Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Political Behavior 4/2020

23-03-2019 | Original Paper

Partisanship, Political Awareness, and Retrospective Evaluations, 1956–2016

Author: Philip Edward Jones

Published in: Political Behavior | Issue 4/2020

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

A long line of research shows that voters frequently evaluate objective conditions through a perceptual screen, seeing a stronger economy and more peaceful world when their party is in power. We know less about how and why these partisan perceptual differences have changed over recent history, however. This paper combines ANES measures of retrospective evaluations from 1956 to 2016 and shows that partisan differences (1) have increased significantly over the past few decades across all types of assessments; (2) are greatest, and have changed the most, amongst the most politically aware; and (3) closely track changes in elite polarization over this time period. The extent of partisan disagreement in retrospective evaluations is thus not constant, but rather contingent on attributes of the voter and the political context. Greater political awareness and more polarized politicians result in larger partisan perceptual differences, as the most engaged citizens are the most likely to receive and internalize cues about the state of the world from their party’s elites.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
I follow previous research (e.g., Bisgaard and Slothuus 2018) and use “partisan perceptual differences” to refer to differences between in- and out-partisans’ retrospective evaluations. Unlike the term “partisan bias”, it is purely descriptive, and agnostic on the exact mechanism producing such differences (see Gerber and Huber (2010) for more).
 
2
I use the term “political awareness” in the sense of Zaller (1992): “the extent to which an individual pays attention to politics and understands what he or she has encountered...Political awareness denotes intellectual or cognitive engagement with public affairs” (p. 21, italics in original). As Zaller notes, this is sometimes labelled sophistication, engagement, attention, and so on. Here, I understand these terms to essentially be synonymous.
 
3
A related question is how economic conditions influence voters’ assessments of the economy. Lewis-Beck et al. (2013), using similar data to this study, find that as the economy grows, evaluations become more positive. My interest here is in a different question, how economic conditions influence the magnitude of partisan differences in perceptions of the economy. I return to this distinction in the conclusion.
 
4
The ANES did not run a Time Series study in the midterm years of 2006, 2010, or 2014. Questions about retrospective evaluations first appear in the 1956 survey, and so I do not use the 1948 or 1952 studies.
 
5
Question wording and details about which year each item was asked in are included in Table A1 in the online appendix.
 
6
In some but not all years, the ANES followed up with questions about how much better or worse conditions had become. In order to maintain comparability across survey years, the results shown here do not utilize this follow-up question. Preliminary analysis using the full responses on five-point scales reached the same substantive conclusions as here.
 
7
The measures of income and education are treated as continuous variables ranging from 1–5 to 1–7 respectively. This assumes that the numerical distances between each category code represent equal increases in each underlying variable. This is a strong assumption; since these variables are used as controls here I adopt this approach for simplicity.
 
8
ANES measurement of race and ethnicity has changed significantly over the years. As such, I rely on the White indicator with all other races/ethnicities as the omitted category. This is obviously not ideal, and sacrifices a more accurate measure of respondents’ racial/ethnic identities in favor of comparability across years.
 
9
This timeframe was chosen for three reasons. First, the survey questions almost all ask for assessments of change over the prior year (see Table A1 in the online appendix). Second, previous studies use this measure (e.g., Lewis-Beck et al. 2013; Lavine et al. 2012), allowing for more comparability with the findings here. Finally, research suggests voters weigh the preceding year most heavily in their assessments (Healy and Lenz 2013).
 
10
Alternative measures of economic conditions used by Lewis-Beck et al. (2013)—the change in the Consumer Price Index, the value of the S&P 500 index, and the unemployment rate—produced substantively similar results as those reported here. Since Lewis-Beck et al. (2013, p. 526) report that change in GDP has the most robust effects on perceptions of the economy, I present those estimates here.
 
11
First differences between partisans and Independents could also be calculated, although the focus of this paper is on differences between identifiers of different parties. I leave it to future work to explore how Independents construct retrospective evaluations, but note that the analysis in the online appendix suggests Independents’ responses frequently resemble those of out-partisans.
 
