Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 4/2014

01-12-2014 | Original Paper

Emotional Transactions in the Paris Subway: Combining Naturalistic Videotaping, Objective Facial Coding and Sequential Analysis in the Study of Nonverbal Emotional Behavior

Authors: Martin Aranguren, Stéphane Tonnelat

Published in: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior | Issue 4/2014

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

We report on what, to our knowledge, represents the first study of nonverbal emotional behavior in crowded public places combining naturalistic videotaping of situated activity, objective coding of facial movement, and sequential analysis of behavior. In the first part of the study we argue that passengers do not lose emotional sensitivity to physical contact as density (passengers per square meter) increases, which indicates that physical contact is experienced as a territorial intrusion regardless of crowdedness. In the second part of the study, we suggest that passengers resolve the emotions due to intrusive physical contacts through two interactional strategies involving facial movements usually interpreted as “expressions of emotions.” Since proxemic violations seem to represent a pervasive emotion elicitor, the protocol can be extended to other means of transportation and replicated in other locations. We conclude that the methodology provides an effective tool for theory-building in the study of nonverbal emotional behavior.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
Pragmatist philosopher C. S. Pierce called them “abductive”. See Fann (1970) for an account of Peirce’s thoughts about abduction.
 
2
According to data released by the RATP, traffic increased over 25 % between 2000 and 2011, reaching a high yearly number of 1,500 million travels.
 
4
Situations described as “unpleasant” were identified by searching instances of the following adjectives (usually employed ironically): agréable, plaisant, jouissif. Situations described as “annoying” were spotted through uses of the reflexive verb: s’énerver.
 
5
Only seven messages describe the situation as “unpleasant” or “annoying” and at the same time make reference to retaliatory response.
 
6
Each videotaping session lasted for about 90 min. The duration was limited by the battery life of the camera.
 
7
Goffman calls such expressions “body gloss” because they provide additional information about the relationship that the offender has to the norms. The body gloss, by conveying to the others present that the offender herself sees her conduct as offensive, makes the transgression more excusable or less severe than if the offender proceeded as nothing had happened.
 
8
In this study the probability of a touch in a dyad was defined as the quotient of the number of times that at least one touch was seen during an observation period divided by the number of observation periods, a probability that turned out to be around 15 % (Hall and Veccia 1990, p. 1157).
 
9
That is, we did not count all observable contacts and then calculated the ratio of contacts followed by facial responses. Neither did we consider facial changes that were not preceded by observable physical contacts.
 
10
In mathematics, a bijection or one-to-one correspondence is a function giving an exact pairing of the elements of two sets. By analogy, a bijective contact incident involves one author of the contact and only one receiver. A non bijective incident, in contrast, involves one author and two or more receivers.
 
11
In the probabilistic analysis of rider exchange units, as we calculated density and searched for occurrences of at least one contact incident, we made side notes on some interactional events, including some forms of conspicuous courtesy or hostility between strangers.
 
12
For example, in contrast to the US, where traffic lights are sometimes equipped with a countdown, in France traffic lights for pedestrians only alternate between red and green. Without knowing at what stage of the countdown the red light may come, pedestrians sometimes attempt to cross the road anyway. If they are unfortunately caught by a green light for the cars in the middle of the road, they often signal their acknowledgement of the transgression to the impatient drivers by an exaggerated trot accompanied by ostensible expressions of surprise. The purely ritual meaning of the trot is revealed by occasions in which no apparent pace acceleration ensues, as compared to just walking.
 
13
The two streams were synchronized with the program Elan (Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, n.d.) using for each pair of files, as a common point of reference, the moment when the doors close.
 
14
Keltner’s prototype includes, in order or appearance: gaze down, smile control (e.g., lip press), smile, head away, gaze shifts.
 
15
An event using one of these ad hoc categories is, for example, “Patient looks Author”. In all the cases in which the code looks was attributed to the patient, we based the attribution on 1) a previously attributed head and eye movement descriptor as defined by the FACS and 2) a reasonable guess about what the patient was looking at. If we thought that patient was looking at author, but no head and eye movement could be coded, the look itself was not coded.
 
16
In order to specify who was the patient (the person receiving the contact) and who was the author of the contact, we used the codes “cp” and “ca”. If the individual taken as the point of reference was the patient, the contact event was coded “cp”; if she was the author, it was coded “ca”. For the sake of simplicity, in what follows we use the expression “physical contact”, but the type of event to which reference is made is more precisely “cp”.
 
