Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Environmental Management 2/2023

Open Access 03-04-2023

Personal and Professional Mitigation Behavioral Intentions of Agricultural Experts to Address Climate Change

Authors: Tahereh Zobeidi, Masoud Yazdanpanah, Laura A. Warner, Alexa Lamm, Katharina Löhr, Stefan Sieber

Published in: Environmental Management | Issue 2/2023

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Mitigation activities, whether at the personal level relating to lifestyle or on the professional level, especially in the agriculture sector, are widely encouraged by scientists and policymakers. This research empirically analyses the association between agricultural experts’ perceptions about climate change and their intention to implement climate change mitigation. Based on survey data, individuals’ reported intention to implement personal and professional mitigation behavior is explained using a conceptual model. The structural equation modeling results suggest that the new ecological paradigm (NEP), institutional trust, and risk salience indirectly influence climate change mitigation intentions. The findings indicate that risk perception, personal efficacy, responsibility, belief in climate change occurring, and low psychological distance trigger a significantly greater intention to support personal and professional mitigation behaviors. However, the research framework is much stronger at predicting the intention to mitigate climate change in professional affairs compared to personal activities. The findings suggest that hypothetical distance factors only have a moderating effect on the relationship between higher climate change environmental values, institutional trust, risk salience, and mitigation intention. This paper analytically explores the regulating role of risk perception, hypothetical distance, personal efficacy, and responsibility between institutional trust, risk salience, and the NEP as independent concepts and intention to personal and professional mitigation behaviors as dependent variables. The findings of the study have important implications for encouraging personal and professional mitigation behaviors.

Introduction

Climate change is one of the most challenging social issues of the current era. As of 2023, anthropogenic activities have pushed global temperatures up by about 1.0 °C compared to pre-industrial levels. If current emission rates continue, this figure is likely to reach 1.5 °C between 2030 and 2052 (Fawzy et al. 2020) and 2 °C before 2100 (Malhi et al. 2021). Statistics show that by 2030 and 2050, developing countries will be responsible for up to 64% and 76% of global greenhouse gas emissions, respectively (Sohoo et al. (2020)). Studies using global data from 1970 onwards show that global warming significantly affects many physical and biological systems (Parry 2007). Climate change due to increasing resource scarcity, escalating natural disasters, and rising sea levels can lead to social instability and conflict (Brown et al. 2013). In particular, natural resource-dependent communities tend to be impacted by climate change more severely and, consequently, increasingly struggle to maintain sustainable livelihoods, including ecological and human well-being (Suckall et al. 2014). Climate change threatens the agricultural sector and resource dependent communities through increased soil erosion, reduced soil quality, reduced agricultural production, and decreased food security (Gezie (2019); Loboguerrero et al. 2019; Karimi and Ataei 2022; Aliabadi et al. 2022). In this sense, countries that rely strongly on agricultural production, including most developing countries, are more vulnerable to climate change (Bryan et al. 2009).
Agriculture, at the same time, also contributes to climate change. For example, it is a source of three greenhouse gases (GHGs): nitrous oxide (N2O), methane (CH4), and carbon dioxide (CO2) (de Oliveira et al. (2019)). Significant GHGs are released through crop and soil management, manure management, and methane production during animal digestion; the latter a process called intestinal fermentation (Massey and Ulmer 2010). The use of chemical nitrogen fertilizers, important inputs in modern agricultural systems, leads to the production of direct and indirect N2O (Elrys et al. 2020), which is a powerful factor in global warming. Over a period of 100 years, this gas is 298 times more effective at heating the atmosphere than CO2 (Gu et al. (2017)). In addition, CO2 is released to a large extent from microbial decay or incineration of plant wastes and soil organic matter (Pradisty et al. 2021; Praveen and Sharma 2019). Arbuckle et al. (2013) indicate that production systems are a significant emitter of CO2 and estimate that agriculture produces between 10 and 15% of the world’s human GHGs emissions. Therefore, controlling and reducing GHGs from the agricultural sector is an essential measure and one of the most important mechanisms to prevent the adverse effects of climate change (Venkateswarlu and Shanker 2009). Efforts to reduce GHGs emissions, like climate change mitigation strategies, are widely endorsed by scientists and policymakers, with positive effects linked to both human and natural systems (Schuldt et al. 2018).
Climate change calls for a range of mitigation measures, from measures implemented by law to change technologies, to changes that citizens voluntarily undertake in their daily behavior (Semenza et al. 2008; Chen 2020a). The word “mitigation” refers to reduction of GHGs, with mitigation activities (i.e., reducing the causes of climate change) resulting in reduced GHGs emissions from the source or by replacing and conserving energy, improving sedimentation carbon, etc. (Dhillon and von Wuehlisch 2013; Honegger et al. 2021; Wang et al. 2021). Since most scientists consider the phenomenon of global warming to be characterized by human activities, they want people to participate in measures that reduce the emissions of heat trapping gases, thus reducing the negative effects of global warming (Kahan (2015)).
Agriculture studies show that even small changes in farm practices (e.g., decreasing nitrogen manure use) (Hamid et al. (2021)) can greatly reduce GHGs emissions (Sanz-Cobena et al. (2017)). Thus, farmers’ role in carrying out mitigation activities in farming is important and vital. However, studies in Iran reveal that farmers’ awareness about climate change and mitigation is still not adequate (Yazdanpanah et al. 2022b). Because of this, agricultural experts can play a fundamental role for encouraging and facilitating these initiatives. Meanwhile, agricultural advisors play an important role and can potentially have a great impact on farmers’ climate change mitigation behavior. Agricultural experts can play an important role in finding regionally suitable solutions that reduce GHG emissions from agricultural and livestock activities as well as in providing them to farmers. Agricultural experts can also play an important role in influencing the acceptance of agricultural innovations by farmers (Wheeler 2008) or they can influence the attitudes and behaviors of others (Ghasemi et al. 2013). Indeed, agricultural advisors are an acknowledged trusted source of information for farmers, so understanding their beliefs and conveyed messages are important for better understanding farmers’ decisions regarding climate change (Chatrchyan et al. 2017).
Extension services use agricultural experts to prepare farmers by providing training on best farming practices, thus increasing the level of acceptance of new technologies. Empowering farmers to deal with different forms of climate change risks is very important and, to achieve this, special attention should be paid to teaching options that increase their capacity building. A number of studies investigate the importance of agricultural professionals in raising awareness and encouraging and educating farmers (and the public) about initiatives like mitigation behavior through teaching and extension work (Ghasemi et al. 2013; Bakhtiyari et al. 2017). Agricultural professionals can act as gatekeepers (Bakhtiyari et al. 2017), either facilitating or hampering the adoption of an initiative (Yaghoubi et al. 2019). In the same vein, Karppinen (2005) states that experts are amongst the most important supporters, consultants, and instructors that growers rely on as a trusted source of evidence. Gautam et al. (2013) point out that insights of agricultural agents are key because they are formed by their experience in the sector and local conditions in which they operate, particularly in remote and rural areas.
To better plan and implement new policies and build capacity, it is crucial to understand those factors driving behavior. Researchers find that external and internal factors influence environmental behavior, including demographic factors and psychological factors such as values, beliefs, perceptions, attitudes, and intentions (Brown et al. 2019). Changing people’s perceptions of climate change and increasing their participation in reducing GHG is essential for a successful transition to a low-carbon economy (Capstick et al. 2015; Wibeck 2014). Perception development is an active process based on what exists externally as well as the internalized experiences, desires, needs, preferences, and dislikes of individuals themselves (Euriga et al. 2021). In the context of climate change, an individual’s response may be largely determined by their perceptions of the problem itself rather than their attitudes toward specific behaviors (Hu and Chen 2016). Semenza et al. (2008) point out that voluntary reduction of energy consumption by individuals – conditioned on their awareness and concern about climate change, their willingness to act, and their ability to change – is an important factor in counteracting climate change. Public support for, or opposition to, climate policies (e.g., treaties, regulations, taxes, subsidies) is largely influenced by the public perception of those risks and harms of exposure to global climate change (Leiserowitz 2006). Committees of the US National Research Council (National Research Council (1992)), for example, the Committee on the Human Dimension of Global Change, have identified public perceptions of global phenomena, such as climate change, as a critical factor in both environmental problems and possible solutions (Weber 2010).
There are few studies examining the determinants of climate change mitigation intentions in agriculture. For example, Zhang et al. (2020) and Niles et al. (2016), examine predictors of intention of mitigation using the theory of planned behavior (TPB) and value-belief-norm theory (VBN), while Chen (2020a, 2020b) did so using protection motivation theory and construal level theory (CLT). To enhance knowledge on the effect of psychological factors on mitigative intention, this study uses a conceptual theory based on beliefs (hypothetical distance) and risk perception to predict the intention of agricultural experts using the case of Iran. To the best of our knowledge, despite the importance of both the beliefs and perceptions of agricultural experts in terms of education and orientation to agricultural education for mitigation, no such study has been undertaken. Further, those studies that exist on mitigation behavior (Ferguson and Branscombe 2010; Spence et al. 2011; Ambusaidi et al. 2012; Sinatra et al. 2012; Broomell et al. 2015; Hu and Chen 2016; Niles et al. 2016) do not study it in the context of Iran. Therefore, this study presents novelty in several ways. First, from a scientific perspective, this study attempts to provide a conceptual framework for mitigation given foundational theories used in previous studies. In addition, the constructs considered by this study have a direct relationship with climate change and, for this reason, they seem more appropriate than theoretical frameworks. However, some constructs, like hypothetical distance and risk perception, are derived from well-known theories. The second novelty is that this study uses a research sample from Iran, a developing country whose farmers are generally low in literacy. This study seeks to identify which psychological factors affect the intentions of agricultural experts to engage in personal and professional mitigation behaviors in agriculture to reduce climate change.

