life satisfaction modeling.
Table 6
Attitudinal models of life satisfaction
Variable
Model specification with attitudes
Model specification without attitudes
 
Coefficient
z−value
Coefficient
z−value
Socioeconomic and demographic
Household income (SD | D)
0.442***
3.94
0.421***
3.76
Household income (D | N)
0.306***
7.82
0.282***
7.26
Household income (N | A)
0.270***
9.98
0.242***
9.08
Household income (A | SA)
0.151***
5.62
0.125***
4.74
Living with others
0.144*
1.88
0.211***
2.79
Age
−0.00874***
−3.40
−0.00632*
−2.50
Age squared
0.000772***
4.30
0.000786***
4.42
Female
0.176***
3.03
0.220***
3.82
Education
0.0973***
3.93
0.0796***
3.26
Manager
0.254***
3.40
0.266***
3.58
Travel-related
Have a driver’s license
0.496***
3.11
0.381**
2.40
Commute time
−0.00261**
−2.34
−0.00450***
−4.23
Physical limitation—transit
−0.288*
−1.81
−0.375**
−2.36
Physical limitation—walk
−0.375***
−3.02
−0.350***
−2.82
General attitudes
Like large yard
0.0736***
2.60
Car is a symbol
−0.0587**
−2.17
Don't mind being stuck in traffic
0.145***
4.83
Travel is wasted time
−0.179***
−5.89
Commute is stressful
−0.196***
−6.18
Context-control
Region indicator (base: Northern Californiaa)
 Southern Californiab
0.0797
0.72
0.0709
0.64
 Other California regions
−0.302**
−2.03
−0.274*
−1.86
Unemployment rate (SD | D)
5.817
1.03
7.679
1.36
Unemployment rate (D | N)
9.047***
4.35
10.768***
5.24
Unemployment rate (N | A)
5.063***
3.61
6.728***
4.90
Unemployment rate (A | SA)
4.015***
2.88
5.525***
4.07
From opinion panel
−0.344***
−4.48
−0.362***
−4.82
Thresholds
Threshold 1 (SD | D)
2.391***
4.02
1.895***
3.34
Threshold 2 (D | N)
0.341
1.06
−0.121
0.45
Threshold 3 (N | A)
−0.503*
−1.74
−0.926***
−4.05
Threshold 4 (A | SA)
−2.845***
−9.43
−3.183***
−13.15
Model summary
 
Number of cases
 
4,887
 
4,887
Log-likelihood (0)
 
−7,865.32
 
−7,865.32
Log-likelihood (thresholds)
 
−5,572.50
 
−5,572.50
Log-likelihood \((\widehat{\beta })\)
 
−5,304.59
 
−5,393.12
\({\rho }^{2}\) (equally-likely base)
 
0.3256
 
0.3143
\({\rho }^{2}\) (thresholds-only base)
 
0.0481
 
0.0322
***Coefficient is statistically significant at the 0.01 level
**Coefficient is statistically significant at the 0.05 level
*Coefficient is statistically significant at the 0.1 level
Note: For this sample, the shares of each response are strongly disagree (SD): 0.94%, disagree (D): 6.92%, neutral (N): 13.75%, agree (A): 58.13%, and strongly agree (SA): 20.26%
aNorthern California includes the Sacramento Area Council of Governments (SACOG) and the San Francisco-based Metropolitan Transportation Commission (MTC) regional planning areas
bSouthern California includes the Los Angeles-based Southern California Association of Governments (SCAG) and the San Diego Association of Governments (SANDAG) regional planning areas

Attitudinal model

As noted, the attitudinal model (Table 6) is developed using a reduced version of the fused dataset, comprising only three surveys (the 1998, 2011, and 2015 surveys), and thus facilitating the inclusion of a larger set of common variables. To be specific, the attitudinal model includes all explanatory variables used in the full-sample model (as shown in Table 4) as well as seven additional variables that encompass travel-related attributes and attitudes. The attitudinal and full-sample models have similar model results (coefficient and significance) for the shared explanatory variables; accordingly, for economy of presentation, we here focus on interpreting the seven new variables.
Two of the seven additional explanatory variables are travel-related attributes that relate to whether respondents have physical conditions or anxieties that limit their use of transit or walking (i.e., Physical limitation—transit and Physical limitation—walk). Results indicate that those with physical limitations have lower life satisfaction propensities, likely due to restricted mobility. Further, the physical limitations may also reflect the presence of health problems that are not directly measured by our surveys, but which conceivably have negative effects on life satisfaction.
The five additional explanatory variables are transport-oriented attitudinal statements, which reflect general values as well. The first statement, “Like large yard,” captures a residential preference for living in locations that allow large yards and lots of space between homes. This statement is usually associated with a pro-suburban attitudinal factor in prior works (e.g., Kim et al., 2019). The positive coefficient indicates that people who prefer living in suburban areas have increased propensities to be satisfied with their lives. Not surprisingly, the attitudinal result is consistent with the behavioral result; for example, Sander (2011) found that those who live in less urban areas have higher levels of happiness. A possible explanation of the positive relationship between pro-suburban attitudes and life satisfaction is that the suburban life is still an aspiration for many Americans, and may therefore be perceived to offer a more satisfactory living environment. The second attitude, “car as a symbol”, reflects the respondent’s vanity with respect to owning a car. The results show that respondents who regard cars as status symbols tend to be less satisfied with their lives, perhaps because there will always be others with more status, to whom it is disappointing to compare oneself. Similarly, Olivos et al. (2020) have shown that upward social comparisons have negative effects on life satisfaction.
The last three attitudinal statements (“Don’t mind being stuck in traffic,” “Travel is wasted time,” and “Commute is stressful”) capture general preferences toward time use, travel liking, and commuting. Those who don’t mind being stuck in traffic have increased propensities for life satisfaction, while—consistently—those who see travel as a waste or commuting as stressful have reduced propensities. These are expected results, especially considering that such attitudes may reflect not only a specific affect toward travel, but also an optimistic/pessimistic outlook on life in general. Note that these attitudes substantially reduce the impact of commute time per se: when attitudes are excluded (as in the second model in Table 6), the coefficient of commute time becomes markedly more negative in compensation, which is not surprising. Nevertheless, even controlling for these attitudes, commute time remains strongly significant.
Thus, we see that the inclusion of these five attitudinal statements allowed for the interpretation of life satisfaction from more dimensions (e.g., personality, time use, travel liking). In Table 6, we also present a model identical to the one discussed here, except with the attitudinal variables removed. Similar to the results for the full-sample model, we note that the remaining explanatory variables have relatively stable coefficients after the attitudes are removed, and also that the likelihood ratio test for the removal of a block of variables decisively rejects the null hypothesis that all the attitudinal variables are irrelevant (\({\chi }^{2}\) = 177.06, d.f. = 5, p < 0.001), which indicates the importance of attitudes in life satisfaction modeling.