17
For these t-patterns the program Theme® was set at significance level 0.0001.
 
18
Combining systematic coding and perception of behavioral configurations (or wholes) is one of the usual descriptive procedures of classical ethology (Lorenz 2003).
 
19
Some researchers claim that the asymmetric AU14 is the universal contempt expression (Ekman and Friesen 1986; Ekman and Heider 1988), but the symmetric and asymmetric appearances of AU14 occupy the same sequential slots in the behavior patterns that we found. Hence, there is nothing in these patterns that could indicate a different meaning for each appearance of AU14.
 
20
In order to constrain the inevitable arbitrariness of time window duration, we took the duration of the longest t-pattern as the point of reference for this backward-looking time window.
 
21
We refrained from using a more stringent sequential association analysis because we were less interested in establishing causal links than in finding descriptive similarities between p3 and p4.
 
Literature
go back to reference Arnold, M. (1960). Emotion and personality (Vol. 1–2). New York: Columbia University Press. Arnold, M. (1960). Emotion and personality (Vol. 1–2). New York: Columbia University Press.
go back to reference Bateson, G., & Mead, M. (1942). Balinese character: A photographic essay. New York: New York Academy of Sciences. Bateson, G., & Mead, M. (1942). Balinese character: A photographic essay. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
go back to reference Camras, L. A. (1992). Expressive development and basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3–4), 269–283.CrossRef Camras, L. A. (1992). Expressive development and basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3–4), 269–283.CrossRef
go back to reference Chong, S. C. F., Werker, J. F., Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (2003). Three facial expressions mothers direct to their infants. Infant and Child Development, 12(3), 211–232.CrossRef Chong, S. C. F., Werker, J. F., Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (2003). Three facial expressions mothers direct to their infants. Infant and Child Development, 12(3), 211–232.CrossRef
go back to reference Craig, K. D., Hyde, S. A., & Patrick, C. J. (1991). Genuine, suppressed and faked facial behavior during exacerbation of chronic low back pain. Pain, 46(2), 161–171.PubMedCrossRef Craig, K. D., Hyde, S. A., & Patrick, C. J. (1991). Genuine, suppressed and faked facial behavior during exacerbation of chronic low back pain. Pain, 46(2), 161–171.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.
go back to reference Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1986). A new pan-cultural facial expression of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 10(2), 159–168.CrossRef Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1986). A new pan-cultural facial expression of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 10(2), 159–168.CrossRef
go back to reference Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Simons, R. C. (1985). Is the startle reaction an emotion? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(5), 1416.PubMedCrossRef Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Simons, R. C. (1985). Is the startle reaction an emotion? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(5), 1416.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ekman, P., & Heider, K. G. (1988). The universality of a contempt expression: A replication. Motivation and Emotion, 12(3), 303–308.CrossRef Ekman, P., & Heider, K. G. (1988). The universality of a contempt expression: A replication. Motivation and Emotion, 12(3), 303–308.CrossRef
go back to reference Evans, G. W., & Wener, R. E. (2007). Crowding and personal space invasion on the train: Please don’t make me sit in the middle. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27, 90–94.CrossRef Evans, G. W., & Wener, R. E. (2007). Crowding and personal space invasion on the train: Please don’t make me sit in the middle. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27, 90–94.CrossRef
go back to reference Fann, K. T. (1970). Peirce’s theory of abduction. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.CrossRef Fann, K. T. (1970). Peirce’s theory of abduction. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.CrossRef
go back to reference Fernández-Dols, J.-M., Carrera, P., & Crivelli, C. (2011). Facial behavior while experiencing sexual excitement. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35(1), 63–71.CrossRef Fernández-Dols, J.-M., Carrera, P., & Crivelli, C. (2011). Facial behavior while experiencing sexual excitement. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35(1), 63–71.CrossRef
go back to reference Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Ruiz-Belda, M.-A. (1995). Are smiles a sign of happiness? Gold medal winners at the Olympic Games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1113.CrossRef Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Ruiz-Belda, M.-A. (1995). Are smiles a sign of happiness? Gold medal winners at the Olympic Games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1113.CrossRef