Conceptual Framework

In this study, we assume that belief in the existence of climate change is pivotal in addressing the tensions between the abstractness of global climate change and the concrete intentions to mitigate climate change locally. To develop our conceptual framework on the intensions of experts to mitigate climate change, we examine the effect of hypothetical distance and risk perception on the intention to engage in mitigation behaviors. Hypothetical distance, one aspect of psychological distance, refers to uncertainty regarding occurrence of a phenomena (see, Spence et al. 2012; McDonald et al. 2015; Maiella et al. 2020). Hypothetical distance is related to the perceived probability (likelihood or unlikelihood) of occurrence or non-occurrence of an event (McDonald et al. 2015). The shorter the hypothetical distance, the more people believe in the occurrence of climate change. Uncertainty about the occurrence of climate change often leads to people not fully understanding the various associated predictions and, therefore, inaccurately analyzing the probability of its occurrence (Maiella et al. 2020). For example, people who are skeptical about climate changes are less likely to adapt their behavior (Spence et al. 2012). Researchers believe that perceived or actual uncertainty reduces the number of times individuals engage in pro-environmental behavior (Gifford (2011); Aitken et al. 2011). Without a belief that climate change is happening, people pay little attention to actions required to address it. Therefore, reduced hypothetical distance and greater belief in the occurrence of climate change will increase people’s willingness to support mitigation measures. Many people do not always behave in a sustainable way, which is said to be partly because they perceive climate change as a distant psychological issue (Spence et al. 2012). Some studies argue that reducing hypothetical distance and relating to it can increase the likelihood of behavior change (McDonald et al. 2015; Schuldt et al. 2018).
Past research demonstrates that perceived risk (judging of the severity and urgency of the problem of climate change) is the strongest predictor of actions. Those who acknowledge that the risks of climate change are high are more likely to act (Kollmuss and Agyeman 2002; Aitken et al. 2011). Arbuckle et al. (2013) show that farmers who are concerned about the effects of climate change on agriculture have more supportive mitigation measures. Feeling worried, or perceiving danger, is one of the most important factors in determining whether people engage in professional environmental behavior (Kollmuss and Agyeman 2002).
However, climate change is considered a “dead” hazard among similar natural processes, like temperature changes and climate fluctuations, with little salience as a high-risk issue because it is not directly experienced (Whitmarsh 2008). While it is difficult to judge climate change as an abstract concept based on personal experience (Weber 2010), risk salience is a factor influencing the perception of climate change, which comprises two components of close proximity to risk and related previous experience (Carlton and Jacobson 2013). Research emphasizes that experience influences risk perception. Previous experience may affect the perception of danger by engaging people cognitively. People who have suffered from an environmental catastrophe are more likely to remember and relate their perceived risks when considering environmental hazards. Past studies also predict that personal experiences or relationships with local climate and extreme climatic events cause climate change to move from being abstract to being a familiar, real, and immediate concept (Akerlof et al. 2012). Personal or direct experience may be a factor that influences the intention to implement mitigation behaviors (Ogunbode et al. 2019; Capstick et al. 2015). Existing research shows that environmental views and perceptions of climate change can be related to people’s physical surroundings and their experiences (Spence et al. 2011). For example, Drummond and Palmer (2014) show that belief in global warming increases when physical heat is experienced by being in a heated room.
Feeling personal efficacy and responsibility are other factors influencing risk perception, with people who feel less effective and responsible for climate change having less concern or risk perception (Kellstedt et al. 2008). Personal efficacy is a central concept used in health studies. Heath and Gifford (2006) also predict the effectiveness of responses in general and/or the belief that their efforts to reduce global warming will make a difference are predictors of intention. Stoutenborough and Vedlitz (2014) show that those who have most perceived efficacy are motivated to better understand climate change. Whitmarsh (2009) also believes that behavioral intentions to address climate change are influenced by perceived responsibility to cause and respond to climate change.
Scientists argue that trust is an important factor determining public perceptions of climate change and, consequently, support of mitigation efforts (Hmielowski et al. (2014)). Trust is defined as an expectation that empowers and motivates the trustee to behave in a way that is valued by the trustor (Hmielowski et al. (2014)). Cologna and Siegrist (2020) believe that individuals who make choices in circumstances of uncertainty and poor knowledge, like climate change, tend to rely on trusted institutions for guidance, with the level of trust determined by public acceptance. Chryssochoidis et al. (2009) point out that institutional trust is flexible, typically shaped by socio-cultural factors and value systems.
The New Environmental or Ecological Paradigm (NEP; Dunlap 2008) is broadly recognized as a reliable multiple‐item scale to capture environmental attitudes (Yu et al. 2019) or environmental values (Ziegler (2021)). NEP consider a set of basic beliefs about humankind’s association with environment, containing the concept that current civilizations have harmed the stability of nature, restricted growth, and the necessity to shed an anthropocentric orientation toward the environment. This set of beliefs is grounded in the notion that these beliefs are more central than attitudes toward precise issues, like support for contamination regulator (Amburgey and Thoman 2012).
NEP assumes that an environmental behavior is the result of individual environmental worldviews, reflecting people’s beliefs about humanity’s ability to disturb nature’s balance, the existence of growth restrictions on human societies, and human rights to rule over nature (Chen 2020a). NEP, according to Dunlap (2008), is a standard instrument in social and behavioral disciplines that is increasingly used as an indicator for environmental values, concern, awareness, or attitudes in economics (Ziegler (2021)). Environmental attitudes and values reflect having a good understanding of a set of beliefs, interests, or laws that influence environmental protection behavior (Rodríguez-Barreiro et al. 2013). Various studies show that environmental values (NEP) can influence many factors. For example, Wang et al. (2022) and Sarrasin et al. (2022) find that pro-environmental values can influence perceived behavioral control or self-efficacy. Stoutenborough and Vedlitz (2014) and Kellstedt et al. (2008) find that those with higher ecological values are likely to try and understand climate change because of their environmental concerns (see Fig. 1).

Methodology

Survey

The purpose of this study is to identify factors affecting the intention to implement personal and professional GHGs emission mitigation behaviors by agricultural experts. Personal mitigation behaviors include walking, riding bicycles, taking public transportation, choosing eco-friendly products, avoiding the purchase of out-of-season food, reusing and repairing items instead of throwing them away, paying more taxes to combat climate change, as well as saving on paper and napkins. Professional mitigation behaviors in an agricultural context include increasing organic agriculture, using renewable energy, using less polluting and more energy efficient machinery, as well as zero tillage management (Kreft et al. 2021).
This research was conducted as a non-experimental cross-sectional survey to test the hypotheses of the conceptual framework. Data collection was conducted using face-to-face questionnaires with randomly selected agricultural professionals (n = 320) in Khuzestan province, Iran, from the two majors agricultural organization (Agricultural-Jihad centers) in 2020.
The distribution of socio-economic characteristics data of these experts shows that more than half the sample are male: 178 (55.6%) males; 142 (44.4%) females. The average age is 35.53 (SD = 8.10) ranging from 22 to 70. Average work experience is 9.12 (SD = 7.56) years, ranging from 0 to 38 years. A significant percentage of experts have a Bachelor’s degree 163 (50.9%). After that, 29.7% (95) have a Master’s degree and 12.2% (39) a Doctorate. Two people (6.0%) have an Associate’s degree, while 13 (4.1%) have a diploma and 8 (2.5%) did not answer this question.
The respondents were asked to respond to statements using a 5-point Likert scale, with 1 = strongly disagree; 2 = disagree; 3 = neutral; 4 = agree; and 5 = strongly agree. The survey items are adapted from several studies, with the items, sources, and descriptive statistics shown in Table 1. The reliability and validity of the survey data were tested using SPSS 23.0. The results show that Cronbach’s alpha values for all constructs are higher than 7.0, thus indicating good internal consistency (Nunnally 1978).
Table 1
Sources, items and descriptive statistics of survey items
Constructs
Items
Factor loading
Cronbach’s alpha
Means
S.D.
Institutional trust
Stoutenborough and Vedlitz 2014
Government agencies provide accurate and reliable information on climate change.
0.84
0.85
3.03
1.15
Government agencies provide timely information on climate change.
0.88
2.82
1.27
Risk salience
Carlton and Jacobson 2013
I know people who have problems due to extreme weather.
0.75
0.83
3.64
1.19
I have never seen a crop or farm that has a problem due to climate change.
0.82
3.61
1.18
I see that the farms in this area are damaged due to climate change.
0.80
3.38
1.22
NEP
Dunlap et al. 2000
I do not think climate change has increased, only these days the media reports on it more.
0.71
0.76
3.86
1.13
What is called climate change is just a natural fluctuation in the earth’s temperature.
0.63
3.72
1.17
Information about climate change and global warming does not apply to me.
0.68
4.21
1.14
I feel a moral duty to do something to combat climate change.
0.58
3.89
1.14
Personal Efficacy
Kellstedt et al. 2008
Yazdanpanah et al. 2022a
I can easily train farmers to produce less GHG.
0.69
0.83
3.28
1.06
If you want, I can easily educate farmers about the effects of climate change.
0.76
3.19
1.08
In my daily life, I can easily do things that reduce GHG emissions.
0.76
3.32
1.14
I can easily train farmers to do things that emit less GHG.
0.76
3.38
1.18
Responsibility
Kellstedt et al. 2008
I feel responsible for the contribution of agriculture to global warming.
0.78
0.83
3.70
1.08
We humans are responsible for climate change.
0.78
3.82
1.09
Tackling climate change is not only the responsibility of government and industry but also individuals like me.
0.74
3.92
1.07
Every citizen is responsible for reducing GHG emissions.
0.72
3.72
1.20
Risk perception/public concern
Leiserowitz 2006;
Kellstedt et al. 2008
In the next 25 years, global warming and climate change will have significant negative effects on human health.
0.64
0.83
4.05
0.88
In the next 25 years, global warming and climate change will have significant negative effects on my economic and financial situation.
0.60
3.67
0.97
In the next 25 years, global warming and climate change will have significant negative effects on the health of the Iranian people.
0.75
4.09
0.88
In the next 25 years, global warming and climate change will have significant negative effects on Iran’s economic development.
0.70
4.01
0.85
Belief in happening/ low Hypothetical distance
Spence et al. 2012; Arbuckle et al. 2013
Variability in precipitation is more than in previous years.
0.78
0.87
4.00
1.12
Winters are not as cold as in previous years.
0.78
3.97
1.07
The current summers are warmer than the previous years.
0.73
4.07
1.04
Compared to previous years, annual precipitation has decreased.
0.85
4.06
1.05
Personal mitigation intention
Chen 2020a
Buy eco-friendly products.
0.59
0.85
4.38
1.27
Do not buy food out of season.
0.66
4.37
1.36
Walk, ride bicycles, or use public transportation.
0.64
4.42
1.24
Reuse and repair items instead of throwing them away.
0.70
4.65
1.30
Pay more taxes to combat climate change.
0.80
4.71
1.19
Save on paper and napkins.
0.67
4.63
1.12
Professional mitigation intention
Fawzy et al. 2020
Zhang et al. 2020
Because of my commitment to future generations, I intent to teach ways to reduce GHG emissions from livestock.
0.73
0.85
3.96
1.00
I intend to teach decarbonization technologies, such as the use of renewable energy in agriculture.
0.75
3.45
1.25
I intend to teach using no-till and direct seeding methods.
0.72
3.27
1.08

Data Analysis

Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) is a regression-based technique that is frequently applied to validate hypothetical or theoretical models (Ho et al. 2016). SEM can calculate measurement error and can simultaneously estimate model path coefficients (Fan et al. 2016). Therefore, SEM analysis is used to evaluate the validity of the measurement model and the explanatory power of the structural model in predicting the intention of agricultural experts to implement personal and professional mitigation behaviors.
The two stages of SEM include confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to appraise the suitability of the measurement model and then modeling of structural equations (Anderson and Gerbing 1988). CFA was performed using AMOS 23 software. CFA is validated using indicators such as Goodness of Fit Index (GFI), Root Mean Square (RMS), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and the Normed Fit Index (NFI). A model is considered as acceptable if the NFI exceeds the threshold of 0.90, GFI is greater than 0.90, CFI is more than 0.90, and RMSEA is less than 0.08 (Hair et al. 2010). CFI was also used to assess the superiority and suitability of the measurement model by investigative convergent validity and discriminant validity.
To confirm the reliability of the research constructs, two indices including the Composite reliability (CR) and Cronbach’s Alpha were used. The CR is the consistent reliability of all measurement variables and its index meaning is like Cronbach’s alpha, representing the degree of internal consistency of the latent constructs. Cronbach’s alpha and CR values should all be more than 0.7. The results satisfied these requirements with Cronbach’s alpha values ranging from 0.76 to 0.87 and CR values ranging from 0.74 to 0.77, indicating high internal consistency and confirming good reliability of scales in this study (Table 2).
Table 2
Correlation between constructs and convergent validity
 
Institutional trust
Risk salience
NEP
Risk perception
Personal Efficacy
Responsibility
Belief in happening
Personal mitigation
Professional mitigation
Institutional trust
1
        
Risk salience
0.360**
1
       
NEP
0.33**
0.64**
1
      
Risk perception
0.16**
0.30**
0.17**
1
     
Personal Efficacy
0.40**
0.51**
0.49**
0.17**
1
    
Responsibility
0.19**
0.52**
0.56**
0.14**
0.55**
1
   
Belief in happening
0.25**
0.67**
0.66**
0.24**
0.38**
0.55**
1
  
Personal mitigation
0.25**
0.47**
0.53**
0.06
0.54**
0.56**
0.55**
1
 
Professional mitigation
0.31**
0.56**
0.56**
0.09
0.66**
0.72**
0.55**
0.61**
1
CR
0.851
0.833
0.746
0.769
0.831
0.842
0.866
0.777
0.836
AVE
0.74
0.625
0.425
0.456
0.552
0.571
0.618
0.538
0.462
**p < 0.01.
To confirm convergent validity, the AVE of each construct should be higher than 0.5 (Fornell and Larcker 1981). As shown in Table 2, the current AVE values ranged from 0.42 to 0.74, thus showing good convergent validity of this study (Yu et al. 2019). The AVE values of all constructs except risk perception, NEP, and intention to professional mitigation, were lower than the threshold of 0.5 and the CR of all constructs was 0.7 (Table 2). Fornell and Larcker (1981) note that if the CR of a construct is higher than 0.7 then AVE values between 0.4 and 0.5 can be considered acceptable (Table 2).
The results of CFA show that all standardized factor loadings were greater than 0.6 and significant at the critical level of 0.01, indicating good discriminant validity. Meanwhile, the estimation of the parameters between the measured items and the conforming structures is statistically significant at the level of 0.01, which shows that each measured item has a strong ability to explain its corresponding latent construct.
Based on Fornell and Larcker (1981), discriminant validity was measured by using paired analysis of correlation coefficients. Comparison of the squared root of AVE and paired variable coefficients demonstrates that the squared root of AVE is higher than the correlation coefficients, representing the existence of discriminant validity. The consequences show that the square of the AVE of the latent variables studied is higher than the correlation of that latent with all other variables; therefore, the research tool has good discriminant validity.
In a structural model, after confirmation of measurement model, the degree of direct and indirect impact between the variables is examined. Here, the findings are presented in two separate structural models, including model 1 to formulate and test the hypotheses in a model that determines personal mitigation behaviors and model 2 for professional mitigation behaviors.

Results

Correlation between Constructs of the Model

The results show that all latent model constructs, except risk perception, have a significant correlation with the intention to implement personal and professional mitigation behaviors.

Verification of Measurement Model

The goodness-of-fit indices of the two CFA model are as follows: model 1: chi-square value is 649.733 (df = 395), p = 0.000, relative chi-sq = 1.645, GFI = 0.885, CFI = 0.948, IFI = 0.949, RMSEA = 0.045, SRMR = 0.0467.
Model 2: chi-square value is 598.083 (df = 312), p = 0.000, relative chi-sq = 1.917, GFI = 0.883, CFI = 0.938, IFI = 0.939, RMSEA = 0.054, SRMR = 0.0493. These results indicate that the conceptual models of personal and professional mitigation behaviors fit the practical data with acceptable validity.

Structural Models of Intention to Personal and Professional Mitigation Behaviors

The goodness-of-fit indices of the two structural models are as follows: model 1: chi-square value is 750.771 (df = 410), p = 0.000, relative chi-sq = 1.827, GFI = 0.869, CFI = 0.931, IFI = 0.931, RMSEA = 0.051, SRMR = 0.0513.
Model 2: chi-square value is 703.398 (df = 327), p = 0.000, relative chi-sq = 2.151, GFI = 0.860, CFI = 0.924, IFI = 0.919, RMSEA = 0.060, SRMR = 0.0539. These results indicated that the conceptual models of personal and professional mitigation behaviors fit the practical data with acceptable validity.
Based on the findings, conceptual model 1 predicts 68% of the intention to implement personal mitigation behaviors. As shown in Table 3, institutional trust (β = 0.206, t = 3.637, p < 0.0001), and risk salience (β = 0.929, t = 11.998, p < 0.0001) predict 74% of hypothetical distance or belief in the existence of climate change. The lower the trust in the government, the greater the confidence and belief in climate change. The greater risk salience, the more people believe in climate change. Model 1 predicts 13%, 46%, and 60% of the variance changes in risk perception, personal efficacy, and responsibility, respectively.
Table 3
Estimates of the structural models
Hypothesis
Unstandardized
Regression Weights
SE
Standardized Regression Weights
C.R
sig
Results
Model 1
 Institutional trust
Hypothetical distance
0.189
0.052
0.206
3.637
*
Supported
 Risk salience
Hypothetical distance
0.869
0.072
0.929
11.998
*
Supported
 Institutional trust
Personal Efficacy
0.207
0.057
0.235
3.676
*
Supported
 Risk salience
Risk perception
0.226
0.044
0.358
5.152
*
Supported
 Risk salience
Personal Efficacy
0.068
0.185
0.075
0.367
0.714
Rejected
 NEP
Personal Efficacy
0.608
0.276
0.475
2.203
0.028
Supported
 Personal Efficacy
Responsibility
0.434
0.063
0.457
6.921
*
Supported
 Hypothetical distance
Responsibility
0.406
0.058
0.445
6.964
*
Supported
 Personal Efficacy
Personal mitigation
0.300
0.066
0.345
4.518
*
Supported
 Responsibility
Personal mitigation
0.235
0.083
0.256
2.837
0.005
Supported
 Risk perception
Personal mitigation
0.215
0.068
0.173
3.155
0.002
Supported
 Hypothetical distance (Belief in happening)
Personal mitigation
0.343
0.064
0.411
5.374
*
Supported
Model 2
 Institutional trust
Hypothetical distance
0.182
0.052
0.198
3.521
*
Supported
 Risk salience
Hypothetical distance
0.860
0.072
0.923
11.972
*
Supported
 Institutional trust
Personal Efficacy
0.188
0.055
0.218
3.421
*
Supported
 Risk salience
Risk perception
0.224
0.044
0.357
5.136
*
Supported
 Risk salience
Personal Efficacy
0.081
0.171
0.093
0.475
0.635
Rejected
 NEP
Personal Efficacy
0.613
0.258
0.491
2.374
0.018
Supported
 Personal Efficacy
Responsibility
0.451
0.065
0.474
6.924
*
Supported
 Hypothetical distance
Responsibility
0.383
0.058
0.430
6.620
*
Supported
 Personal Efficacy
Professional mitigation
0.337
0.061
0.393
5.542
*
Supported
 Responsibility
Professional mitigation
0.478
0.078
0.529
6.139
*
Supported
 Risk perception
Professional mitigation
0.122
0.054
0.102
2.279
0.023
Supported
 Hypothetical distance
Professional mitigation
0.147
0.049
0.182
2.298
0.003
Supported
Sig significance
*p < 0.001
The variance explanatory power (R2) value in conceptual model 2 is 90% for the intention to mitigation professional behaviors in agriculture. The study model predicts 78%, 13%, 49%, and 61% of variance changes in hypothetical distance, risk perception, personal efficacy, and responsibility, respectively.
Belief in the occurrence climate change is the strongest predictor of the intention to implement personal mitigation behaviors (β = 0.411, t = 5.374, p < 0.0001), while responsibility is the strongest predictor of the intention to engage in professional mitigation behaviors (β = 0.529, t = 6.139, p < 0.0001).

Discussion

Globally, just as in past scientific research and political debates, climate change mitigation activities are attracting much attention both in public debate and academic research. This study uses a conceptual framework to predict the personal (Model 1) and professional (Model 2) behavioral intentions of agricultural experts to take action to reduce GHG emissions. The results show that both models explain high and very high percentages of the intention to implement both personal and professional mitigation behaviors. According to the findings, hypothetical distance is significantly associated with intention to reduce GHG emission, such that more belief in climate change happening is consistent with a greater intention to mitigate. People are more likely to engage in personal and professional mitigating behaviors if they are less skeptical and more confident about the occurrence of climate change and, in fact, know their hypothetical distance to climate change is low. Therefore, the findings emphasize the importance of reducing psychological distance, and specifically hypothetical distance, to reduce GHG emissions. This reduction of psychological distance probably leads to increased anxiety and, in turn, an increased tendency to act. The findings also show that perceptions of risks in the next 25 years, which can also be defined as concerns, directly affect the intention to implement personal and professional mitigation behaviors. Therefore, the more people feel that climate change in the next 25 years will affect human health and economic status, both globally and within Iran, the more they intend to implement mitigation behaviors. In this regard, Weber et al. (2010) highlight that if individuals do not suppose that climate change is occurring or do not perceive climate change as a threat to their livelihood, then it is more likely they will not take action to mitigate climate change. Spence et al. (2012) show that concerns about the effects of climate change are associated with intention to act. Arbuckle et al. (2013) consider the vulnerability perceived by farmers to be important and believe that farmers’ concerns about the impact of climate change are key to successful adaptation and mitigation of the effects of climate change. Yazdanpanah et al. (2022b) find that farmers’ risk perception at the farm scale plays a mediating role between overall climate belief and mitigation practices. However, there are different findings about the effect of belief in climate change happening and intention. For example, O’Connor et al. (2002) find no association between believing in a rise in temperature and supporting politics or expressing the possibility of participating in voluntary GHG mitigation behaviors.
Findings confirm that personal efficacy directly influences mitigation intention. People are more interested in implementing mitigation actions if they believe that their individual responses in daily life and in agricultural education will reduce GHG emissions. In this regard, Gifford (2011) acknowledges ‘limited cognition,’ which demonstrates itself as low self-efficacy or inefficacy as one of seven “dragons of inaction”. Inefficacy views can arise from the insight that climate variability is an unavoidable and, consequently, that individual behaviors, or even the mitigation efforts of a solitary cluster or country, will have a minimal outcome.
Risk salience, which refers to the proximity and experience of the adverse effects of climate change, influences belief in climate change happening or hypothetical distance. Recent personal experiences also strongly affect risk perception. Although personal experience of the serious consequences of global warming is still rare in many parts of the world, its effects are highly visible on agricultural land and in other vulnerable rural areas. For example, observing people or crops and lands that have been damaged by climate change leads to understanding of the dangers of climate change. Further, Carlton and Jacobson (2013) conclude that risk salience is one of the important factors in determining the perception of risk. The notion that the risk salience of climate change provides a potentially important path to intention is widely confirmed. Demski et al. (2017) show that personal issue salience directly affects behavioral intention and support for mitigation policies. In addition, Broomell et al. (2015) study of 11,000 respondents from 24 countries finds that personal experience with global warming corresponds to intention to take specific measures, like using less air conditioning in the summer. Such personal experiences may lead to greater familiarity with risk and, thus, to greater understanding by individuals (Demski et al. (2017)).
Evidence supports that agricultural experts’ views on personal responsibility are generally positive and significant in influencing mitigation. This finding indicates the importance of personal responsibility in influencing mitigation behavior. In terms of responsibility, personal responsibility refers to agricultural experts’ beliefs about doing something for a better future and refers all human responsible for reducing GHG emissions. Therefore, individuals are more likely to engage in mitigating behaviors if they know humans are responsible for the causes of climate change and have high risk perception.
There are several mediators between risk experience, risk salience, institutional trust, and the intention to engage in mitigation behaviors. It is important to increase risk salience or provide opportunities for direct experience among agricultural professionals. For this purpose, it is possible to increase the practical visits by agricultural experts to farms where crops have been damaged due to climate change, as well as visits with farmers whose health has been threatened. Indeed, direct observation of farms and crops damaged is necessary to facilitate real communication and increased personal observation of damaged crops by agricultural experts and researchers. Viewing products damaged by extreme heat or water shortages, thus gaining objective and personal experience with climate change events, can have three important consequences: First, it increases risk salience; second, it increases risk perception; and, third, it reduces the hypothetical distance, thus strengthening people’s beliefs about the reality of climate change. The most successful way to strengthen mitigation involves government interventions in agricultural service organizations that focus on local, tangible, and practical aspects. Executive policies should draw office-based experts to field experiences on agricultural land, thus increasing the likelihood of implementing mitigation strategies both personally and professionally.
Institutional trust plays an important role in believing in climate change and personal efficacy. Trust is especially important when the level of uncertainty is high and the level of knowledge is low, such as those for climate change risks. In these situations, people depend on information provided by risk managers, seeking their knowledge before making informed decisions. The more experts feel that complete, up-to-date, and accurate information is being provided by government agencies, the more they believe in climate change. In a meta-analysis, Cologna and Siegrist (2020) show that trusting that institutions provide relevant information is associated with climate-friendly behaviors, but this relationship is weak. Agricultural experts who believe that information from the government and other upstream organizations is accurate, up-to-date, and timely will also have more confidence in their ability to take action to mitigate climate change. The findings also show that the NEP significantly affects beliefs in climate change and personnel efficacy. This construct has an indirect effect on the intention to mitigate both personal and professional behaviors. In this regard, Bouman et al. (2020) show that stronger endorsements of biospheric values are coupled with greater commitment to weather mitigation behaviors.
The explanatory power of the model is 68% for personal behavioral intention and 90% for professional behavioral intention. Explanatory power of endogenous latent variables exceeds the recommended value of 0.5 (Yu et al. 2019) for both personal and professional behaviors, which shows that the model is strong and stable. However, the power of explanation in professional intentions is much higher than personal behavioral intention. In other word, occupational mitigation behaviors are more influenced by perceptions. Personal behaviors are probably perceived as being more costly than professional behaviors, thus economic factors also influence implementation of diminishing personal behaviors. The better predictive power of the professional model reveals there may be more factors that influence personal mitigation behaviors beyond those captured in our study.
From a theoretical perspective, the findings from this study represent an important step in developing a comprehensive understanding of the mitigation behaviors of Iranian agricultural experts, which can be used to adjust their communication and education systems in response to climate change.
Policy interventions should emphasize growing community and expert efforts in agricultural extension for actual mitigation performances and eliminating obstacles. In this regard, there is a clear need to improve communication efforts that emphasize and reveal the efficiency of individual actions and to create a stronger sense of obligation for addressing climate change. Public education must clearly address misunderstandings and identify those actions that are most effective in mitigating climate change. In this regard, sources of information, including, among others, the government, must be understood as trusted information sources.
Considering that the strongest predictor of the intention to implement personal actions is hypothetical distance and the strongest predictor of professional intentions is responsibility, our clearest suggestion is to focus on these two concepts. In order to reduce the hypothetical distance, it is suggested that agricultural experts should be frequently exposed to general and detailed information through climate change reports and statistics (temperature, precipitation,…) in different ways. These can include, among others, radio, television, and other mass media that increase awareness about this phenomenon and the need to respond to it. Thus, they will increase mitigation measures in their daily lives. In order to improve responsibility, it is necessary to learn about the causes of climate change, the role of human activities, and the role of the agricultural sector, especially in the workplace.
Since climate risk perception is a precondition for effective climate communication and mitigation, agricultural politicians and organizations need to increase the discourses on climate risks through the media, farmers’ associations, and other farmer groups. Improving agricultural specialists’ knowledge and opinions of risk issues with respect to climate change could be one long-term structural reply to address climate variability.