Discussion

In this section, we further discuss our research findings and limitations. Firstly, we note that although the life satisfaction models presented have acceptable \({\rho }^{2}\) s using the equally likely benchmark (0.2976 and 0.3256), the \({\rho }^{2}\) fit statistics using the thresholds-only benchmark (0.0405 and 0.0481) appear quite low. However, this is a typical outcome when the distribution of responses is unbalanced across the points on the ordinal scale, as is the case here (see Fig. 1 and the footnote to Table 4), and does not give a fair picture of the model’s explanatory power (Mokhtarian 2016). Furthermore, even if using the thresholds-only benchmark, this model fit is consistent with life satisfaction models in the literature. For example, ordered logit/probit models usually have a pseudo-\({R}^{2}\) less than 0.05 (Blanchflower and Oswald 2004; Nie and Sousa-Poza 2016). Linear regression models usually have \({R}^{2}\) values around 0.2 (Helliwell and Putnam 2004) and adjusted \({R}^{2}\) values around 0.05 (Hilbrecht et al. 2014; Nikolaev 2015). It is also notable that many life satisfaction studies do not report model fits, thus precluding the comparison of their overall model performance relative to others in the literature. Low model fits for life satisfaction indicate that there is a sizable range of factors that influence individuals’ conscious evaluation of their lives, and at any one time, a typical study is only able to account for a small portion of these explanatory variables (Rojas 2006). Despite low overall model fits in the satisfaction domain, these models are still able to provide critical insights into the factors that influence life satisfaction across studies.
This study demonstrates the possibility of utilizing diverse cross-sectional surveys in a specific domain to examine factors that influence a variable across time and space, i.e., life satisfaction in this case. However, such an approach inevitably results in shortcomings of the resultant fused dataset. Although we have implemented various approaches to control for the influence of these limitations, we may expect residual problems to remain. For example, through the survey fusion process, we attempted to avoid the influence of different question wordings by systematic question categorization and careful manual selection of common questions. Despite this, we cannot be certain that we have excluded all wording ambiguities that may have influenced responses. Then, to account for the influence of different sampling methods, we used a context-control variable to distinguish different data sources. However, for certain sampling methods, inherent biases are difficult to remove, e.g., the self-selection bias of web surveys (Bethlehem 2010). In addition, question orders vary across surveys, which may influence respondents’ performance on certain questions (Erdogan et al. 2012). Despite these limitations, we believe that the approach shown in this paper can substantially increase the utility of small cross-sectional survey datasets, and thus, can allow for increased contributions to the literature.

Conclusion

This study develops generalized ordered logit (GOL) models to study the life satisfaction of commuters using a fused dataset of five cross-sectional surveys conducted in California between the years 1992 to 2018. Explanatory variables studied include demographic characteristics, travel-related attributes, attitudinal variables, and context-control variables. Regarding demographic characteristics, we find that higher income is associated with higher propensities for life satisfaction, but the same income increment has a greater return on satisfaction for less-satisfied groups. In line with previous studies, we see a U-shaped relationship between age and life satisfaction, suggesting that the lowest life satisfaction tends to occur while individuals are in their 40s. We also find that those who are female, more educated, and those who live with others have increased propensities toward higher life satisfaction.
Regarding travel attributes, we see that increased commute time and mobility limitations (e.g., not having a driver’s license, physical limitations) are associated with lower life satisfaction propensities. Since the fused survey dataset is multi-year and multi-region, the model uses GDP per capita and the unemployment rate to control for contextual influences. Results show that GDP per capita is positively associated with life satisfaction. Increased unemployment rates are associated with higher life satisfaction, which might be because the study focuses on employed people, who may feel fortunate compared to their unemployed peers. Moreover, we present a substitution model to demonstrate the usefulness of year and region indicators as context-control variables (albeit having lower explanatory power; see the Appendix) when specific variables such as GDP per capita and unemployment rate are not available. In this latter (context-control indicator) model, we find an increasing trend of life satisfaction over the years, which might relate to the increasing GDP per capita over the years.
Additionally, we find significantly lower mean life satisfaction for online panel respondents compared to those recruited via more traditional approaches, in support of other studies (Blasius and Brandt 2010; Fan and Yan 2010; Szolnoki and Hoffmann 2013) finding that online panel members are not representative of the general population with respect to a number of variables including attitudes. We also see that attitudes significantly improve the model fit, and aid in understanding the influence of latent characteristics on life satisfaction.
Overall, this study explores the influence of a wide range of variables on life satisfaction using a fused dataset. Combining multiple travel survey datasets to model life satisfaction enables analysis of the contextual variables and generalizable conclusions about the impact of the transportation-related attributes and attitudes on life satisfaction. It also, not insignificantly, creates a substantially larger sample, which can improve the precision of estimated parameters and (in other studies) permit segmentations that would not be practical with smaller samples. We hope that this study will serve as a foundation for other researchers in specific domains to explore the approach of fusing multiple survey datasets for the purpose of modeling life satisfaction or other key variables. We especially urge early-career scholars conducting survey-based studies to begin now to consider the possibility of fusing multiple samples in the future, and with an eye to doing so, to give intentional thought to (1) specific questions that could be repeated in multiple surveys, and (2) the need for optimizing uniformity of question and response wording across surveys. Based on our experiences, we recommend the development of a question inventory for common variable selection, as well as the inclusion of relevant context-control variables for models developed using fused datasets.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to the survey design and implementation teams for the following surveys: 1992 Telecommuting Survey, 1998 Mobility Survey, 2011 Multitasking Northern California Survey, 2015 Millennials Wave 1 Survey, 2016 NCHRP Bicycle Facility Survey, 2017 Georgia Department of Transportation Survey, and 2018 Panel Study of Emerging Transportation Technologies and Trends in California Wave 2 Survey. The original data collections for these surveys were funded by several different agencies, including US DOT, Caltrans, Georgia DOT, the Sustainable Transportation Center, and the National Center for Sustainable Transportation. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the sponsor organizations. The comments of the anonymous referees have improved the paper.