12
The local weighted regression uses a span of 0.75, meaning that the nearest 3/4 of the data points are used to calculate the average for each year, with greater weight given to closer data points. See Cleveland et al. (1991).
 
13
The average awareness score is .56 but these minimum and maximum values are not particularly unusual: 6% of all respondents score a 0, and 9% a 1. Section A4 of the online appendix provides more descriptive information about the measure.
 
14
This is consistent across types of evaluations: among the least aware, there were significant partisan perceptual differences on 14% of pocketbook evaluations, 19% of sociotropic evaluations, and 8% of foreign affairs evaluations. Among the most aware, those percentages are 56%, 89%, and 88%, respectively.
 
15
This is not the full range in the dataset; polarization reached a high of 0.84 in 2016. However, the highest levels of polarization have occurred solely during divided government. Simulating for values of both terms simultaneously results in highly imprecise estimates at higher levels of polarization. As such, I limit the simulations to values of polarization of between 0.50 and 0.75, which is roughly equivalent to the increase in polarization between the 1950s and 2008.
 
16
First differences simulated from model 3 that assess how changes in economic context influence the least and most aware respondents separately can be found in Section A6 of the online appendix. They do not suggest that the conclusions about the economy’s lack of impact change when breaking the results down by awareness.
 
Literature
go back to reference Achen, C. H., & Bartels, L. M. (2016). Democracy for realists: Why elections do not produce responsive government. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Achen, C. H., & Bartels, L. M. (2016). Democracy for realists: Why elections do not produce responsive government. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
go back to reference Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117–150. Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117–150.
go back to reference Bisgaard, M. (2015). Bias will find a way: Economic perceptions, attributions of blame, and partisan-motivated reasoning during crisis. Journal of Politics, 77(3), 849–861. Bisgaard, M. (2015). Bias will find a way: Economic perceptions, attributions of blame, and partisan-motivated reasoning during crisis. Journal of Politics, 77(3), 849–861.
go back to reference Bisgaard, M., & Slothuus, R. (2018). Partisan elites as culprits? How party cues shape partisan perceptual gaps. American Journal of Political Science, 62(2), 456–469. Bisgaard, M., & Slothuus, R. (2018). Partisan elites as culprits? How party cues shape partisan perceptual gaps. American Journal of Political Science, 62(2), 456–469.
go back to reference Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Chzhen, K., Evans, G., & Pickup, M. (2014). When do economic perceptions matter for party approval? Political Behavior, 36(2), 291–313. Chzhen, K., Evans, G., & Pickup, M. (2014). When do economic perceptions matter for party approval? Political Behavior, 36(2), 291–313.
go back to reference Clarke, H. D., & McCutcheon, A. L. (2009). The dynamics of party identification reconsidered. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(4), 704–728. Clarke, H. D., & McCutcheon, A. L. (2009). The dynamics of party identification reconsidered. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(4), 704–728.
go back to reference Conover, P. J., Feldman, S., & Knight, K. (1987). The personal and political underpinnings of economic forecasts. American Journal of Political Science, 31(3), 559–83. Conover, P. J., Feldman, S., & Knight, K. (1987). The personal and political underpinnings of economic forecasts. American Journal of Political Science, 31(3), 559–83.
go back to reference Druckman, J. N., Peterson, E., & Slothuus, R. (2013). How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. American Political Science Review, 107(1), 57–79. Druckman, J. N., Peterson, E., & Slothuus, R. (2013). How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. American Political Science Review, 107(1), 57–79.
go back to reference Duch, R. M., Palmer, H. D., & Anderson, C. J. (2000). Heterogeneity in perceptions of national economic conditions. American Journal of Political Science, 44(4), 635–652. Duch, R. M., Palmer, H. D., & Anderson, C. J. (2000). Heterogeneity in perceptions of national economic conditions. American Journal of Political Science, 44(4), 635–652.
go back to reference Enns, P. K., & McAvoy, G. E. (2012). The role of partisanship in aggregate opinion. Political Behavior, 34, 637–651. Enns, P. K., & McAvoy, G. E. (2012). The role of partisanship in aggregate opinion. Political Behavior, 34, 637–651.
go back to reference Evans, G., & Andersen, R. (2006). The political conditioning of economic perceptions. Journal of Politics, 68(1), 194–207. Evans, G., & Andersen, R. (2006). The political conditioning of economic perceptions. Journal of Politics, 68(1), 194–207.
go back to reference Evans, G., & Pickup, M. (2010). Reversing the causal arrow: The political conditioning of economic perceptions in the 2000–2004 U.S. presidential election cycle. Journal of Politics, 72(4), 1236–1251. Evans, G., & Pickup, M. (2010). Reversing the causal arrow: The political conditioning of economic perceptions in the 2000–2004 U.S. presidential election cycle. Journal of Politics, 72(4), 1236–1251.
go back to reference Fiorina, M. P. (1981). Retrospective voting in American national elections. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Fiorina, M. P. (1981). Retrospective voting in American national elections. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Gaines, B. J., Kuklinski, J. H., Quirk, P. J., Peyton, B., & Verkuilen, J. (2007). Same facts, different interpretations: Partisan motivation and opinion on Iraq. Journal of Politics, 69(4), 957–974. Gaines, B. J., Kuklinski, J. H., Quirk, P. J., Peyton, B., & Verkuilen, J. (2007). Same facts, different interpretations: Partisan motivation and opinion on Iraq. Journal of Politics, 69(4), 957–974.
go back to reference Gerber, A. S., & Huber, G. A. (2010). Partisanship, political control, and economic assessments. American Journal of Political Science, 54(1), 153–173. Gerber, A. S., & Huber, G. A. (2010). Partisanship, political control, and economic assessments. American Journal of Political Science, 54(1), 153–173.
go back to reference Green, D., Bradley, P., & Eric, S. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds: Political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven: Yale University Press. Green, D., Bradley, P., & Eric, S. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds: Political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Healy, A., Kuo, A. G., & Malhotra, N. (2014). Partisan bias in blame attribution: When does it occur? Journal of Experimental Political Science, 1, 144–158. Healy, A., Kuo, A. G., & Malhotra, N. (2014). Partisan bias in blame attribution: When does it occur? Journal of Experimental Political Science, 1, 144–158.
go back to reference Healy, A., & Lenz, G. S. (2013). Substituting the end for the whole: Why voters respond primarily to the election year economy. American Journal of Political Science, 58(1), 34–47. Healy, A., & Lenz, G. S. (2013). Substituting the end for the whole: Why voters respond primarily to the election year economy. American Journal of Political Science, 58(1), 34–47.
go back to reference Healy, A., & Malhotra, N. (2013). Retrospective voting reconsidered. Annual Review of Political Science, 16, 285–306. Healy, A., & Malhotra, N. (2013). Retrospective voting reconsidered. Annual Review of Political Science, 16, 285–306.
go back to reference Jerit, J., & Barabas, J. (2012). Partisan perceptual bias and the information environment. Journal of Politics, 74(3), 672–684. Jerit, J., & Barabas, J. (2012). Partisan perceptual bias and the information environment. Journal of Politics, 74(3), 672–684.
go back to reference Joslyn, M. R., & Haider-Markel, D. P. (2014). Who knows best? Education, partisanship, and contested facts. Politics & Policy, 42(6), 919–947. Joslyn, M. R., & Haider-Markel, D. P. (2014). Who knows best? Education, partisanship, and contested facts. Politics & Policy, 42(6), 919–947.
go back to reference Key, V. O. (1966). The responsible electorate. New York: Vintage Books. Key, V. O. (1966). The responsible electorate. New York: Vintage Books.