Conclusions and Limitations

People who think climate change is hypothetical, who do not recognize the behaviors that emit GHG and its consequences, ignore their responsibility for climate change, or have a low perceived risk, are unlikely to support GHG mitigation strategies or modify their behavior to reduce GHG emissions. Individuals may be anxious about climate change and want to do the right thing, but they are unlikely to do the right thing if they do not know that their behaviors – like using cars or consumption that results in trees being cut down – are directly linked to climate change. Therefore, accurate understanding of the causes of climate change can help improve the accountability and effectiveness of perceived responses and help provide a strong cognitive predictor of GHG mitigation measures. This contributes to current debates about the role of psychological distance and skepticism about climate change as a potential barrier to both public and professional participation in mitigation. Researchers should equally consider the possible limitations of climate change approximation and recognize that bringing the effects of climate change closer is unlikely to amplify climate change mitigation alone. Instead changing psychological distance should be associated with perceived risk, increased efficacy, and responsibility. Therefore, educating people about environmental issues needs to cover different dimensions if climate change mitigation behavior is to be induced.
This study contributes to the literature by (1) empirically examining the moderating role of risk perception, hypothetical distance, personal effectiveness, and responsibility between institutional trust, risk salience, and the new ecological paradigm with intention to implement personal and professional mitigation; and (2) developing a conceptual model that combines VBN and NAM with hypothetical distances. This research has several limits that must be considered when interpreting the outcomes. First, the analysis in this study uses non-experimental and cross-sectional data. Future research should duplicate this study using longitudinal data in which samples are randomly separated into groups. Second, the random sample used in this study is from Khuzestan province, Iran, where people’s livelihood is highly dependent on agriculture. Therefore, the generalizability of our result is limited to this province only. Future research should include examples from other parts of Iran and other developing countries. In addition, this study focuses on the intentions of individuals, although, in many cases, individuals fail to translate intention into behavior, so future studies should examine the actual behavior of individuals. Other groups, like farmers and policymakers, among others, may have different values, perceptions, and behaviors toward mitigating climate change; thus, their views should be examined. The lack of participation in mitigative measures can be due to cognitive constraints: this must be considered in future work. Hence, future research should also consider the effect of other social psychological constructs like norms, awareness, social trust, perceived barriers, perceived costs, and cultural factors on mitigating behaviors in response to climate change.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. All materials and methods are performed in accordance with the instructions and regulations and this research has been approved by a committee at Agricultural Sciences and Natural Resources University of Khuzestan, Mollasani, Iran.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.
Literature
go back to reference Aitken C, Chapman R, McClure J (2011) Climate change, powerlessness and the commons dilemma: assessing New Zealanders’ preparedness to act. Glob Environ Change 21(2):752–760CrossRef Aitken C, Chapman R, McClure J (2011) Climate change, powerlessness and the commons dilemma: assessing New Zealanders’ preparedness to act. Glob Environ Change 21(2):752–760CrossRef
go back to reference Akerlof K, Maibach EW, Fitzgerald D, Cedeno AY, Neuman A (2012) Do people “personally experience” global warming, and if so how, and does it matter? Glob Environ Change 23(1):81–91CrossRef Akerlof K, Maibach EW, Fitzgerald D, Cedeno AY, Neuman A (2012) Do people “personally experience” global warming, and if so how, and does it matter? Glob Environ Change 23(1):81–91CrossRef
go back to reference Aliabadi V, Ataei P, Gholamrezai S (2022) Farmers’ strategies for drought adaptation based on the indigenous knowledge system: the case of Iran. Weather Clim Soc 14(2):561–568CrossRef Aliabadi V, Ataei P, Gholamrezai S (2022) Farmers’ strategies for drought adaptation based on the indigenous knowledge system: the case of Iran. Weather Clim Soc 14(2):561–568CrossRef
go back to reference Amburgey JW, Thoman DB (2012) Dimensionality of the new ecological paradigm: issues of factor structure and measurement. Environ Behav 44(2):235–256CrossRef Amburgey JW, Thoman DB (2012) Dimensionality of the new ecological paradigm: issues of factor structure and measurement. Environ Behav 44(2):235–256CrossRef
go back to reference Ambusaidi A, Boyes E, Stanisstreet M, Taylor N (2012) Omani students’ views about global warming: Beliefs about actions and willingness to act. Int Res Geogr Environ 21(1):21–39CrossRef Ambusaidi A, Boyes E, Stanisstreet M, Taylor N (2012) Omani students’ views about global warming: Beliefs about actions and willingness to act. Int Res Geogr Environ 21(1):21–39CrossRef
go back to reference Anderson JC, Gerbing DW (1988) Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol Bull 103(3):411CrossRef Anderson JC, Gerbing DW (1988) Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol Bull 103(3):411CrossRef
go back to reference Arbuckle Jr JG, Morton LW, Hobbs J (2013) Farmer beliefs and concerns about Climate Change and attitudes toward adaptation and mitigation: evidence from Iowa. Clim Change 118:551–563CrossRef Arbuckle Jr JG, Morton LW, Hobbs J (2013) Farmer beliefs and concerns about Climate Change and attitudes toward adaptation and mitigation: evidence from Iowa. Clim Change 118:551–563CrossRef
go back to reference Bakhtiyari Z, Yazdanpanah M, Forouzani M, Kazemi N (2017) Intention of agricultural professionals toward biofuels in Iran: Implications for energy security, society, and policy. Renew Sust Energ Rev 69:341–349CrossRef Bakhtiyari Z, Yazdanpanah M, Forouzani M, Kazemi N (2017) Intention of agricultural professionals toward biofuels in Iran: Implications for energy security, society, and policy. Renew Sust Energ Rev 69:341–349CrossRef
go back to reference Bouman T, Verschoor M, Albers CJ, Böhm G, Fisher SD, Poortinga W, Steg L (2020) When worry about climate change leads to climate action: How values, worry and personal responsibility relate to various climate actions. Glob Environ Change 62:102061CrossRef Bouman T, Verschoor M, Albers CJ, Böhm G, Fisher SD, Poortinga W, Steg L (2020) When worry about climate change leads to climate action: How values, worry and personal responsibility relate to various climate actions. Glob Environ Change 62:102061CrossRef
go back to reference Broomell SB, Budescu DV, Por HH (2015) Personal experience with climate change predicts intentions to act. Glob Environ Change 32:67–73CrossRef Broomell SB, Budescu DV, Por HH (2015) Personal experience with climate change predicts intentions to act. Glob Environ Change 32:67–73CrossRef
go back to reference Brown HCP, Smit B, Somorin OA, Sonwa DJ, Ngana F (2013) Institutional perceptions, adaptive capacity and climate change response in a post-conflict country: a case study from Central African Republic. Clim Dev 5(3):206–216CrossRef Brown HCP, Smit B, Somorin OA, Sonwa DJ, Ngana F (2013) Institutional perceptions, adaptive capacity and climate change response in a post-conflict country: a case study from Central African Republic. Clim Dev 5(3):206–216CrossRef
go back to reference Brown P, Daigneault A, Dawson J (2019) Age, values, farming objectives, past management decisions, and future intentions in New Zealand agriculture. J Environ Manag 231:110–120CrossRef Brown P, Daigneault A, Dawson J (2019) Age, values, farming objectives, past management decisions, and future intentions in New Zealand agriculture. J Environ Manag 231:110–120CrossRef
go back to reference Bryan E, Deressa TT, Gbetibouo GA, Ringler C (2009) Adaptation to climate change in Ethiopia and South Africa: options and constraints. Environ Sci Policy 12(4):413–426CrossRef Bryan E, Deressa TT, Gbetibouo GA, Ringler C (2009) Adaptation to climate change in Ethiopia and South Africa: options and constraints. Environ Sci Policy 12(4):413–426CrossRef
go back to reference Capstick SB, Demski CC, Sposato RG, Pidgeon NF, Spence A, Corner AJ (2015) Public perception of climate change in Britain following the winter 2013/2014 flooding Capstick SB, Demski CC, Sposato RG, Pidgeon NF, Spence A, Corner AJ (2015) Public perception of climate change in Britain following the winter 2013/2014 flooding
go back to reference Carlton SJ, Jacobson SK (2013) Climate change and coastal environmental risk perceptions in Florida. J Environ Manag 130:32–39CrossRef Carlton SJ, Jacobson SK (2013) Climate change and coastal environmental risk perceptions in Florida. J Environ Manag 130:32–39CrossRef
go back to reference Chatrchyan AM, Erlebacher RC, Chaopricha NT, Chan J, Tobin D, Allred SB (2017) United States agricultural stakeholder views and decisions on climate change. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Clim 8(5):e469 Chatrchyan AM, Erlebacher RC, Chaopricha NT, Chan J, Tobin D, Allred SB (2017) United States agricultural stakeholder views and decisions on climate change. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Clim 8(5):e469
go back to reference Chen MF (2020a) Effects of psychological distance perception and psychological factors on pro-environmental behaviors in Taiwan: application of construal level theory. Int J Socio 35(1):70–89CrossRef Chen MF (2020a) Effects of psychological distance perception and psychological factors on pro-environmental behaviors in Taiwan: application of construal level theory. Int J Socio 35(1):70–89CrossRef
go back to reference Chen MF (2020b) Moral extension of the protection motivation theory model to predict climate change mitigation behavioral intentions in Taiwan. Environ Sci Pollut Res 27(12):13714–13725CrossRef Chen MF (2020b) Moral extension of the protection motivation theory model to predict climate change mitigation behavioral intentions in Taiwan. Environ Sci Pollut Res 27(12):13714–13725CrossRef
go back to reference Chryssochoidis G, Strada A, Krystallis A (2009) Public trust in institutions and information sources regarding risk management and communication: Towards integrating extant knowledge. J Risk Res 12(2):137–185CrossRef Chryssochoidis G, Strada A, Krystallis A (2009) Public trust in institutions and information sources regarding risk management and communication: Towards integrating extant knowledge. J Risk Res 12(2):137–185CrossRef
go back to reference Cologna V, Siegrist M (2020) The role of trust for climate change mitigation and adaptation behaviour: a meta-analysis. J Environ Psychol 69:101428CrossRef Cologna V, Siegrist M (2020) The role of trust for climate change mitigation and adaptation behaviour: a meta-analysis. J Environ Psychol 69:101428CrossRef
go back to reference Demski C, Capstick S, Pidgeon N, Sposato RG, Spence A (2017) Experience of extreme weather affects climate change mitigation and adaptation responses. Clim Change 140(2):149–164CrossRef Demski C, Capstick S, Pidgeon N, Sposato RG, Spence A (2017) Experience of extreme weather affects climate change mitigation and adaptation responses. Clim Change 140(2):149–164CrossRef
go back to reference Dhillon RS, von Wuehlisch G (2013) Mitigation of global warming through renewable biomass. Biomass Bioenerg 48:75–89CrossRef Dhillon RS, von Wuehlisch G (2013) Mitigation of global warming through renewable biomass. Biomass Bioenerg 48:75–89CrossRef
go back to reference Drummond A, Palmer MA (2014) Heart rate change and attitudes to global warming: A conceptual replication of the visceral fit mechanism. J Environ Psychol 38:10–16CrossRef Drummond A, Palmer MA (2014) Heart rate change and attitudes to global warming: A conceptual replication of the visceral fit mechanism. J Environ Psychol 38:10–16CrossRef
go back to reference Dunlap RE (2008) The new environmental paradigm scale: from marginality to worldwide use. J Environ Educ 40(1):3–18CrossRef Dunlap RE (2008) The new environmental paradigm scale: from marginality to worldwide use. J Environ Educ 40(1):3–18CrossRef
go back to reference Dunlap RE, Van Liere KD, Mertig AG, Jones RE (2000) New trends in measuring environmental attitudes: measuring endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: a revised NEP scale. J Soc Issues 56(3):425–442CrossRef Dunlap RE, Van Liere KD, Mertig AG, Jones RE (2000) New trends in measuring environmental attitudes: measuring endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: a revised NEP scale. J Soc Issues 56(3):425–442CrossRef
go back to reference Elrys AS, Raza S, Elnahal AS, Na M, Ahmed M, Zhou J, Chen Z (2020) Do soil property variations affect dicyandiamide efficiency in inhibiting nitrification and minimizing carbon dioxide emissions? Ecotoxicol Environ Saf 202:110875CrossRef Elrys AS, Raza S, Elnahal AS, Na M, Ahmed M, Zhou J, Chen Z (2020) Do soil property variations affect dicyandiamide efficiency in inhibiting nitrification and minimizing carbon dioxide emissions? Ecotoxicol Environ Saf 202:110875CrossRef
go back to reference Euriga E, Boehme MH, Amanah S (2021) Changing farmers’ perception towards sustainable horticulture: a case study of extension education in farming community in Yogyakarta, Indonesia. AGRARIS: J Agribus Rural Dev Res 7(2):225–240 Euriga E, Boehme MH, Amanah S (2021) Changing farmers’ perception towards sustainable horticulture: a case study of extension education in farming community in Yogyakarta, Indonesia. AGRARIS: J Agribus Rural Dev Res 7(2):225–240
go back to reference Fan Y, Chen J, Shirkey G, John R, Wu SR, Park H, Shao C (2016) Applications of structural equation modeling (SEM) in ecological studies: an updated review. Ecol Process 5:1–12.CrossRef Fan Y, Chen J, Shirkey G, John R, Wu SR, Park H, Shao C (2016) Applications of structural equation modeling (SEM) in ecological studies: an updated review. Ecol Process 5:1–12.CrossRef
go back to reference Fawzy S, Osman AI, Doran J, Rooney DW (2020) Strategies for mitigation of climate change: a review. Environ Chem Lett 18(6):2069–2094CrossRef Fawzy S, Osman AI, Doran J, Rooney DW (2020) Strategies for mitigation of climate change: a review. Environ Chem Lett 18(6):2069–2094CrossRef
go back to reference Ferguson MA, Branscombe NR (2010) Collective guilt mediates the effect of beliefs about global warming on willingness to engage in mitigation behavior. J Environ Psychol 30(2):135–142CrossRef Ferguson MA, Branscombe NR (2010) Collective guilt mediates the effect of beliefs about global warming on willingness to engage in mitigation behavior. J Environ Psychol 30(2):135–142CrossRef
go back to reference Fornell C, Larcker DF (1981) Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J Mark Res 18(1):39–50CrossRef Fornell C, Larcker DF (1981) Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J Mark Res 18(1):39–50CrossRef
go back to reference Gautam YB, Pelkonen P, Halder P (2013) Perceptions of bioenergy among Nepalese foresters–Survey results and policy implications. Renew. Energy 57:533–538 Gautam YB, Pelkonen P, Halder P (2013) Perceptions of bioenergy among Nepalese foresters–Survey results and policy implications. Renew. Energy 57:533–538
go back to reference Gezie M (2019) Farmer’s response to climate change and variability in Ethiopia: a review. Cogent Food Agric 5(1):1613770CrossRef Gezie M (2019) Farmer’s response to climate change and variability in Ethiopia: a review. Cogent Food Agric 5(1):1613770CrossRef
go back to reference Ghasemi S, Karami E, Azadi H (2013) Knowledge, attitudes and behavioral intentions of agricultural professionals toward genetically modified (GM) foods: a case study in Southwest Iran. Sci Eng Ethics 19(3):1201–1227CrossRef Ghasemi S, Karami E, Azadi H (2013) Knowledge, attitudes and behavioral intentions of agricultural professionals toward genetically modified (GM) foods: a case study in Southwest Iran. Sci Eng Ethics 19(3):1201–1227CrossRef
go back to reference Gifford R (2011) The dragons of inaction: psychological barriers that limit climate change mitigation and adaptation. Am Psychol 66(4):290CrossRef Gifford R (2011) The dragons of inaction: psychological barriers that limit climate change mitigation and adaptation. Am Psychol 66(4):290CrossRef
go back to reference Gu J, Yuan M, Liu J, Hao Y, Zhou Y, Qu D, Yang X (2017) Trade-off between soil organic carbon sequestration and nitrous oxide emissions from winter wheat-summer maize rotations: Implications of a 25-year fertilization experiment in Northwestern China. Sci Total Environ 595:371–379CrossRef Gu J, Yuan M, Liu J, Hao Y, Zhou Y, Qu D, Yang X (2017) Trade-off between soil organic carbon sequestration and nitrous oxide emissions from winter wheat-summer maize rotations: Implications of a 25-year fertilization experiment in Northwestern China. Sci Total Environ 595:371–379CrossRef
go back to reference Hair JFJ, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE (2010) Multivariate data analysis, 7th edn. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ Hair JFJ, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE (2010) Multivariate data analysis, 7th edn. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ
go back to reference Hamid F, Yazdanpanah M, Baradaran M, Khalilimoghadam B, Azadi H (2021) Factors affecting farmers’ behavior in using nitrogen fertilizers: society vs. farmers’ valuation in southwest Iran. J Environ Plan Manag 64(10):1886–1908CrossRef Hamid F, Yazdanpanah M, Baradaran M, Khalilimoghadam B, Azadi H (2021) Factors affecting farmers’ behavior in using nitrogen fertilizers: society vs. farmers’ valuation in southwest Iran. J Environ Plan Manag 64(10):1886–1908CrossRef
go back to reference Heath Y, Gifford R (2006) Free-market ideology and environmental degradation: The case of belief in global climate change. Environ Behav 38(1):48–71CrossRef Heath Y, Gifford R (2006) Free-market ideology and environmental degradation: The case of belief in global climate change. Environ Behav 38(1):48–71CrossRef
go back to reference Hmielowski JD, Feldman L, Myers TA, Leiserowitz A, Maibach E (2014) An attack on science? Media use, trust in scientists, and perceptions of global warming. Public Underst Sci 23(7):866–883CrossRef Hmielowski JD, Feldman L, Myers TA, Leiserowitz A, Maibach E (2014) An attack on science? Media use, trust in scientists, and perceptions of global warming. Public Underst Sci 23(7):866–883CrossRef
go back to reference Ho FJ, Lin YJ, Lai WL (2016) Exploration of human behavior of water-saving under climate change using expanded theory of planned behavior model. Int J Sci Technol Res 2(3):22–39 Ho FJ, Lin YJ, Lai WL (2016) Exploration of human behavior of water-saving under climate change using expanded theory of planned behavior model. Int J Sci Technol Res 2(3):22–39
go back to reference Honegger M, Burns W, Morrow DR (2021) Is carbon dioxide removal ‘mitigation of climate change’? Rev Eur Comp Int Environ Law 30(3):327–335CrossRef Honegger M, Burns W, Morrow DR (2021) Is carbon dioxide removal ‘mitigation of climate change’? Rev Eur Comp Int Environ Law 30(3):327–335CrossRef
go back to reference Hu S, Chen J (2016) Place-based inter-generational communication on local climate improves adolescents’ perceptions and willingness to mitigate climate change. Clim Change 138(3):425–438CrossRef Hu S, Chen J (2016) Place-based inter-generational communication on local climate improves adolescents’ perceptions and willingness to mitigate climate change. Clim Change 138(3):425–438CrossRef
go back to reference Kahan DM (2015) Climate‐science communication and the measurement problem. Polit Psychol 36:1–43CrossRef Kahan DM (2015) Climate‐science communication and the measurement problem. Polit Psychol 36:1–43CrossRef
go back to reference Karimi H, Ataei P (2022) Farmers’ cultural biases and adaptation behavior towards drought. J Agric Sci Technol 24(4):791–807 Karimi H, Ataei P (2022) Farmers’ cultural biases and adaptation behavior towards drought. J Agric Sci Technol 24(4):791–807
go back to reference Karppinen H (2005) Forest owners’ choice of reforestation method: an application of the theory of planned behavior. Policy Econ 7(3):393–409CrossRef Karppinen H (2005) Forest owners’ choice of reforestation method: an application of the theory of planned behavior. Policy Econ 7(3):393–409CrossRef
go back to reference Kellstedt PM, Zahran S, Vedlitz A (2008) Personal efficacy, the information environment, and attitudes toward global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk Anal 28(1):113–126CrossRef Kellstedt PM, Zahran S, Vedlitz A (2008) Personal efficacy, the information environment, and attitudes toward global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk Anal 28(1):113–126CrossRef
go back to reference Kollmuss A, Agyeman J (2002) Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ Educ Res 8(3):239–260CrossRef Kollmuss A, Agyeman J (2002) Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ Educ Res 8(3):239–260CrossRef
go back to reference Kreft C, Huber R, Wuepper D, Finger R (2021) The role of non-cognitive skills in farmers’ adoption of climate change mitigation measures. Ecol Econ 189:107169CrossRef Kreft C, Huber R, Wuepper D, Finger R (2021) The role of non-cognitive skills in farmers’ adoption of climate change mitigation measures. Ecol Econ 189:107169CrossRef
go back to reference Leiserowitz A (2006) Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: the role of affect, imagery, and values. Clim Change 77(1):45–72CrossRef Leiserowitz A (2006) Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: the role of affect, imagery, and values. Clim Change 77(1):45–72CrossRef
go back to reference Loboguerrero AM, Campbell BM, Cooper PJ, Hansen JW, Rosenstock T, Wollenberg E (2019) Food and earth systems: priorities for climate change adaptation and mitigation for agriculture and food systems. Sustain 11(5):1372CrossRef Loboguerrero AM, Campbell BM, Cooper PJ, Hansen JW, Rosenstock T, Wollenberg E (2019) Food and earth systems: priorities for climate change adaptation and mitigation for agriculture and food systems. Sustain 11(5):1372CrossRef
go back to reference Maiella R, La Malva P, Marchetti D, Pomarico E, Di Crosta A, Palumbo R,… Verrocchio MC (2020) The psychological distance and climate change: a systematic review on the mitigation and adaptation behaviors. Front Psychol 11, p. 568899 Maiella R, La Malva P, Marchetti D, Pomarico E, Di Crosta A, Palumbo R,… Verrocchio MC (2020) The psychological distance and climate change: a systematic review on the mitigation and adaptation behaviors. Front Psychol 11, p. 568899
go back to reference Malhi GS, Kaur M, Kaushik P (2021) Impact of climate change on agriculture and its mitigation strategies: a review. Sustain 13(3):1318CrossRef Malhi GS, Kaur M, Kaushik P (2021) Impact of climate change on agriculture and its mitigation strategies: a review. Sustain 13(3):1318CrossRef
go back to reference McDonald RI, Chai HY, Newell BR (2015) Personal experience and the ‘psychological distance’of climate change: an integrative review. J Environ Psychol 44:109–118CrossRef McDonald RI, Chai HY, Newell BR (2015) Personal experience and the ‘psychological distance’of climate change: an integrative review. J Environ Psychol 44:109–118CrossRef
go back to reference National Research Council (1992) Restoration of aquatic ecosystems: science, technology, and public policy. National Academies Press National Research Council (1992) Restoration of aquatic ecosystems: science, technology, and public policy. National Academies Press
go back to reference Niles MT, Brown M, Dynes R (2016) Farmer’s intended and actual adoption of climate change mitigation and adaptation strategies. Clim Change 135(2):277–295CrossRef Niles MT, Brown M, Dynes R (2016) Farmer’s intended and actual adoption of climate change mitigation and adaptation strategies. Clim Change 135(2):277–295CrossRef
go back to reference Nunnally JC (1978). An overview of psychological measurement. In: Wolman B (ed) Clinical diagnosis of mental disorders. Springer, Boston, MA, p 97–146 Nunnally JC (1978). An overview of psychological measurement. In: Wolman B (ed) Clinical diagnosis of mental disorders. Springer, Boston, MA, p 97–146
go back to reference O’Connor RE, Bord RJ, Yarnal B, Wiefek N (2002) Who wants to reduce greenhouse gas emissions? Soc Sci Q 83(1):1–17CrossRef O’Connor RE, Bord RJ, Yarnal B, Wiefek N (2002) Who wants to reduce greenhouse gas emissions? Soc Sci Q 83(1):1–17CrossRef
go back to reference Ogunbode CA, Böhm G, Capstick SB, Demski C, Spence A, Tausch N (2019) The resilience paradox: flooding experience, coping and climate change mitigation intentions. Clim Policy 19(6):703–715CrossRef Ogunbode CA, Böhm G, Capstick SB, Demski C, Spence A, Tausch N (2019) The resilience paradox: flooding experience, coping and climate change mitigation intentions. Clim Policy 19(6):703–715CrossRef
go back to reference Parry ML (Ed.) (2007) Climate Change 2007: impacts, adaptation and vulnerability: contribution of Working Group II to the fourth assessment report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (Vol. 4). Cambridge University Press Parry ML (Ed.) (2007) Climate Change 2007: impacts, adaptation and vulnerability: contribution of Working Group II to the fourth assessment report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (Vol. 4). Cambridge University Press
go back to reference Pradisty NA, Amir AA, Zimmer M (2021) Plant species-and stage-specific differences in microbial decay of mangrove leaf litter: the older the better? Oecologia 195(4):843–858CrossRef Pradisty NA, Amir AA, Zimmer M (2021) Plant species-and stage-specific differences in microbial decay of mangrove leaf litter: the older the better? Oecologia 195(4):843–858CrossRef
go back to reference Praveen B, Sharma P (2019) A review of literature on climate change and its impacts on agriculture productivity. J Public Aff 19(4):e1960CrossRef Praveen B, Sharma P (2019) A review of literature on climate change and its impacts on agriculture productivity. J Public Aff 19(4):e1960CrossRef
go back to reference Rodríguez-Barreiro LM, Fernández-Manzanal R, Serra LM, Carrasquer J, Murillo MB, Morales MJ, del Valle J (2013) Approach to a causal model between attitudes and environmental behaviour. A graduate case study. J Clean Prod 48:116–125CrossRef Rodríguez-Barreiro LM, Fernández-Manzanal R, Serra LM, Carrasquer J, Murillo MB, Morales MJ, del Valle J (2013) Approach to a causal model between attitudes and environmental behaviour. A graduate case study. J Clean Prod 48:116–125CrossRef
go back to reference Sanz-Cobena A, Lassaletta L, Aguilera E, del Prado A, Garnier J, Billen G, Smith P (2017) Strategies for greenhouse gas emissions mitigation in Mediterranean agriculture: a review. Agric Ecosyst Environ 238:5–24CrossRef Sanz-Cobena A, Lassaletta L, Aguilera E, del Prado A, Garnier J, Billen G, Smith P (2017) Strategies for greenhouse gas emissions mitigation in Mediterranean agriculture: a review. Agric Ecosyst Environ 238:5–24CrossRef
go back to reference Sarrasin O, Crettaz von Roten F, Butera F (2022) Who’s to act? Perceptions of intergenerational obligation and pro-environmental behaviours among youth. Sustain 14(3):1414CrossRef Sarrasin O, Crettaz von Roten F, Butera F (2022) Who’s to act? Perceptions of intergenerational obligation and pro-environmental behaviours among youth. Sustain 14(3):1414CrossRef
go back to reference Schuldt JP, Rickard LN, Yang ZJ (2018) Does reduced psychological distance increase climate engagement? On the limits of localizing climate change. J Environ Psychol 55:147–153CrossRef Schuldt JP, Rickard LN, Yang ZJ (2018) Does reduced psychological distance increase climate engagement? On the limits of localizing climate change. J Environ Psychol 55:147–153CrossRef
go back to reference Semenza JC, Hall DE, Wilson DJ, Bontempo BD, Sailor DJ, George LA (2008) Public perception of climate change: voluntary mitigation and barriers to behavior change. Am J Prev Med 35(5):479–487CrossRef Semenza JC, Hall DE, Wilson DJ, Bontempo BD, Sailor DJ, George LA (2008) Public perception of climate change: voluntary mitigation and barriers to behavior change. Am J Prev Med 35(5):479–487CrossRef
go back to reference Sinatra GM, Kardash CM, Taasoobshirazi G, Lombardi D (2012) Promoting attitude change and expressed willingness to take action toward climate change in college students. Instr Sci 40(1):1–17CrossRef Sinatra GM, Kardash CM, Taasoobshirazi G, Lombardi D (2012) Promoting attitude change and expressed willingness to take action toward climate change in college students. Instr Sci 40(1):1–17CrossRef
go back to reference Sohoo I, Ritzkowski M, Kuchta K, Cinar SÖ (2020) Environmental sustainability enhancement of waste disposal sites in developing countries through controlling greenhouse gas emissions. Sustain 13(1):151CrossRef Sohoo I, Ritzkowski M, Kuchta K, Cinar SÖ (2020) Environmental sustainability enhancement of waste disposal sites in developing countries through controlling greenhouse gas emissions. Sustain 13(1):151CrossRef
go back to reference Spence A, Poortinga W, Pidgeon N (2012) The psychological distance of climate change. Risk Anal 32(6):957–972CrossRef Spence A, Poortinga W, Pidgeon N (2012) The psychological distance of climate change. Risk Anal 32(6):957–972CrossRef
go back to reference Spence A, Poortinga W, Butler C, Pidgeon NF (2011) Perceptions of climate change and willingness to save energy related to flood experience. Nat Clim Change 1(1):46–49CrossRef Spence A, Poortinga W, Butler C, Pidgeon NF (2011) Perceptions of climate change and willingness to save energy related to flood experience. Nat Clim Change 1(1):46–49CrossRef
go back to reference Stoutenborough JW, Vedlitz A (2014) The effect of perceived and assessed knowledge of climate change on public policy concerns: An empirical comparison. Environ Sci Policy 37:23–33CrossRef Stoutenborough JW, Vedlitz A (2014) The effect of perceived and assessed knowledge of climate change on public policy concerns: An empirical comparison. Environ Sci Policy 37:23–33CrossRef
go back to reference Suckall N, Tompkins E, Stringer L (2014) Identifying trade-offs between adaptation, mitigation and development in community responses to climate and socio-economic stresses: evidence from Zanzibar, Tanzania. Appl Geogr 46:111–121CrossRef Suckall N, Tompkins E, Stringer L (2014) Identifying trade-offs between adaptation, mitigation and development in community responses to climate and socio-economic stresses: evidence from Zanzibar, Tanzania. Appl Geogr 46:111–121CrossRef
go back to reference Venkateswarlu B, Shanker AK (2009) Climate change and agriculture: adaptation and mitigation stategies. Indian J Agron 54(2):226–230 Venkateswarlu B, Shanker AK (2009) Climate change and agriculture: adaptation and mitigation stategies. Indian J Agron 54(2):226–230
go back to reference Wang F, Harindintwali JD, Yuan Z, Wang M, Wang F, Li S, Chen JM (2021) Technologies and perspectives for achieving carbon neutrality. Innov 2(4):100180 Wang F, Harindintwali JD, Yuan Z, Wang M, Wang F, Li S, Chen JM (2021) Technologies and perspectives for achieving carbon neutrality. Innov 2(4):100180
go back to reference Wang L, Zhang Q, Wong PPW (2022) Purchase intention for green cars among Chinese millennials: merging the value–attitude–behavior theory and theory of planned behavior. Front Psychol 13:316 Wang L, Zhang Q, Wong PPW (2022) Purchase intention for green cars among Chinese millennials: merging the value–attitude–behavior theory and theory of planned behavior. Front Psychol 13:316
go back to reference Weber EU (2010) What shapes perceptions of climate change? Wiley Interdiscip Rev Clim Change 1(3):332–342CrossRef Weber EU (2010) What shapes perceptions of climate change? Wiley Interdiscip Rev Clim Change 1(3):332–342CrossRef
go back to reference Wheeler SA (2008) What influences agricultural professionals’ views towards organic agriculture? Ecol Econ 65(1):145–154CrossRef Wheeler SA (2008) What influences agricultural professionals’ views towards organic agriculture? Ecol Econ 65(1):145–154CrossRef
go back to reference Whitmarsh L (2008) Are flood victims more concerned about climate change than other people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and behavioural response. J Risk Res 11(3):351–374CrossRef Whitmarsh L (2008) Are flood victims more concerned about climate change than other people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and behavioural response. J Risk Res 11(3):351–374CrossRef
go back to reference Whitmarsh L (2009) Behavioural responses to climate change: asymmetry of intentions and impacts. J Environ Psychol 29(1):13–23CrossRef Whitmarsh L (2009) Behavioural responses to climate change: asymmetry of intentions and impacts. J Environ Psychol 29(1):13–23CrossRef
go back to reference Wibeck V (2014) Enhancing learning, communication and public engagement about climate change–some lessons from recent literature. Environ Educ Res 20(3):387–411CrossRef Wibeck V (2014) Enhancing learning, communication and public engagement about climate change–some lessons from recent literature. Environ Educ Res 20(3):387–411CrossRef
go back to reference Yaghoubi J, Yazdanpanah M, Komendantova N (2019) Iranian agriculture advisors’ perception and intention toward biofuel: Green way toward energy security, rural development and climate change mitigation. Renew Energ 130:452–459CrossRef Yaghoubi J, Yazdanpanah M, Komendantova N (2019) Iranian agriculture advisors’ perception and intention toward biofuel: Green way toward energy security, rural development and climate change mitigation. Renew Energ 130:452–459CrossRef
go back to reference Yazdanpanah M, Komendantova N, Zobeidi T (2022a) Explaining intention to apply renewable energy in agriculture: the case of broiler farms in Southwest Iran. Int J Green Energy 19(8):836–846CrossRef Yazdanpanah M, Komendantova N, Zobeidi T (2022a) Explaining intention to apply renewable energy in agriculture: the case of broiler farms in Southwest Iran. Int J Green Energy 19(8):836–846CrossRef
go back to reference Yu TK, Lin FY, Kao KY, Chao CM, Yu TY (2019) An innovative environmental citizen behavior model: Recycling intention as climate change mitigation strategies. J Environ Manag 247:499–508CrossRef Yu TK, Lin FY, Kao KY, Chao CM, Yu TY (2019) An innovative environmental citizen behavior model: Recycling intention as climate change mitigation strategies. J Environ Manag 247:499–508CrossRef
go back to reference Zhang L, Ruiz-Menjivar J, Luo B, Liang Z, Swisher ME (2020) Predicting climate change mitigation and adaptation behaviors in agricultural production: A comparison of the theory of planned behavior and the Value-Belief-Norm Theory. J Environ Psychol 68:101408CrossRef Zhang L, Ruiz-Menjivar J, Luo B, Liang Z, Swisher ME (2020) Predicting climate change mitigation and adaptation behaviors in agricultural production: A comparison of the theory of planned behavior and the Value-Belief-Norm Theory. J Environ Psychol 68:101408CrossRef
go back to reference Ziegler A (2021) New Ecological Paradigm meets behavioral economics: On the relationship between environmental values and economic preferences. J Environ Econ Manag 109:102516CrossRef Ziegler A (2021) New Ecological Paradigm meets behavioral economics: On the relationship between environmental values and economic preferences. J Environ Econ Manag 109:102516CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Personal and Professional Mitigation Behavioral Intentions of Agricultural Experts to Address Climate Change
Authors
Tahereh Zobeidi
Masoud Yazdanpanah
Laura A. Warner
Alexa Lamm
Katharina Löhr
Stefan Sieber
Publication date
03-04-2023
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Environmental Management / Issue 2/2023
Print ISSN: 0364-152X
Electronic ISSN: 1432-1009
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00267-023-01815-y

Other articles of this Issue 2/2023

Environmental Management 2/2023 Go to the issue