Declarations

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Appendix

Appendix

See Table 7 .
Table 7
Full-sample model of life satisfaction using context-control indicators
Variable
Generalized ordered logit
 
 
Coefficient
z−value
Socioeconomic and demographic
Household income (SD | D)
0.349***
4.54
Household income (D | N)
0.314***
10.07
Household income (N | A)
0.267***
12.61
Household income (A | SA)
0.165***
7.96
Living with others
0.229***
3.73
Age (mean-centered)
−0.00182
−0.92
Age (mean-centered) squared
0.000771***
5.91
Female
0.177***
3.88
Education
0.0744***
3.83
Travel-related
Have a driver’s license
0.345**
2.58
Commute time
−0.00428***
−4.77
Context-control
Year indicator (SD | D)
−0.0147
−0.80
Year indicator (D | N)
0.0244***
3.61
Year indicator (N | A)
0.0226***
5.08
Year indicator (A | SA)
0.0512***
12.28
Region Indicator (Region: Northern California)
 Southern California (SD | D)
−0.316
−0.56
 Southern California (D | N)
−0.0354
−0.35
 Southern California (N | A)
0.00375
0.05
 Southern California (A | SA)
0.205***
2.97
 Other California regions (SD | D)
−0.140
−0.42
 Other California regions (D | N)
−0.106
−0.69
 Other California regions (N | A)
−0.0415
−0.39
 Other California regions (A | SA)
0.268***
2.65
From opinion panel (SD | D)
−0.559**
−2.01
From opinion panel (D | N)
−0.579***
−4.82
From opinion panel (N | A)
−0.598***
−7.64
From opinion panel (A | SA)
−0.699***
−9.64
Thresholds
Threshold 1 (SD | D)
2.888***
6.25
Threshold 2 (D | N)
0.230
1.07
Threshold 3 (N | A)
−0.799***
−4.35
Threshold 4 (A | SA)
−3.596***
−18.61
Model summary
Number of cases
 
7,514
Log-likelihood (0)
 
−12,093.32
Log-likelihood (thresholds)
 
−8,853.27
Log-likelihood \((\widehat{\beta })\)
 
−8,514.04
\({\rho }^{2}\) (equally-likely base)
 
0.2960
\({\rho }^{2}\) (thresholds-only base)
 
0.0383
*Coefficient is statistically significant at the 0.1 level
**Coefficient is statistically significant at the 0.05 level
***Coefficient is statistically significant at the 0.01 level
Footnotes
1
In this section, we use SWB to broadly refer to multiple terms related to well-being evaluations, including subjective well-being, happiness, and life satisfaction. This is largely because scholars often use these terms interchangeably in different domains, thus precluding any attempts to disentangle the results (Gärling and Gamble 2018).
 
2
This result diverges from the literature. For example, Blanchflower and Oswald (2004) report that life satisfaction has been declining for the last quarter of the twentieth century. This may be attributable to a host of reasons. On the one hand, the study reported in this paper captures more recent time periods that are not reported in the literature, i.e., through 2018. On the other hand, differences in sampling method, survey design, and other factors may be contributing to the differences in trends as well. It is also pertinent to note that given the trend of declining survey response rates around the world (National Research Council 2013; Morton, Cahill and Hartge 2005), the sample of respondents who are willing to spend time to complete the fairly lengthy, detailed surveys analyzed here may be becoming increasingly less representative with each new cross-section, potentially biased toward having less time pressure, a greater sense of social responsibility, and/or more positive life attitudes.
 
3
To further test the stability of the model, we conducted bootstrap estimations with 200 replications for both the full-sample model and the attitudinal model (see the “Attitudinal model” section). The standard errors and the corresponding z-values are very similar between our one-time estimations (Tables 4 and 6) and the bootstrap estimations, additionally corroborating the stability of the coefficients
 