go back to reference Fried, M. L., & DeFazio, V. J. (1974). Territoriality and boundary conflicts in the subway. Psychiatry, 37, 47–59.PubMed Fried, M. L., & DeFazio, V. J. (1974). Territoriality and boundary conflicts in the subway. Psychiatry, 37, 47–59.PubMed
go back to reference Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Paris: Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’Homme. Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Paris: Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’Homme.
go back to reference Garfinkel, H. (1956). Conditions of successful degradation ceremonies. American Journal of Sociology, 61(5), 420–424. Garfinkel, H. (1956). Conditions of successful degradation ceremonies. American Journal of Sociology, 61(5), 420–424.
go back to reference Goffman, E. (1966). Behavior in public places: Notes on the social organization of gatherings. Simon and Schuster. Goffman, E. (1966). Behavior in public places: Notes on the social organization of gatherings. Simon and Schuster.
go back to reference Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays on face-to-face interaction. New York: Anchor Books. Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays on face-to-face interaction. New York: Anchor Books.
go back to reference Goffman, E. (1971). Relations in public: Microstudies of the social order. New York: Basic Books. Goffman, E. (1971). Relations in public: Microstudies of the social order. New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Griffiths, P. E., & Scarantino, A. (2005). Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotion. Cambridge University Press: In The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Griffiths, P. E., & Scarantino, A. (2005). Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotion. Cambridge University Press: In The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition.
go back to reference Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. New York: Doubleday. Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. New York: Doubleday.
go back to reference Hall, J. A., & Veccia, E. M. (1990). More“touching” observations: New insights on men, women, and interpersonal touch. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 1155.CrossRef Hall, J. A., & Veccia, E. M. (1990). More“touching” observations: New insights on men, women, and interpersonal touch. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 1155.CrossRef
go back to reference Hayduk, Leslie A. (1981). The Permeability of Personal Space. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement, 13(3), 274.CrossRef Hayduk, Leslie A. (1981). The Permeability of Personal Space. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement, 13(3), 274.CrossRef
go back to reference Hinde, R. A. (1985). Was “the expression of the emotions” a misleading phrase? Animal Behaviour, 33(3), 985–992.CrossRef Hinde, R. A. (1985). Was “the expression of the emotions” a misleading phrase? Animal Behaviour, 33(3), 985–992.CrossRef
go back to reference Izard, C. E., & Haynes, O. M. (1988). On the form and universality of the contempt expression: A challenge to Ekman and Friesen’s claim of discovery. Motivation and Emotion, 12(1), 1–16.CrossRef Izard, C. E., & Haynes, O. M. (1988). On the form and universality of the contempt expression: A challenge to Ekman and Friesen’s claim of discovery. Motivation and Emotion, 12(1), 1–16.CrossRef
go back to reference Judge, P. G., & De Waal, F. (1997). Rhesus monkey behaviour under diverse population densities: Coping with long-term crowding. Animal Behaviour, 54(3), 643–662.PubMedCrossRef Judge, P. G., & De Waal, F. (1997). Rhesus monkey behaviour under diverse population densities: Coping with long-term crowding. Animal Behaviour, 54(3), 643–662.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Katz, J. (1999). How emotions work. Chicago and London: The University of Chicago Press. Katz, J. (1999). How emotions work. Chicago and London: The University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Keltner, D. (1995). Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(3), 441.CrossRef Keltner, D. (1995). Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(3), 441.CrossRef
go back to reference Kendon, A. (1990). Conducting interaction: Patterns of behavior in focused interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kendon, A. (1990). Conducting interaction: Patterns of behavior in focused interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: University Press. Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: University Press.
go back to reference Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Levine, J., Vinson, A., & Wood, D. (1973). Subway behavior. In People in places, the sociology of the familiar. New York, NY: Praeger Publishers. Levine, J., Vinson, A., & Wood, D. (1973). Subway behavior. In People in places, the sociology of the familiar. New York, NY: Praeger Publishers.
go back to reference Lorenz, K. (2003). The foundations of ethology. New York: Springer. Lorenz, K. (2003). The foundations of ethology. New York: Springer.
go back to reference Magnusson, M. S. (2000). Discovering hidden time patterns in behavior: T-patterns and their detection. Behavior Research Methods, 32(1), 93–110.CrossRef Magnusson, M. S. (2000). Discovering hidden time patterns in behavior: T-patterns and their detection. Behavior Research Methods, 32(1), 93–110.CrossRef
go back to reference Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. (n.d.). Elan—Linguistic Annotator. Nijmegen. Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. (n.d.). Elan—Linguistic Annotator. Nijmegen.
go back to reference Mehu, M., & Dunbar, R. I. (2008). Naturalistic observations of smiling and laughter in human group interactions. Behaviour, 145(12), 1747–1780.CrossRef Mehu, M., & Dunbar, R. I. (2008). Naturalistic observations of smiling and laughter in human group interactions. Behaviour, 145(12), 1747–1780.CrossRef
go back to reference Messinger, D. S., Fogel, A., & Dickson, K. L. (2001). All smiles are positive, but some smiles are more positive than others. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 642.PubMedCrossRef Messinger, D. S., Fogel, A., & Dickson, K. L. (2001). All smiles are positive, but some smiles are more positive than others. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 642.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Newman, Robert C., & Pollack, Donald. (1973). Proxemics in Deviant Adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 40(1), 6.PubMedCrossRef Newman, Robert C., & Pollack, Donald. (1973). Proxemics in Deviant Adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 40(1), 6.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Noldus Information Technology. (n.d.). Theme® version 5. Wageningen. Noldus Information Technology. (n.d.). Theme® version 5. Wageningen.
go back to reference Parkinson, B. (2001). Anger on and off the road. British Journal of Psychology, 92(3), 507–526.CrossRef Parkinson, B. (2001). Anger on and off the road. British Journal of Psychology, 92(3), 507–526.CrossRef
go back to reference Prkachin, K. M. (1992). The consistency of facial expressions of pain: A comparison across modalities. Pain, 51(3), 297–306.PubMedCrossRef Prkachin, K. M. (1992). The consistency of facial expressions of pain: A comparison across modalities. Pain, 51(3), 297–306.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rosenstein, D., & Oster, H. (1988). Differential facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns. Child Development, 59(6), 1555–1568. Rosenstein, D., & Oster, H. (1988). Differential facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns. Child Development, 59(6), 1555–1568.
go back to reference Ruiz-Belda, M.-A., Fernández-Dols, J.-M., Carrera, P., & Barchard, K. (2003). Spontaneous facial expressions of happy bowlers and soccer fans. Cognition and Emotion, 17(2), 315–326.CrossRef Ruiz-Belda, M.-A., Fernández-Dols, J.-M., Carrera, P., & Barchard, K. (2003). Spontaneous facial expressions of happy bowlers and soccer fans. Cognition and Emotion, 17(2), 315–326.CrossRef
go back to reference Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50(4), 696–735. Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50(4), 696–735.
go back to reference Scherer, K. (1987). Toward a dynamic theory of emotion: The component process model of affective states. Geneva Studies in Emotion and Communication. Geneva: University of Geneva. Scherer, K. (1987). Toward a dynamic theory of emotion: The component process model of affective states. Geneva Studies in Emotion and Communication. Geneva: University of Geneva.
go back to reference Scherer, K. R., & Ceschi, G. (2000). Criteria for emotion recognition from verbal and nonverbal expression: Studying baggage loss in the airport. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(3), 327–339.CrossRef Scherer, K. R., & Ceschi, G. (2000). Criteria for emotion recognition from verbal and nonverbal expression: Studying baggage loss in the airport. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(3), 327–339.CrossRef
go back to reference Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. New York: Oxford University Press. Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Tonnelat, S. (2012). Confiance et émotions dans le métro de New York [Trust and emotions in the New York subway]. In E. Le Mouël & C. Espinasse (Eds.), Des lieux qui créent des liens [Places that create ties]. Paris: L’Harmattan. Tonnelat, S. (2012). Confiance et émotions dans le métro de New York [Trust and emotions in the New York subway]. In E. Le Mouël & C. Espinasse (Eds.), Des lieux qui créent des liens [Places that create ties]. Paris: L’Harmattan.
go back to reference Transit Cooperative Research Program. (2003). Transit capacity and quality of service manual (2nd ed). Transportation Research Board. Transit Cooperative Research Program. (2003). Transit capacity and quality of service manual (2nd ed). Transportation Research Board.
Metadata
Title
Emotional Transactions in the Paris Subway: Combining Naturalistic Videotaping, Objective Facial Coding and Sequential Analysis in the Study of Nonverbal Emotional Behavior
Authors
Martin Aranguren
Stéphane Tonnelat
Publication date
01-12-2014
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior / Issue 4/2014
Print ISSN: 0191-5886
Electronic ISSN: 1573-3653
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-014-0193-1

Other articles of this Issue 4/2014

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 4/2014 Go to the issue