Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 2/2021

10-02-2021 | Original Paper

Parental Speech and Gesture Input to Girls Versus Boys in Singletons and Twins

Authors: Ebru Pınar, Sumeyra Ozturk, F. Nihan Ketrez, Şeyda Özçalışkan

Published in: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior | Issue 2/2021

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Children show sex differences in early speech development, with girls producing a greater number and variety of words at an earlier age than boys (Berglund et al. in Scand J Psychol 46(6): 485–491, 2005)—a pattern that also becomes evident in gesture (Butterworth and Morisette in J Reprod Infant Psychol 14(3): 219–231, 1996). Importantly, parents show variability in how they produce speech when interacting with their singleton sons vs. daughters (i.e., Cherry and Lewis in Dev Psychol 12: 278–282, 1976; Leaper et al. in Dev Psychol 34: 3–27, 1998). However, it is unknown whether the variability in speech input extends to different twin dyads or becomes evident in gesture input. In this study, we examined parental gesture and speech input to 35 singleton (19 boys, 16 girls) and 62 twin (10 boy–boy, 9 girl–girl, and 12 girl–boy dyads) Turkish children (age range = 0;10–3;4) in parent–child interactions. We asked whether there is evidence of sex (girls vs. boys) or group (singletons vs. twins) differences in parents’ speech and gesture production, and whether these differences also become evident in different twin dyads (girl–girl, boy–boy, girl–boy). Our results, based on parent-child interactions, largely showed no evidence of sex or dyad-composition difference in either parent speech or gesture, but evidence of a group difference in gesture, with the parents of singletons providing a greater amount, diversity, and complexity of gestures than parents of twins in their interactions. These results suggest that differences in parent input to singletons vs. twins might become evident initially in gesture.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
In the recordings where both mother and father were present, they took turns while interacting with their children; we therefore included input across the two parents. The co-presence of mother and father during the interaction was equally distributed across all 5 groups, with n = 1 per group.
 
2
The age of the grandmother was not recorded.
 
3
The mean length of interaction varied by group, Msingletons = 10.06 (SD = 4.31) vs. Mtwins = 14.32 (SD = 4.65), F(1, 64) = 14.95, p < .001, but not by sex, Mgirls = 12.40 (SD = 5.65) vs. Mboys = 10.97 (SD = 4.31), F(1, 52) = 1.12, p = .29, or by twin dyad type, Mtwin boy–boy = 13.40 (SD = 3.41), Mtwin girl–girl = 15.78 (SD = 6.16), Mtwin boy- girl = 14.00 (SD = 4.39), F(2, 28) = 0.65, p = .53.
 
4
For the 5 parent-child interactions where both parents were present, we tabulated speech production across the two parents.
 
5
For the 5 parent-child interactions where both parents were present, we tabulated gesture production across the two parents.
 
6
We conducted all analyses first with the whole sample, and then with a reduced sample by excluding the 5 parent-child interactions (n = 1/group) in which both parents were present. The pattern of results remained identical; we therefore only reported the results based on the whole sample.
 