go back to reference Kinder, D. R., & Roderick Kiewiet, D. (1981). Sociotropic politics: The American case. British Journal of Political Science, 11(2), 129–141. Kinder, D. R., & Roderick Kiewiet, D. (1981). Sociotropic politics: The American case. British Journal of Political Science, 11(2), 129–141.
go back to reference Lavine, H., Johnston, C. D., & Steenbergen, M. R. (2012). The ambivalent partisan: How critical loyalty promotes democracy. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Lavine, H., Johnston, C. D., & Steenbergen, M. R. (2012). The ambivalent partisan: How critical loyalty promotes democracy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Lenz, G. S. (2012). Follow the leader? How voters respond to politicians’ policies and performance. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Lenz, G. S. (2012). Follow the leader? How voters respond to politicians’ policies and performance. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Lewis-Beck, M. S., Martini, N. F., & Roderick Kiewiet, D. (2013). The nature of economic perceptions in mass publics. Electoral Studies, 32, 524–528. Lewis-Beck, M. S., Martini, N. F., & Roderick Kiewiet, D. (2013). The nature of economic perceptions in mass publics. Electoral Studies, 32, 524–528.
go back to reference Markus, G. B. (1988). The impact of personal and national economic conditions on the presidential vote: A pooled cross-sectional analysis. American Journal of Political Science, 32(1), 137–154. Markus, G. B. (1988). The impact of personal and national economic conditions on the presidential vote: A pooled cross-sectional analysis. American Journal of Political Science, 32(1), 137–154.
go back to reference Parker-Stephen, E. (2013). Tides of disagreement: How reality facilitates (and inhibits) partisan public opinion. Journal of Politics, 75(4), 1077–1088. Parker-Stephen, E. (2013). Tides of disagreement: How reality facilitates (and inhibits) partisan public opinion. Journal of Politics, 75(4), 1077–1088.
go back to reference Redlawsk, D. P. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. Journal of Politics, 64(4), 1021–1044. Redlawsk, D. P. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. Journal of Politics, 64(4), 1021–1044.
go back to reference Rudolph, T. J. (2003). Who’s responsible for the economy? The formation and consequences of responsibility attributions. American Journal of Political Science, 47(4), 698–713. Rudolph, T. J. (2003). Who’s responsible for the economy? The formation and consequences of responsibility attributions. American Journal of Political Science, 47(4), 698–713.
go back to reference Schaffner, B., & Roche, C. (2017). Misinformation and motivated reasoning: responses to economic news in a politicized environment. Public Opinion Quarterly, 81(1), 86–110. Schaffner, B., & Roche, C. (2017). Misinformation and motivated reasoning: responses to economic news in a politicized environment. Public Opinion Quarterly, 81(1), 86–110.
go back to reference Shyu, W. M., Grosse, E., & Cleveland, W. S. (2017). Local regression models. In Statistical models in S (pp. 309–376). Routledge. Shyu, W. M., Grosse, E., & Cleveland, W. S. (2017). Local regression models. In Statistical models in S (pp. 309–376). Routledge.
go back to reference Slothuus, R., & de Vreese, C. H. (2010). Political parties, motivated reasoning, and issue framing effects. Journal of Politics, 72(3), 630–645. Slothuus, R., & de Vreese, C. H. (2010). Political parties, motivated reasoning, and issue framing effects. Journal of Politics, 72(3), 630–645.
go back to reference Taber, C. S., Cann, D., & Kucsova, S. (2009). The motivated processing of political arguments. Political Behavior, 31(2), 137–155. Taber, C. S., Cann, D., & Kucsova, S. (2009). The motivated processing of political arguments. Political Behavior, 31(2), 137–155.
go back to reference Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769. Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.
go back to reference Tilley, J., & Hobolt, S. B. (2011). Is the government to blame? An experimental test of how partisanship shapes perceptions of performance and responsibility. Journal of Politics, 73(2), 316–330. Tilley, J., & Hobolt, S. B. (2011). Is the government to blame? An experimental test of how partisanship shapes perceptions of performance and responsibility. