Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Learning & Behavior 4/2016

29-04-2016

What is timed in a fixed-interval temporal bisection procedure?

Authors: Adam E. Fox, Katelyn E. Prue, Elizabeth G. E. Kyonka

Published in: Learning & Behavior | Issue 4/2016

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Recent research on interval timing in the behavioral and neurological sciences has employed a concurrent fixed-interval (FI) procedure first reported by Platt and Davis (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 160-170, 1983). Studies employing the task typically assess just 1 dependent variable, the switch/bisection point; however, multiple measures of timing are available in the procedure and it is unclear (a) what is timed (i.e., learned) by subjects and (b) what other measures might tell us about timing in the task and generally. The main objective of the current experiment was to utilize multiple dependent measures of timing accuracy and precision derived from the task to assess whether the 2 FIs are timed independently or if timing 1 FI interferes with timing the other, and vice versa. Four pigeons were exposed to an FI temporal bisection procedure with parametric manipulations across two phases. In the constant phase, the short FI was always the same; the long FI was 2 to 16 times the short FI and changed across conditions. In the proportional phase, the long FI was always 4 times the duration of the short FI. Across both phases, pigeon mean bisection points were near the geometric mean of the 2 FIs. Coefficients of variance increased as the durations to be timed increased. Results suggested pigeons’ timing of the short FI was affected by the presence of the long FI, and vice versa. The FI temporal bisection task offers multiple dependent variables for analysis and is well suited for studying temporal learning and decision making.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Literature
go back to reference Allan, L. G., & Gibbon, J. (1991). Human bisection at the geometric mean. Learning and Motivation, 22, 39–58.CrossRef Allan, L. G., & Gibbon, J. (1991). Human bisection at the geometric mean. Learning and Motivation, 22, 39–58.CrossRef
go back to reference Balci, F., Freestone, D., & Gallistel, C. R. (2009). Risk assessment in man and mouse. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 2459–2463.CrossRef Balci, F., Freestone, D., & Gallistel, C. R. (2009). Risk assessment in man and mouse. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 2459–2463.CrossRef
go back to reference Balci, F., Papchristos, E. B., Gallistel, C. R., Brunner, D., Gibson, J., & Shumyatsky, G. P. (2008). Interval timing in genetically modified mice: A simple paradigm. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 7, 373–384.CrossRef Balci, F., Papchristos, E. B., Gallistel, C. R., Brunner, D., Gibson, J., & Shumyatsky, G. P. (2008). Interval timing in genetically modified mice: A simple paradigm. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 7, 373–384.CrossRef
go back to reference Bizo, L. A., & White, G. (1994). Pacemaker rate in the behavioral theory of timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 308–321. Bizo, L. A., & White, G. (1994). Pacemaker rate in the behavioral theory of timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 308–321.
go back to reference Bizo, L. A., & White, G. (1995). Reinforcement context and pacemaker rate in the behavioral theory of timing. Animal Learning & Behavior, 23, 376–382.CrossRef Bizo, L. A., & White, G. (1995). Reinforcement context and pacemaker rate in the behavioral theory of timing. Animal Learning & Behavior, 23, 376–382.CrossRef
go back to reference Catania, A. C. (1970). Reinforcement schedules and pscyho-physical judgments: A study of some temporal properties of behavior. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules (pp. ). New York: Appleton-Century-Croft. Catania, A. C. (1970). Reinforcement schedules and pscyho-physical judgments: A study of some temporal properties of behavior. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules (pp. ). New York: Appleton-Century-Croft.
go back to reference Cheung, T. H. C., Bezzina, G., Hampson, C. L., Body, S., Fone, K. C. F., Bradshaw, C. M., & Szabadi, E. (2007). Evidence for the sensitivity of operant timing behaviour to stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors. Psychopharmacology, 195, 213–222.CrossRefPubMed Cheung, T. H. C., Bezzina, G., Hampson, C. L., Body, S., Fone, K. C. F., Bradshaw, C. M., & Szabadi, E. (2007). Evidence for the sensitivity of operant timing behaviour to stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors. Psychopharmacology, 195, 213–222.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Church, R. M., & Deluty, M. Z. (1977). Bisection of temporal intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3, 216–228.PubMed Church, R. M., & Deluty, M. Z. (1977). Bisection of temporal intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3, 216–228.PubMed
go back to reference Cordes, S., & Meck, W. H. (2014). Ordinal judgments in the rat: An understanding of longer and shorter for suprasecond, but not subsecond, durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General., 143, 710–720.CrossRef Cordes, S., & Meck, W. H. (2014). Ordinal judgments in the rat: An understanding of longer and shorter for suprasecond, but not subsecond, durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General., 143, 710–720.CrossRef
go back to reference Crystal, J. D. (1999). Systematic nonlinearities in the perception of temporal intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 25, 3–17.PubMed Crystal, J. D. (1999). Systematic nonlinearities in the perception of temporal intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 25, 3–17.PubMed
go back to reference Daniels, C. W., Watterson, E., Garcia, R., Mazur, G. J., Brackney, R. J., & Sanabria, F. (2015b). Revisiting the effect of nicotine on interval timing. Behavioural Brain Research, 283, 238–250.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Daniels, C. W., Watterson, E., Garcia, R., Mazur, G. J., Brackney, R. J., & Sanabria, F. (2015b). Revisiting the effect of nicotine on interval timing. Behavioural Brain Research, 283, 238–250.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Dews, P. B. (1970). The theory of fixed-interval responding. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules (pp. 43-62). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Dews, P. B. (1970). The theory of fixed-interval responding. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules (pp. 43-62). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
go back to reference Fetterman, J. G., & Killeen, P. (1995). Categorical scaling of time: Implications for clock-counter models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 43–63.PubMed Fetterman, J. G., & Killeen, P. (1995). Categorical scaling of time: Implications for clock-counter models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 43–63.PubMed
go back to reference Fox, A. E., Prue, K. E., & Kyonka, E. G. E. (2016). Human timing in a fixed-interval temporal bisection procedure. Manuscript submitted for publication. Fox, A. E., Prue, K. E., & Kyonka, E. G. E. (2016). Human timing in a fixed-interval temporal bisection procedure. Manuscript submitted for publication.
go back to reference Gallistel, C. R., Tucci, V., Nolan, P. M., Schachner, M., Jakovcevski, I., Kheifets, A., & Barboza, L. (2014). Cognitive assessment of mouse strains heterozygous for cell-adhesion genes reveals strain-specific alterations in timing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 369. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0464 Gallistel, C. R., Tucci, V., Nolan, P. M., Schachner, M., Jakovcevski, I., Kheifets, A., & Barboza, L. (2014). Cognitive assessment of mouse strains heterozygous for cell-adhesion genes reveals strain-specific alterations in timing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 369. doi:10.​1098/​rstb.​2012.​0464
go back to reference Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279–325.CrossRef Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279–325.CrossRef
go back to reference Guilhardi, P., & Church, R. M. (2005). Dynamics of temporal discrimination. Learning & Behavior, 33, 399–416.CrossRef Guilhardi, P., & Church, R. M. (2005). Dynamics of temporal discrimination. Learning & Behavior, 33, 399–416.CrossRef
go back to reference Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (2006). Scalar properties in animal timing: Conformity and violations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1875–1908.CrossRef Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (2006). Scalar properties in animal timing: Conformity and violations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1875–1908.CrossRef
go back to reference Machado, A., & Pata, P. (2005). Testing the scalar expectancy theory (SET) and the learning-to-time model (LeT) in a double bisection task. Learning & Behavior, 33, 111–222.CrossRef Machado, A., & Pata, P. (2005). Testing the scalar expectancy theory (SET) and the learning-to-time model (LeT) in a double bisection task. Learning & Behavior, 33, 111–222.CrossRef
go back to reference Maggi, S., Garbugino, L., Heise, I., Nieus, T., Balci, F., Wells, S., . . . Tucci, V. (2014). A cross-laboratory investigation of timing endophenotypes in mouse behavior. Timing & Time Perception, 2, 35–50. Maggi, S., Garbugino, L., Heise, I., Nieus, T., Balci, F., Wells, S., . . . Tucci, V. (2014). A cross-laboratory investigation of timing endophenotypes in mouse behavior. Timing & Time Perception, 2, 35–50.
go back to reference Pinheiro de Carvalho, M., Machado, A., & Tonneau, F. (2016). Learning in the temporal bisection task: Relative or absolute? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 42, 67–81. Pinheiro de Carvalho, M., Machado, A., & Tonneau, F. (2016). Learning in the temporal bisection task: Relative or absolute? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 42, 67–81.
go back to reference Platt, J. R., & Davis, E. R. (1983). Bisection of temporal intervals by pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 160–170.PubMed Platt, J. R., & Davis, E. R. (1983). Bisection of temporal intervals by pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 160–170.PubMed
go back to reference Roberts, S. (1981). Isolation of an internal clock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 242–268.PubMed Roberts, S. (1981). Isolation of an internal clock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 242–268.PubMed
go back to reference Sargisson, R. J., Lockhart, R. A., McEwan, J. S., & Bizo, L. A. (2016). Demonstration of the scalar property of timing with possums (Trichosurus vulpecula). Journal of Comparative Psychology. doi:10.1037/com0000022 PubMed Sargisson, R. J., Lockhart, R. A., McEwan, J. S., & Bizo, L. A. (2016). Demonstration of the scalar property of timing with possums (Trichosurus vulpecula). Journal of Comparative Psychology. doi:10.​1037/​com0000022 PubMed
go back to reference Schneider, B. A. (1969). A two-state analysis of fixed-interval responding in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 677-687. doi:10.1901/jeab.1969.12-677. Schneider, B. A. (1969). A two-state analysis of fixed-interval responding in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 677-687. doi:10.1901/jeab.1969.12-677.
go back to reference Shull, R. L., Gaynor, S. T., & Grimes, J. A. (2001). Response rate viewed as engagement bouts: Effects of relative reinforcement and schedule type. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 247–274.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Shull, R. L., Gaynor, S. T., & Grimes, J. A. (2001). Response rate viewed as engagement bouts: Effects of relative reinforcement and schedule type. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 247–274.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Staddon, J. E. R., & Innis, N. K. (1969). Reinforcement omission on fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 689–700.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Staddon, J. E. R., & Innis, N. K. (1969). Reinforcement omission on fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 689–700.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Stubbs, D. A. (1980). Temporal discrimination and a free-operant psychophysical procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 167–185.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Stubbs, D. A. (1980). Temporal discrimination and a free-operant psychophysical procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 167–185.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Tosun, T., Gur, E., & Balci, F. (2016). Mice plan decision strategies based on previously learned time intervals, locations, and probabilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 787–792.CrossRef Tosun, T., Gur, E., & Balci, F. (2016). Mice plan decision strategies based on previously learned time intervals, locations, and probabilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 787–792.CrossRef
go back to reference Wearden, J. H. (1991). Human performance on an analogue of an interval bisection task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B, 59–81. Wearden, J. H. (1991). Human performance on an analogue of an interval bisection task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B, 59–81.
go back to reference Wearden, J. H. (1995). Categorical scaling of stimulus duration by humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 318–330.PubMed Wearden, J. H. (1995). Categorical scaling of stimulus duration by humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 318–330.PubMed
go back to reference Wearden, J. H., & Ferrara, A. (1995). Stimulus spacing effects in temporal bisection by humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48B, 289–310. Wearden, J. H., & Ferrara, A. (1995). Stimulus spacing effects in temporal bisection by humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48B, 289–310.
Metadata
Title
What is timed in a fixed-interval temporal bisection procedure?
Authors
Adam E. Fox
Katelyn E. Prue
Elizabeth G. E. Kyonka
Publication date
29-04-2016
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Learning & Behavior / Issue 4/2016
Print ISSN: 1543-4494
Electronic ISSN: 1543-4508
DOI
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-016-0228-z

Other articles of this Issue 4/2016

Learning & Behavior 4/2016 Go to the issue

Premium Partner