Literature
go back to reference Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1985). Symbolic gesturing in language development. Human Development, 28, 40–49.CrossRef Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1985). Symbolic gesturing in language development. Human Development, 28, 40–49.CrossRef
go back to reference Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1989). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59, 450–466.CrossRef Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1989). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59, 450–466.CrossRef
go back to reference Aldrich, N. J., Brooks, P. J., Yuksel-Sokmen, P. O., Ragir, S., Flory, M. J., Lennon, E. M., Karmel, B. Z., & Gardner, J. M. (2015). Infant twins’ social interactions with caregivers and same-age siblings. Infant Behavior and Development, 41, 127–141.PubMedCrossRef Aldrich, N. J., Brooks, P. J., Yuksel-Sokmen, P. O., Ragir, S., Flory, M. J., Lennon, E. M., Karmel, B. Z., & Gardner, J. M. (2015). Infant twins’ social interactions with caregivers and same-age siblings. Infant Behavior and Development, 41, 127–141.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L. (2015). The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42, 239–273.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L. (2015). The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42, 239–273.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Aznar, A., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2013). Spanish parents’ emotion talk and their children’s understanding of emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 670.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Aznar, A., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2013). Spanish parents’ emotion talk and their children’s understanding of emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 670.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1986). Infants’ conventionalized acts: Gestures and words with mothers and peers. Infant Behavior and Development, 9(2), 215–230.CrossRef Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1986). Infants’ conventionalized acts: Gestures and words with mothers and peers. Infant Behavior and Development, 9(2), 215–230.CrossRef
go back to reference Bates, E. (1976). Language and context. New York: Academic Press. Bates, E. (1976). Language and context. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Bates, E., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). Cognition and communication from nine to thirteen months: Correlation findings. The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in iIfancy, 33–68. Bates, E., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). Cognition and communication from nine to thirteen months: Correlation findings. The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in iIfancy, 33–68.
go back to reference Baumann, S., Özçalışkan, Ş, & Adamson, L. B. (2019). Do early school-aged children’s gestures reflect parental gesture input in autism and typical development? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 67. Baumann, S., Özçalışkan, Ş, & Adamson, L. B. (2019). Do early school-aged children’s gestures reflect parental gesture input in autism and typical development? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 67.
go back to reference Bekken, K. (1989). Is there motherese in gesture? [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago. Bekken, K. (1989). Is there motherese in gesture? [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago.
go back to reference Berglund, E. V. A., Eriksson, M., & Westerlund, M. (2005). Communicative skills in relation to gender, birth order, childcare and socioeconomic status in 18-month‐old children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46(6), 485–491.PubMedCrossRef Berglund, E. V. A., Eriksson, M., & Westerlund, M. (2005). Communicative skills in relation to gender, birth order, childcare and socioeconomic status in 18-month‐old children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46(6), 485–491.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 207–220.PubMedCrossRef Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 207–220.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Butcher, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 235–258). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Butcher, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 235–258). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Butler, S., McMahon, C., & Ungerer, J. A. (2003). Maternal speech style with prelinguistic twin infants. Infant and Child Development, 12(2), 129–143.CrossRef Butler, S., McMahon, C., & Ungerer, J. A. (2003). Maternal speech style with prelinguistic twin infants. Infant and Child Development, 12(2), 129–143.CrossRef
go back to reference Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 9–33). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 9–33). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Butterworth, G., & Morisette, P. (1996). Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 14(3), 219–231.CrossRef Butterworth, G., & Morisette, P. (1996). Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 14(3), 219–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Cherry, L., & Lewis, M. (1976). Mothers and two-year-olds: A study of sex differentiated aspects of verbal interaction. Developmental Psychology, 12, 278–282.CrossRef Cherry, L., & Lewis, M. (1976). Mothers and two-year-olds: A study of sex differentiated aspects of verbal interaction. Developmental Psychology, 12, 278–282.CrossRef
go back to reference Clark, P. M., & Dickman, Z. (1984). Features of interaction of infant twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 33, 165–171.PubMedCrossRef Clark, P. M., & Dickman, Z. (1984). Features of interaction of infant twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 33, 165–171.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. M., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 10(4), 352–366.CrossRef Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. M., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 10(4), 352–366.CrossRef
go back to reference Conway, D., Lytton, H., & Pysh, F. (1980). Twin—singleton language differences. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 12, 264–271.CrossRef Conway, D., Lytton, H., & Pysh, F. (1980). Twin—singleton language differences. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 12, 264–271.CrossRef
go back to reference Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berkman, S., & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 477–488.CrossRef Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berkman, S., & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 477–488.CrossRef
go back to reference Davis, E. A. (1937). The development of linguistic skills in twins, singletons with siblings, and only children from age five to ten years. University of Minnesota Child Welfare Monograph Series, 14, 174. Davis, E. A. (1937). The development of linguistic skills in twins, singletons with siblings, and only children from age five to ten years. University of Minnesota Child Welfare Monograph Series, 14, 174.
go back to reference Day, E. (1932). The development of language in twins. Child Development, 3, 298–316. Day, E. (1932). The development of language in twins. Child Development, 3, 298–316.
go back to reference Dimitrova, N., Özçalışkan, Ş, & Adamson, L. B. (2017). Do verbal children with autism comprehend gesture as readily as typically developing children? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47, 3267–3280.PubMedCrossRef Dimitrova, N., Özçalışkan, Ş, & Adamson, L. B. (2017). Do verbal children with autism comprehend gesture as readily as typically developing children? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47, 3267–3280.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eriksson, M., & Berglund, E. (1999). Swedish early communicative development. First Language, 19, 55–90.CrossRef Eriksson, M., & Berglund, E. (1999). Swedish early communicative development. First Language, 19, 55–90.CrossRef
go back to reference Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., & Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 326–343.CrossRef Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., & Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 326–343.CrossRef
go back to reference Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1–173.PubMedCrossRef Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1–173.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short form versions of the MacArthur communicative development inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(1), 95–116.CrossRef Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short form versions of the MacArthur communicative development inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(1), 95–116.CrossRef
go back to reference Furrow, D., Nelson, K., & Benedict, H. (1979). Mothers’ speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships. Journal of Child Language, 6, 423–442.PubMedCrossRef Furrow, D., Nelson, K., & Benedict, H. (1979). Mothers’ speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships. Journal of Child Language, 6, 423–442.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Galsworthy, M. J., Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science, 3(2), 206–215.CrossRef Galsworthy, M. J., Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science, 3(2), 206–215.CrossRef
go back to reference Garitte, C., Almodovar, J. P., Benjamin, E., & Canhao, C. (2002). Speech in same-and different- sex twins 4 and 5 years old. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 5(6), 538–543.CrossRef Garitte, C., Almodovar, J. P., Benjamin, E., & Canhao, C. (2002). Speech in same-and different- sex twins 4 and 5 years old. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 5(6), 538–543.CrossRef
go back to reference Gleitman, L. R., Newport, E. L., & Gleitman, H. (1984). The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 11(1), 43–79.PubMedCrossRef Gleitman, L. R., Newport, E. L., & Gleitman, H. (1984). The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 11(1), 43–79.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Greenfield, P., & Smith, J. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press. Greenfield, P., & Smith, J. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Gücüyener, K., Arhan, E., Soysal, A. S., Ergenekon, E., Turan, O., Onal, E., Koç, Turkyılmaz, C., & Atalay, Y. (2011). Language assessment of non-handicapped twins at 5 years of age. Pediatrics International, 53(6), 944–949.PubMedCrossRef Gücüyener, K., Arhan, E., Soysal, A. S., Ergenekon, E., Turan, O., Onal, E., Koç, Turkyılmaz, C., & Atalay, Y. (2011). Language assessment of non-handicapped twins at 5 years of age. Pediatrics International, 53(6), 944–949.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.
go back to reference Hay, D., Prior, M., Collett, S., & Williams, M. (1987). Speech and language development in pre- school twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae Gemellologiae, 36, 239–248.CrossRef Hay, D., Prior, M., Collett, S., & Williams, M. (1987). Speech and language development in pre- school twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae Gemellologiae, 36, 239–248.CrossRef
go back to reference Hodges, L., & Özçalışkan, Ş, & Williamson, R. (2018). Type of iconicity influences children’s comprehension of gesture. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 327–339.PubMedCrossRef Hodges, L., & Özçalışkan, Ş, & Williamson, R. (2018). Type of iconicity influences children’s comprehension of gesture. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 327–339.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88.CrossRef Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88.CrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236–248.CrossRef Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236–248.CrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language Input and Child Syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45(3), 337–374.PubMedCrossRef Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language Input and Child Syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45(3), 337–374.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H. R., Vevea, J. L., & Hedges, L. V. (2007). The varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1062–1083.PubMedCrossRef Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H. R., Vevea, J. L., & Hedges, L. V. (2007). The varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1062–1083.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69. Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69.
go back to reference Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 368–371. Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 368–371.
go back to reference Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, 23–43.CrossRef Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, 23–43.CrossRef
go back to reference Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E., & Caselli, M. C. (1999). Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cognitive Development, 14(1), 57–75.CrossRef Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E., & Caselli, M. C. (1999). Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cognitive Development, 14(1), 57–75.CrossRef
go back to reference Katz, J., Önen, F., Demir, N., Uzlukaya, A., & Uludağ, P. (1974). A Turkish Peabody picture- vocabulary test. Hacettepe Bulletin of Social Sciences & Humanities, 6(1–2), 129–142. Katz, J., Önen, F., Demir, N., Uzlukaya, A., & Uludağ, P. (1974). A Turkish Peabody picture- vocabulary test. Hacettepe Bulletin of Social Sciences & Humanities, 6(1–2), 129–142.
go back to reference Ketrez, F. N. (2014). İkizlerde dil edinimi. Unpublished research report submitted to TUBITAK. Ketrez, F. N. (2014). İkizlerde dil edinimi. Unpublished research report submitted to TUBITAK.
go back to reference Ketrez, F. N. (2016). Different paces (but not different paths) in language acquisition. In M. Güven, D. Akar, B. Öztürk & M. Kelepir (eds). Exploring the Turkish linguistic landscape: Essays in honor of Eser Erguvanlı-Taylan (pp. 183–202). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Ketrez, F. N. (2016). Different paces (but not different paths) in language acquisition. In M. Güven, D. Akar, B. Öztürk & M. Kelepir (eds). Exploring the Turkish linguistic landscape: Essays in honor of Eser Erguvanlı-Taylan (pp. 183–202). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
go back to reference Ketrez, F. N. (2017). Sibling influence on morphological development? In Ketrez, F. Nihan, Aylin Küntay, Şeyda Özçalışkan, & Aslı Özyürek (Eds.) Social environment and cognition in language development: Studies in honor of Ayhan Aksu-Koç. Trends in Language Acquisition Research 21, 99–110. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Ketrez, F. N. (2017). Sibling influence on morphological development? In Ketrez, F. Nihan, Aylin Küntay, Şeyda Özçalışkan, & Aslı Özyürek (Eds.) Social environment and cognition in language development: Studies in honor of Ayhan Aksu-Koç. Trends in Language Acquisition Research 21, 99–110. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
go back to reference Ketrez, F. N., & Aksu-Koç, A. (2020). Turkish lexicon for morphosyntactic coding. Manuscript in preparation. Ketrez, F. N., & Aksu-Koç, A. (2020). Turkish lexicon for morphosyntactic coding. Manuscript in preparation.
go back to reference Kimura, D. (1998). Sex and cognition. Cambridge: The MIT Press. Kimura, D. (1998). Sex and cognition. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Laflamme, D., Pomerleau, A., & Malcuit, G. (2002). A comparison of fathers’ and mothers’ involvement in childcare and stimulation behaviors during free-play with their infants at 9 and 15 months. Sex Roles, 47, 507–518.CrossRef Laflamme, D., Pomerleau, A., & Malcuit, G. (2002). A comparison of fathers’ and mothers’ involvement in childcare and stimulation behaviors during free-play with their infants at 9 and 15 months. Sex Roles, 47, 507–518.CrossRef
go back to reference Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34, 3–27.PubMedCrossRef Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34, 3–27.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lillard, A. S. (1993). Pretend play skills and the child’s theory of mind. Child Development, 64(2), 348–371.CrossRef Lillard, A. S. (1993). Pretend play skills and the child’s theory of mind. Child Development, 64(2), 348–371.CrossRef
go back to reference Liu, H. M. (2014). Lexical and acoustic features of maternal utterances addressing preverbal infants in picture book reading link to 5-year-old children’s language development. Early Education and Development, 25, 1103–1117.CrossRef Liu, H. M. (2014). Lexical and acoustic features of maternal utterances addressing preverbal infants in picture book reading link to 5-year-old children’s language development. Early Education and Development, 25, 1103–1117.CrossRef
go back to reference Lock, A. E. (Ed.). (1978). Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language. New York: Academic Press. Lock, A. E. (Ed.). (1978). Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Lytton, H., Conway, D., & Suave, R. (1977). The impact of twinship on parent-child interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(2), 97–107. Lytton, H., Conway, D., & Suave, R. (1977). The impact of twinship on parent-child interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(2), 97–107.
go back to reference Maccoby, E. (1966). The development of sex differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Maccoby, E. (1966). The development of sex differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
go back to reference MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Mascaro, J. S., Rentscher, K. E., Hackett, P. D., Mehl, M. R., & Rilling, J. K. (2017). Child gender influences paternal behavior, language, and brain function. Behavioral Neuroscience, 131(3), 262–273.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mascaro, J. S., Rentscher, K. E., Hackett, P. D., Mehl, M. R., & Rilling, J. K. (2017). Child gender influences paternal behavior, language, and brain function. Behavioral Neuroscience, 131(3), 262–273.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Masur, E. F. (1983). Gestural development, dual-directional signaling, and the transition to words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12, 93–109.CrossRef Masur, E. F. (1983). Gestural development, dual-directional signaling, and the transition to words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12, 93–109.CrossRef
go back to reference Namy, L. L., Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (2000). Verbal labels and gestural routines in parental communication with young children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 63–80.CrossRef Namy, L. L., Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (2000). Verbal labels and gestural routines in parental communication with young children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 63–80.CrossRef
go back to reference Namy, L., Vallas, R., & Knight-Schwarz, J. (2008). Linking parent input and child receptivity to symbolic gestures. Gesture, 8, 302–324.CrossRef Namy, L., Vallas, R., & Knight-Schwarz, J. (2008). Linking parent input and child receptivity to symbolic gestures. Gesture, 8, 302–324.CrossRef
go back to reference Ninio, A. (1983). A pragmatic approach to early language acquisition. Paper presented at: The Study Group on Crosscultural and Crosslinguistic Aspects of Native Language Acquisition. Jerusalem, Israel: Institute for Advanced Studies, Hebrew University. Ninio, A. (1983). A pragmatic approach to early language acquisition. Paper presented at: The Study Group on Crosscultural and Crosslinguistic Aspects of Native Language Acquisition. Jerusalem, Israel: Institute for Advanced Studies, Hebrew University.
go back to reference Ninio, A. (1984). Functions of speech in mother–infant interaction. Israel: Final science report to the U.S.-Israel Binational Science Foundation. Jerusalem. Ninio, A. (1984). Functions of speech in mother–infant interaction. Israel: Final science report to the U.S.-Israel Binational Science Foundation. Jerusalem.
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, Adamson, L. B., Dimitrova, N., & Baumann, S. (2017). Early gesture provides a helping hand to spoken vocabulary development for children with autism, Down syndrome, and typical development. Journal of Cognition and Development, 18(3), 325–337.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Özçalışkan, Ş, Adamson, L. B., Dimitrova, N., & Baumann, S. (2017). Early gesture provides a helping hand to spoken vocabulary development for children with autism, Down syndrome, and typical development. Journal of Cognition and Development, 18(3), 325–337.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, Adamson, L. B., Dimitrova, N., & Baumann, S. (2018). Do parents model gestures differently when children’s gestures differ? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(5), 1492–1507.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Özçalışkan, Ş, Adamson, L. B., Dimitrova, N., & Baumann, S. (2018). Do parents model gestures differently when children’s gestures differ? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(5), 1492–1507.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, & Dimitrova, N. (2013). How gesture Input provides a helping hand to language development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(4), 227–236.PubMedCrossRef Özçalışkan, Ş, & Dimitrova, N. (2013). How gesture Input provides a helping hand to language development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(4), 227–236.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Do iconic gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 1143–1162.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Özçalışkan, Ş, Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Do iconic gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 1143–1162.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005a). Do parents lead their children by the hand? Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 481–505.PubMedCrossRef Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005a). Do parents lead their children by the hand? Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 481–505.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005b). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96, B101–B113.PubMedCrossRef Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005b). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96, B101–B113.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006a). ‘X is like Y’: The emergence of similarity mappings in children’s early speech and gesture. In G. Kristianssen, M Achard, R.Dirven & F. R. de Mendoza (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics: Foundations and fields of application (pp. 229–262). Mouton de Gruyter. Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006a). ‘X is like Y’: The emergence of similarity mappings in children’s early speech and gesture. In G. Kristianssen, M Achard, R.Dirven & F. R. de Mendoza (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics: Foundations and fields of application (pp. 229–262). Mouton de Gruyter.
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006b). Role of gesture in children’s early constructions. In E. Clark & B. Kelly (Eds.), Constructions in acquisition (pp. 31–58). Stanford: CSLI Publications. Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006b). Role of gesture in children’s early constructions. In E. Clark & B. Kelly (Eds.), Constructions in acquisition (pp. 31–58). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, Goldin-Meadow, S., Gentner, D., & Mylander, C. (2009). Does language about similarity foster similarity comparisons in children? Cognition, 112(2), 217–228.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Özçalışkan, Ş, Goldin-Meadow, S., Gentner, D., & Mylander, C. (2009). Does language about similarity foster similarity comparisons in children? Cognition, 112(2), 217–228.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). When gesture–speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 190–217.CrossRef Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). When gesture–speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 190–217.CrossRef
go back to reference Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 163–174). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 163–174). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef
go back to reference Ozturk, S., Pınar, E., Ketrez, F. N., & Özçalışkan, Ş (2021). Effect of sex and dyad composition on speech and gesture development of singleton and twin children. Journal of Child Language. Ozturk, S., Pınar, E., Ketrez, F. N., & Özçalışkan, Ş (2021). Effect of sex and dyad composition on speech and gesture development of singleton and twin children. Journal of Child Language.
go back to reference Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76, 763–782.PubMed Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76, 763–782.PubMed
go back to reference Phillips, J. R. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers’ speech to young children: Age and sex comparisons. Child Development, 44, 182–185.CrossRef Phillips, J. R. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers’ speech to young children: Age and sex comparisons. Child Development, 44, 182–185.CrossRef
go back to reference Ramer, A. L. (1976). Syntactic styles in emerging language. Journal of Child Language, 3(1), 49–62.CrossRef Ramer, A. L. (1976). Syntactic styles in emerging language. Journal of Child Language, 3(1), 49–62.CrossRef
go back to reference Rice, M. L., Zubrick, S. R., Taylor, C. L., Gayán, J., & Bontempo, D. E. (2014). Late language emergence in 24-month-old twins: Heritable and increased risk for late language emergence in twins. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(3), 917–928.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rice, M. L., Zubrick, S. R., Taylor, C. L., Gayán, J., & Bontempo, D. E. (2014). Late language emergence in 24-month-old twins: Heritable and increased risk for late language emergence in twins. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(3), 917–928.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers’ and mothers’ speech in early language development. Journal of Child Language, 7, 353–369.PubMedCrossRef Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers’ and mothers’ speech in early language development. Journal of Child Language, 7, 353–369.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rowe, M. L. (2000). Pointing and talk by low-income mothers and their 14-month-old children. First Language, 20, 305–330. Rowe, M. L. (2000). Pointing and talk by low-income mothers and their 14-month-old children. First Language, 20, 305–330.
go back to reference Rowe, M. L., Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language, 28(2), 182–199.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rowe, M. L., Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language, 28(2), 182–199.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Rutter, M., & Redshaw, J. (1991). Growing up as a twin: Twin–singleton differences in psychological development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 885–896.PubMedCrossRef Rutter, M., & Redshaw, J. (1991). Growing up as a twin: Twin–singleton differences in psychological development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 885–896.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rutter, M., Thorpe, K., Greenwood, R., Northstone, K., & Golding, J. (2003). Twins as a natural experiment to study the causes of mild language delay: I: design; twin–singleton differences in language, and obstetric risks. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 44(3), 326–341.CrossRef Rutter, M., Thorpe, K., Greenwood, R., Northstone, K., & Golding, J. (2003). Twins as a natural experiment to study the causes of mild language delay: I: design; twin–singleton differences in language, and obstetric risks. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 44(3), 326–341.CrossRef
go back to reference Savić, S. (1980). How twins learn to talk: A study of the speech development of twins from 1 to 3. Academic Press. Savić, S. (1980). How twins learn to talk: A study of the speech development of twins from 1 to 3. Academic Press.
go back to reference Schaffer, H. R., & Crook, C. K. (1979). Maternal control techniques in a directed play situation.Child Development, 50, 989–996. Schaffer, H. R., & Crook, C. K. (1979). Maternal control techniques in a directed play situation.Child Development, 50, 989–996.
go back to reference Shatz, M. (1982). On mechanisms of language acquisition: can features of the communicative environment account for development? In E. Wanner & E. L. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 102–127). New York: Cambridge University Press. Shatz, M. (1982). On mechanisms of language acquisition: can features of the communicative environment account for development? In E. Wanner & E. L. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 102–127). New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43, 549–565.CrossRef Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43, 549–565.CrossRef
go back to reference Snow, C. E. (1995). Issues in the study of input: fine-tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In P. M. Fletcher (Ed.), The handbook of child language (pp. 180–194). Oxford: Blackwell. Snow, C. E. (1995). Issues in the study of input: fine-tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In P. M. Fletcher (Ed.), The handbook of child language (pp. 180–194). Oxford: Blackwell.
go back to reference Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press. Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Stafford, L. (1987). Maternal input to twin and singleton children: Implications for language acquisition. Human Communication Research, 13(4), 429–462.CrossRef Stafford, L. (1987). Maternal input to twin and singleton children: Implications for language acquisition. Human Communication Research, 13(4), 429–462.CrossRef
go back to reference Stanfield, C., Williamson, R. A., & Özçalışkan, Ş (2014). How early do children understand gesture-speech combinations with iconic gestures? Journal of Child Language, 41(2), 462–471.PubMedCrossRef Stanfield, C., Williamson, R. A., & Özçalışkan, Ş (2014). How early do children understand gesture-speech combinations with iconic gestures? Journal of Child Language, 41(2), 462–471.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thorpe, K. J., Golding, J., MacGillivray, I., & Greenwood, R. (1991). Comparison of prevalence of depression in mothers of twins and mothers of singletons. British Medical Journal, 302, 875–878.PubMedCrossRef Thorpe, K. J., Golding, J., MacGillivray, I., & Greenwood, R. (1991). Comparison of prevalence of depression in mothers of twins and mothers of singletons. British Medical Journal, 302, 875–878.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thorpe, K. J., Greenwood, R., & Rutter, M. (2003). Twins as a natural experiment to study the causes of mild language delay: II. Family interaction risk factors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(3), 342–355.PubMedCrossRef Thorpe, K. J., Greenwood, R., & Rutter, M. (2003). Twins as a natural experiment to study the causes of mild language delay: II. Family interaction risk factors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(3), 342–355.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.PubMedCrossRef Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tomasello, M., Mannie, S., & Barton, M. (1989). The development of communicative competence in twins. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 2(1), 49–59. Tomasello, M., Mannie, S., & Barton, M. (1989). The development of communicative competence in twins. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 2(1), 49–59.
go back to reference Tomasello, M., Mannle, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1986). Linguistic environment of 1-to 2-year-old twins. Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 169–176.CrossRef Tomasello, M., Mannle, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1986). Linguistic environment of 1-to 2-year-old twins. Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 169–176.CrossRef
go back to reference Tse, S. K., Chan, C., Li, H., & Kwong, S. M. (2002). Sex differences in syntactic development: Evidence from Cantonese-speaking preschoolers in Hong Kong. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(6), 509–517.CrossRef Tse, S. K., Chan, C., Li, H., & Kwong, S. M. (2002). Sex differences in syntactic development: Evidence from Cantonese-speaking preschoolers in Hong Kong. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(6), 509–517.CrossRef
go back to reference Zinober, B., & Martlew, M. (1985). Developmental changes in four types of gesture in relation to acts and vocalizations from 10 to 21 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 293–306.CrossRef Zinober, B., & Martlew, M. (1985). Developmental changes in four types of gesture in relation to acts and vocalizations from 10 to 21 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 293–306.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Parental Speech and Gesture Input to Girls Versus Boys in Singletons and Twins
Authors
Ebru Pınar
Sumeyra Ozturk
F. Nihan Ketrez
Şeyda Özçalışkan
Publication date
10-02-2021
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior / Issue 2/2021
Print ISSN: 0191-5886
Electronic ISSN: 1573-3653
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-020-00356-w

Other articles of this Issue 2/2021

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 2/2021 Go to the issue