Literature
go back to reference Ajzen, I.: Attitudes, Personality, and Behavior. Open University Press, London, UK (2005) Ajzen, I.: Attitudes, Personality, and Behavior. Open University Press, London, UK (2005)
go back to reference Banister, D., Bowling, A.: Quality of life for the elderly: the transport dimension. Transp. Policy 11, 105–115 (2004)CrossRef Banister, D., Bowling, A.: Quality of life for the elderly: the transport dimension. Transp. Policy 11, 105–115 (2004)CrossRef
go back to reference Bethlehem, J.: Selection bias in web surveys. Int. Stat. Rev. 78(2), 161–188 (2010)CrossRef Bethlehem, J.: Selection bias in web surveys. Int. Stat. Rev. 78(2), 161–188 (2010)CrossRef
go back to reference Beutel, M.E., Glaesmer, H., Wiltink, J., Marian, H., Brähler, E.: Life satisfaction, anxiety, depression and resilience across the life span of men. Aging Male 13(1), 32–39 (2010)CrossRef Beutel, M.E., Glaesmer, H., Wiltink, J., Marian, H., Brähler, E.: Life satisfaction, anxiety, depression and resilience across the life span of men. Aging Male 13(1), 32–39 (2010)CrossRef
go back to reference Blanchflower, D.G., Oswald, A.J.: Well-being over time in Britain and the USA. J. Public Econ. 88(7–8), 1359–1386 (2004)CrossRef Blanchflower, D.G., Oswald, A.J.: Well-being over time in Britain and the USA. J. Public Econ. 88(7–8), 1359–1386 (2004)CrossRef
go back to reference Blanchflower, D.G., Oswald, A.J.: Is well-being U-shaped over the life cycle? Soc. Sci. Med. 66(8), 1733–1749 (2008)CrossRef Blanchflower, D.G., Oswald, A.J.: Is well-being U-shaped over the life cycle? Soc. Sci. Med. 66(8), 1733–1749 (2008)CrossRef
go back to reference Blasius, J., Brandt, M.: Representativeness in online surveys through stratified samples. Bull. Sociol. Methodol./bulletin De Methodologie Sociologique 107(1), 5–21 (2010)CrossRef Blasius, J., Brandt, M.: Representativeness in online surveys through stratified samples. Bull. Sociol. Methodol./bulletin De Methodologie Sociologique 107(1), 5–21 (2010)CrossRef
go back to reference Cheung, F., Lucas, R.E.: Assessing the validity of single-item life satisfaction measures: Results from three large samples. Qual. Life Res. 23(10), 2809–2818 (2014)CrossRef Cheung, F., Lucas, R.E.: Assessing the validity of single-item life satisfaction measures: Results from three large samples. Qual. Life Res. 23(10), 2809–2818 (2014)CrossRef
go back to reference Circella, G., Matson, G., Alemi, F., Handy, S.: Panel study of emerging transportation technologies and trends in California: Phase 2 data collection. National Center for Sustainable Transportation, University of California, Davis. (2019) Available at https://escholarship.org/uc/item/35x894mg Circella, G., Matson, G., Alemi, F., Handy, S.: Panel study of emerging transportation technologies and trends in California: Phase 2 data collection. National Center for Sustainable Transportation, University of California, Davis. (2019) Available at https://​escholarship.​org/​uc/​item/​35x894mg
go back to reference Cracolici, M.F., Giambona, F., Cuffaro, M.: Family structure and subjective economic well-being: Some new evidence. Soc. Indic. Res. 118(1), 433–456 (2014)CrossRef Cracolici, M.F., Giambona, F., Cuffaro, M.: Family structure and subjective economic well-being: Some new evidence. Soc. Indic. Res. 118(1), 433–456 (2014)CrossRef
go back to reference Cummins, R.A.: Measuring population happiness to inform public policy. Paper presented at the The 3rd OECD World Forum on “Statistics, Knowledge and Policy” Charting Progress, Building Visions, Improving Life, Busan, Korea (2009) Cummins, R.A.: Measuring population happiness to inform public policy. Paper presented at the The 3rd OECD World Forum on “Statistics, Knowledge and Policy” Charting Progress, Building Visions, Improving Life, Busan, Korea (2009)
go back to reference Curry, R.: Attitudes toward travel: the relationships among perceived mobility, travel liking, and relative desired mobility. Master’s Thesis, Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, University of California, Davis (2000) Curry, R.: Attitudes toward travel: the relationships among perceived mobility, travel liking, and relative desired mobility. Master’s Thesis, Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, University of California, Davis (2000)
go back to reference De Vos, J., Schwanen, T., Van Acker, V., Witlox, F.: Travel and subjective well-being: a focus on findings, methods and future research needs. Transp. Rev. 33(4), 421–442 (2013)CrossRef De Vos, J., Schwanen, T., Van Acker, V., Witlox, F.: Travel and subjective well-being: a focus on findings, methods and future research needs. Transp. Rev. 33(4), 421–442 (2013)CrossRef
go back to reference DeNeve, K.M., Cooper, H.: The happy personality: a meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 124(2), 197–229 (1998)CrossRef DeNeve, K.M., Cooper, H.: The happy personality: a meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 124(2), 197–229 (1998)CrossRef
go back to reference Di Tella, R., MacCulloch, R.J., Oswald, A.J.: Preferences over inflation and unemployment: evidence from surveys of happiness. American Economic Review 91(1), 335–341 (2001)CrossRef Di Tella, R., MacCulloch, R.J., Oswald, A.J.: Preferences over inflation and unemployment: evidence from surveys of happiness. American Economic Review 91(1), 335–341 (2001)CrossRef
go back to reference Diener, E.: Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. Am. Psychol. 55(1), 34–41 (2000)CrossRef Diener, E.: Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. Am. Psychol. 55(1), 34–41 (2000)CrossRef
go back to reference Diener, E., Biswas-Diener, R.: Will money increase subjective well-being? Soc. Indic. Res. 57(2), 119–169 (2002)CrossRef Diener, E., Biswas-Diener, R.: Will money increase subjective well-being? Soc. Indic. Res. 57(2), 119–169 (2002)CrossRef
go back to reference Diener, E., Gohm, C.L., Suh, E., Oishi, S.: Similarity of the relations between marital status and subjective well-being across cultures. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 31(4), 419–436 (2000)CrossRef Diener, E., Gohm, C.L., Suh, E., Oishi, S.: Similarity of the relations between marital status and subjective well-being across cultures. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 31(4), 419–436 (2000)CrossRef
go back to reference Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Tov, W., Arora, R.: Income’s association with judgments of life versus feelings. In: Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Helliwell, J.F. (eds.) International Differences in Well-being, pp. 3–15. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK (2010)CrossRef Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Tov, W., Arora, R.: Income’s association with judgments of life versus feelings. In: Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Helliwell, J.F. (eds.) International Differences in Well-being, pp. 3–15. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK (2010)CrossRef
go back to reference Diener, E., Oishi, S.: Money and happiness: Income and subjective well-being across nations. In: Diener, E., Suh, E.M. (eds.) Culture and Subjective Well-being, pp. 185–218. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, US (2000) Diener, E., Oishi, S.: Money and happiness: Income and subjective well-being across nations. In: Diener, E., Suh, E.M. (eds.) Culture and Subjective Well-being, pp. 185–218. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, US (2000)
go back to reference Diener, E., Oishi, S., Lucas, R.E.: Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Ann Rev. Psychol. 54(1), 403–425 (2003)CrossRef Diener, E., Oishi, S., Lucas, R.E.: Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Ann Rev. Psychol. 54(1), 403–425 (2003)CrossRef
go back to reference Enders, C.K.: Centering predictors and contextual effects. In: Scott, M.A., Simonoff, J.S., Marx, B.D. (eds.) The SAGE Handbook of Multilevel Modeling, pp. 89–108. SAGE Publications Ltd, London (2016) Enders, C.K.: Centering predictors and contextual effects. In: Scott, M.A., Simonoff, J.S., Marx, B.D. (eds.) The SAGE Handbook of Multilevel Modeling, pp. 89–108. SAGE Publications Ltd, London (2016)
go back to reference Erdogan, B., Bauer, T.N., Truxillo, D.M., Mansfield, L.R.: Whistle while you work: a review of the life satisfaction literature. J. Manag. 38(4), 1038–1083 (2012) Erdogan, B., Bauer, T.N., Truxillo, D.M., Mansfield, L.R.: Whistle while you work: a review of the life satisfaction literature. J. Manag. 38(4), 1038–1083 (2012)
go back to reference Eriksson, L., Friman, M., Gärling, T.: Perceived attributes of bus and car mediating satisfaction with the work commute. Transp. Res. Part A 47, 87–96 (2013) Eriksson, L., Friman, M., Gärling, T.: Perceived attributes of bus and car mediating satisfaction with the work commute. Transp. Res. Part A 47, 87–96 (2013)
go back to reference Frey, B.S., Stutzer, A.: Happiness, economy and institutions. Econ. J. 110(466), 918–938 (2000)CrossRef Frey, B.S., Stutzer, A.: Happiness, economy and institutions. Econ. J. 110(466), 918–938 (2000)CrossRef
go back to reference Gärling, T., Gamble, A.: Life satisfaction and emotional well-being: psychological, economic and social factors. In: Ranyard, R. (ed.) Economic psychology, p. 405–420. John Wiley & Sons, West Sussex, UK (2018) Gärling, T., Gamble, A.: Life satisfaction and emotional well-being: psychological, economic and social factors. In: Ranyard, R. (ed.) Economic psychology, p. 405–420. John Wiley & Sons, West Sussex, UK (2018)
go back to reference Greene, W.H.: Econometric Analysis, 8th edn. Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ (2018) Greene, W.H.: Econometric Analysis, 8th edn. Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ (2018)
go back to reference Hansen, J.L., Formby, J.P., Smith, W.J.: Estimating the income elasticity of demand for housing: A comparison of traditional and Lorenz-concentration curve methodologies. J. Hous. Econ. 7(4), 328–342 (1998)CrossRef Hansen, J.L., Formby, J.P., Smith, W.J.: Estimating the income elasticity of demand for housing: A comparison of traditional and Lorenz-concentration curve methodologies. J. Hous. Econ. 7(4), 328–342 (1998)CrossRef
go back to reference Hartley-Clark, L.: Investigating the Affective Core of SWB: the Circular Way. PhD thesis, Deakin University (2014) Hartley-Clark, L.: Investigating the Affective Core of SWB: the Circular Way. PhD thesis, Deakin University (2014)
go back to reference Heller, D., Watson, D., Ilies, R.: The dynamic process of life satisfaction. J. Pers. 74(5), 1421–1450 (2006)CrossRef Heller, D., Watson, D., Ilies, R.: The dynamic process of life satisfaction. J. Pers. 74(5), 1421–1450 (2006)CrossRef
go back to reference Helliwell, J.F., Putnam, R.D.: The social context of well-being. Philos. Trans. Royal Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 359(1449), 1435–1446 (2004)CrossRef Helliwell, J.F., Putnam, R.D.: The social context of well-being. Philos. Trans. Royal Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 359(1449), 1435–1446 (2004)CrossRef
go back to reference Hilbrecht, M., Smale, B., Mock, S.E.: Highway to health? Commute time and well-being among Canadian adults. World Leis. J. 56(2), 151–163 (2014)CrossRef Hilbrecht, M., Smale, B., Mock, S.E.: Highway to health? Commute time and well-being among Canadian adults. World Leis. J. 56(2), 151–163 (2014)CrossRef
go back to reference Huta, V., Ryan, R.M.: Pursuing pleasure or virtue: The differential and overlapping well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. J. Happiness Stud. 11(6), 735–762 (2010)CrossRef Huta, V., Ryan, R.M.: Pursuing pleasure or virtue: The differential and overlapping well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. J. Happiness Stud. 11(6), 735–762 (2010)CrossRef
go back to reference Ifcher, J., Zarghamee, H.: The happiness of single mothers: Evidence from the General Social Survey. J. Happiness Stud. 15(5), 1219–1238 (2014)CrossRef Ifcher, J., Zarghamee, H.: The happiness of single mothers: Evidence from the General Social Survey. J. Happiness Stud. 15(5), 1219–1238 (2014)CrossRef
go back to reference Jovanović, V.: The validity of the Satisfaction with Life Scale in adolescents and a comparison with single-item life satisfaction measures: a preliminary study. Qual. Life Res. 25(12), 3173–3180 (2016)CrossRef Jovanović, V.: The validity of the Satisfaction with Life Scale in adolescents and a comparison with single-item life satisfaction measures: a preliminary study. Qual. Life Res. 25(12), 3173–3180 (2016)CrossRef
go back to reference Kelley, J., Evans, M.: Societal inequality and individual subjective well-being: Results from 68 societies and over 200,000 individuals, 1981–2008. Soc. Sci. Res. 62, 1–23 (2017)CrossRef Kelley, J., Evans, M.: Societal inequality and individual subjective well-being: Results from 68 societies and over 200,000 individuals, 1981–2008. Soc. Sci. Res. 62, 1–23 (2017)CrossRef
go back to reference Kim, D.: Cross-national pattern of happiness: do higher education and less urbanization degrade happiness? Appl. Res. Qual. Life 13(1), 21–35 (2018)CrossRef Kim, D.: Cross-national pattern of happiness: do higher education and less urbanization degrade happiness? Appl. Res. Qual. Life 13(1), 21–35 (2018)CrossRef
go back to reference Levin, J.: Religion and happiness among Israeli Jews: findings from the ISSP religion III survey. J. Happiness Stud. 15(3), 593–611 (2014)CrossRef Levin, J.: Religion and happiness among Israeli Jews: findings from the ISSP religion III survey. J. Happiness Stud. 15(3), 593–611 (2014)CrossRef
go back to reference Lorenz, O.: Does commuting matter to subjective well-being? J. Transp. Geogr. 66, 180–199 (2018)CrossRef Lorenz, O.: Does commuting matter to subjective well-being? J. Transp. Geogr. 66, 180–199 (2018)CrossRef
go back to reference Lucas, R.E., Donnellan, M.B.: How stable is happiness? Using the STARTS model to estimate the stability of life satisfaction. J. Res. Pers. 41(5), 1091–1098 (2007)CrossRef Lucas, R.E., Donnellan, M.B.: How stable is happiness? Using the STARTS model to estimate the stability of life satisfaction. J. Res. Pers. 41(5), 1091–1098 (2007)CrossRef
go back to reference Marmot, M.: The influence of income on health: views of an epidemiologist. Health Aff. 21(2), 31–46 (2002)CrossRef Marmot, M.: The influence of income on health: views of an epidemiologist. Health Aff. 21(2), 31–46 (2002)CrossRef
go back to reference Martin, A., Goryakin, Y., Suhrcke, M.: Does active commuting improve psychological wellbeing? Longitudinal evidence from eighteen waves of the British Household Panel Survey. Prev. Med. 69, 296–303 (2014)CrossRef Martin, A., Goryakin, Y., Suhrcke, M.: Does active commuting improve psychological wellbeing? Longitudinal evidence from eighteen waves of the British Household Panel Survey. Prev. Med. 69, 296–303 (2014)CrossRef
go back to reference McCarthy, S., Habib, M.A.: Investigation of life satisfaction, travel, built environment, and attitudes. J. Transp. Health 11, 15–24 (2018)CrossRef McCarthy, S., Habib, M.A.: Investigation of life satisfaction, travel, built environment, and attitudes. J. Transp. Health 11, 15–24 (2018)CrossRef
go back to reference McLaughlin, K.A., Kubzansky, L.D., Dunn, E.C., Waldinger, R., Vaillant, G., Koenen, K.C.: Childhood social environment, emotional reactivity to stress, and mood and anxiety disorders across the life course. Depress. Anxiety 27(12), 1087–1094 (2010)CrossRef McLaughlin, K.A., Kubzansky, L.D., Dunn, E.C., Waldinger, R., Vaillant, G., Koenen, K.C.: Childhood social environment, emotional reactivity to stress, and mood and anxiety disorders across the life course. Depress. Anxiety 27(12), 1087–1094 (2010)CrossRef
go back to reference Mokhtarian, P.L.: Discrete choice models’ ρ2: a reintroduction to an old friend. J. Choice Modell. 21, 60–65 (2016)CrossRef Mokhtarian, P.L.: Discrete choice models’ ρ2: a reintroduction to an old friend. J. Choice Modell. 21, 60–65 (2016)CrossRef
go back to reference Mokhtarian, P.L.: Subjective well-being and travel: retrospect and prospect. Transportation 46(2), 493–513 (2019)CrossRef Mokhtarian, P.L.: Subjective well-being and travel: retrospect and prospect. Transportation 46(2), 493–513 (2019)CrossRef
go back to reference Mokhtarian, P.L., Salomon, I.: Modeling the choice of telecommuting: a case of the preferred impossible alternative. Environ. Plann. A 28(10), 1859–1876 (1996)CrossRef Mokhtarian, P.L., Salomon, I.: Modeling the choice of telecommuting: a case of the preferred impossible alternative. Environ. Plann. A 28(10), 1859–1876 (1996)CrossRef
go back to reference Morris, E.A., Guerra, E.: Mood and mode: does how we travel affect how we feel? Transportation 42(1), 25–43 (2015)CrossRef Morris, E.A., Guerra, E.: Mood and mode: does how we travel affect how we feel? Transportation 42(1), 25–43 (2015)CrossRef
go back to reference Morrison, P.S., Weckroth, M.: Human values, subjective well-being and the metropolitan region. Reg. Stud. 52(3), 325–337 (2018)CrossRef Morrison, P.S., Weckroth, M.: Human values, subjective well-being and the metropolitan region. Reg. Stud. 52(3), 325–337 (2018)CrossRef
go back to reference Morton, L.M., Cahill, J., Hartge, P.: Reporting participation in epidemiologic studies: a survey of practice. Am. J. Epidemiol. 163(3), 197–203 (2005)CrossRef Morton, L.M., Cahill, J., Hartge, P.: Reporting participation in epidemiologic studies: a survey of practice. Am. J. Epidemiol. 163(3), 197–203 (2005)CrossRef
go back to reference National Research Council: Nonresponse in Social Science Surveys: A Research Agenda. National Academies Press, Washington (2013) National Research Council: Nonresponse in Social Science Surveys: A Research Agenda. National Academies Press, Washington (2013)
go back to reference Nie, P., Sousa-Poza, A.: Commute time and subjective well-being in urban China. China Econ. Rev. 48, 188–204 (2016)CrossRef Nie, P., Sousa-Poza, A.: Commute time and subjective well-being in urban China. China Econ. Rev. 48, 188–204 (2016)CrossRef
go back to reference Nikolaev, B.: Living with mom and dad and loving it… or are you? J. Econ. Psychol. 51, 199–209 (2015)CrossRef Nikolaev, B.: Living with mom and dad and loving it… or are you? J. Econ. Psychol. 51, 199–209 (2015)CrossRef
go back to reference Nikolaev, B.: Does Higher Education Increase Hedonic and Eudaimonic Happiness? J. Happiness Stud. 19(2), 483–504 (2018) Nikolaev, B.: Does Higher Education Increase Hedonic and Eudaimonic Happiness? J. Happiness Stud. 19(2), 483–504 (2018)
go back to reference Ochsen, C.: Subjective well-being and aggregate unemployment: further evidence. Scott. J. Polit. Econ. 58(5), 634–655 (2011)CrossRef Ochsen, C.: Subjective well-being and aggregate unemployment: further evidence. Scott. J. Polit. Econ. 58(5), 634–655 (2011)CrossRef
go back to reference Oishi, S.: The concept of life satisfaction across cultures: an IRT analysis. J. Res. Pers. 40(4), 411–423 (2006)CrossRef Oishi, S.: The concept of life satisfaction across cultures: an IRT analysis. J. Res. Pers. 40(4), 411–423 (2006)CrossRef
go back to reference Olivos, F., Olivos-Jara, P., Browne, M.: Asymmetric social comparison and life satisfaction in social networks. J. Happiness Stud. 22, 363–384 (2020)CrossRef Olivos, F., Olivos-Jara, P., Browne, M.: Asymmetric social comparison and life satisfaction in social networks. J. Happiness Stud. 22, 363–384 (2020)CrossRef
go back to reference Oswald, A.J., Wu, S.: Objective confirmation of subjective measures of human well-being: Evidence from the USA. Science 327(5965), 576–579 (2010)CrossRef Oswald, A.J., Wu, S.: Objective confirmation of subjective measures of human well-being: Evidence from the USA. Science 327(5965), 576–579 (2010)CrossRef
go back to reference Pavot, W., Diener, E.: Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychol. Assess. 5(2), 164–172 (1993)CrossRef Pavot, W., Diener, E.: Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychol. Assess. 5(2), 164–172 (1993)CrossRef
go back to reference Pickett, K.E., Wilkinson, R.G.: Income inequality and health: a causal review. Soc. Sci. Med. 128, 316–326 (2015)CrossRef Pickett, K.E., Wilkinson, R.G.: Income inequality and health: a causal review. Soc. Sci. Med. 128, 316–326 (2015)CrossRef
go back to reference Requena, F.: Rural–urban living and level of economic development as factors in subjective well-being. Soc. Indic. Res. 128(2), 693–708 (2016)CrossRef Requena, F.: Rural–urban living and level of economic development as factors in subjective well-being. Soc. Indic. Res. 128(2), 693–708 (2016)CrossRef
go back to reference Rojas, M.: Life satisfaction and satisfaction in domains of life: Is it a simple relationship? J. Happiness Stud. 7(4), 467–497 (2006)CrossRef Rojas, M.: Life satisfaction and satisfaction in domains of life: Is it a simple relationship? J. Happiness Stud. 7(4), 467–497 (2006)CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenberg, S.D., Rosenberg, H.J., Farrell, M.P.: The midlife crisis revisited. In: Willis, S.L., Reid, J.D. (eds.) Life in the middle: psychological and social development in middle age. Academic Press, San Diego (1999) Rosenberg, S.D., Rosenberg, H.J., Farrell, M.P.: The midlife crisis revisited. In: Willis, S.L., Reid, J.D. (eds.) Life in the middle: psychological and social development in middle age. Academic Press, San Diego (1999)
go back to reference Ryan, R.M., Deci, E.L.: On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52(1), 141–166 (2001)CrossRef Ryan, R.M., Deci, E.L.: On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52(1), 141–166 (2001)CrossRef
go back to reference Sander, W.: Location and happiness in the United States. Econ. Lett. 112(3), 277–279 (2011)CrossRef Sander, W.: Location and happiness in the United States. Econ. Lett. 112(3), 277–279 (2011)CrossRef
go back to reference Sarracino, F.: Social capital and subjective well-being trends: comparing 11 western European countries. J. Socio-Econ. 39(4), 482–517 (2010)CrossRef Sarracino, F.: Social capital and subjective well-being trends: comparing 11 western European countries. J. Socio-Econ. 39(4), 482–517 (2010)CrossRef
go back to reference Shaw, F.A., Malokin, A., Mokhtarian, P.L., Circella, G.: It’s not all fun and games: an investigation of the reported benefits and disadvantages of conducting activities while commuting. Travel Behaviour and Society 17, 8–25 (2019)CrossRef Shaw, F.A., Malokin, A., Mokhtarian, P.L., Circella, G.: It’s not all fun and games: an investigation of the reported benefits and disadvantages of conducting activities while commuting. Travel Behaviour and Society 17, 8–25 (2019)CrossRef
go back to reference Shields, M., Wooden, M.: Marriage, children and subjective well-being. Presented at the 8th Australian Institute of Family Studies Conference. Melbourne, Australia (2003) Shields, M., Wooden, M.: Marriage, children and subjective well-being. Presented at the 8th Australian Institute of Family Studies Conference. Melbourne, Australia (2003)
go back to reference Shields, M.A., Price, S.W., Wooden, M.: Life satisfaction and the economic and social characteristics of neighbourhoods. J. Popul. Econ. 22(2), 421–443 (2009)CrossRef Shields, M.A., Price, S.W., Wooden, M.: Life satisfaction and the economic and social characteristics of neighbourhoods. J. Popul. Econ. 22(2), 421–443 (2009)CrossRef
go back to reference Smith, O.: Commute well-being differences by mode: evidence from Portland, Oregon, USA. J. Transp. Health 4, 246–254 (2017)CrossRef Smith, O.: Commute well-being differences by mode: evidence from Portland, Oregon, USA. J. Transp. Health 4, 246–254 (2017)CrossRef
go back to reference Soto, C.J.: Is happiness good for your personality? Concurrent and prospective relations of the big five with subjective well-being. J. Pers. 83(1), 45–55 (2015)CrossRef Soto, C.J.: Is happiness good for your personality? Concurrent and prospective relations of the big five with subjective well-being. J. Pers. 83(1), 45–55 (2015)CrossRef
go back to reference St-Louis, E., Manaugh, K., van Lierop, D., El-Geneidy, A.: The happy commuter: a comparison of commuter satisfaction across modes. Transp. Res. Part F 26, 160–170 (2014)CrossRef St-Louis, E., Manaugh, K., van Lierop, D., El-Geneidy, A.: The happy commuter: a comparison of commuter satisfaction across modes. Transp. Res. Part F 26, 160–170 (2014)CrossRef
go back to reference Stevenson, B., Wolfers, J.: Economic Growth and Subjective Well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin Paradox. CESifo Working Paper, No. 2394, Center for Economic Studies and Ifo Institute (CESifo), Munich (2008) Stevenson, B., Wolfers, J.: Economic Growth and Subjective Well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin Paradox. CESifo Working Paper, No. 2394, Center for Economic Studies and Ifo Institute (CESifo), Munich (2008)
go back to reference Stutzer, A., Frey, B.S.: Stress that doesn’t pay: the commuting paradox. Scand. J. Econ. 110(2), 339–366 (2008)CrossRef Stutzer, A., Frey, B.S.: Stress that doesn’t pay: the commuting paradox. Scand. J. Econ. 110(2), 339–366 (2008)CrossRef
go back to reference Sweet, M., Kanaroglou, P.: Gender differences: the role of travel and time use in subjective well-being. Transp. Res. Part F 40, 23–34 (2016)CrossRef Sweet, M., Kanaroglou, P.: Gender differences: the role of travel and time use in subjective well-being. Transp. Res. Part F 40, 23–34 (2016)CrossRef
go back to reference Szolnoki, G., Hoffmann, D.: Online, face-to-face and telephone surveys—comparing different sampling methods in wine consumer research. Wine Economics and Policy 2(2), 57–66 (2013)CrossRef Szolnoki, G., Hoffmann, D.: Online, face-to-face and telephone surveys—comparing different sampling methods in wine consumer research. Wine Economics and Policy 2(2), 57–66 (2013)CrossRef
go back to reference Waldinger, R.J., Vaillant, G.E., Orav, E.J.: Childhood sibling relationships as a predictor of major depression in adulthood: A 30-year prospective study. Am. J. Psychiatry 164(6), 949–954 (2007)CrossRef Waldinger, R.J., Vaillant, G.E., Orav, E.J.: Childhood sibling relationships as a predictor of major depression in adulthood: A 30-year prospective study. Am. J. Psychiatry 164(6), 949–954 (2007)CrossRef
go back to reference Welsch, H., Kuehling, J.: Divided We Stand: Immigration Attitudes, Identity, and Subjective Well-Being. Oldenburg Discussion Papers in Economics, No. V-401–17, University of Oldenburg, Department of Economics, Oldenburg. Welsch, H., Kuehling, J.: Divided We Stand: Immigration Attitudes, Identity, and Subjective Well-Being. Oldenburg Discussion Papers in Economics, No. V-401–17, University of Oldenburg, Department of Economics, Oldenburg.
go back to reference Wener, R.E., Evans, G.W.: Comparing stress of car and train commuters. Transp. Res. Part F 14(2), 111–116 (2011)CrossRef Wener, R.E., Evans, G.W.: Comparing stress of car and train commuters. Transp. Res. Part F 14(2), 111–116 (2011)CrossRef
go back to reference Williams, R.: Generalized ordered logit/partial proportional odds models for ordinal dependent variables. Stata Journal 6(1), 58 (2006)CrossRef Williams, R.: Generalized ordered logit/partial proportional odds models for ordinal dependent variables. Stata Journal 6(1), 58 (2006)CrossRef
go back to reference Williams, R.: Understanding and interpreting generalized ordered logit models. J. Math. Sociol. 40(1), 7–20 (2016)CrossRef Williams, R.: Understanding and interpreting generalized ordered logit models. J. Math. Sociol. 40(1), 7–20 (2016)CrossRef
go back to reference Witter, R.A., Okun, M.A., Stock, W.A., Haring, M.J.: Education and subjective well-being: a meta-analysis. Educ. Eval. Policy Anal. 6(2), 165–173 (1984)CrossRef Witter, R.A., Okun, M.A., Stock, W.A., Haring, M.J.: Education and subjective well-being: a meta-analysis. Educ. Eval. Policy Anal. 6(2), 165–173 (1984)CrossRef
go back to reference Yakovlev, P., Leguizamon, S.: Ignorance is not bliss: on the role of education in subjective well-being. J. Socio-Econ. 41(6), 806–815 (2012)CrossRef Yakovlev, P., Leguizamon, S.: Ignorance is not bliss: on the role of education in subjective well-being. J. Socio-Econ. 41(6), 806–815 (2012)CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Combining disparate surveys across time to study satisfaction with life: the effects of study context, sampling method, and transport attributes
Authors
Xinyi Wang
F. Atiyya Shaw
Patricia L. Mokhtarian
Giovanni Circella
Kari E. Watkins
Publication date
03-02-2022
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Transportation / Issue 2/2023
Print ISSN: 0049-4488
Electronic ISSN: 1572-9435
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11116-021-10252-x

Other articles of this Issue 2/2023

Transportation 2/2023 Go to the issue

Premium Partner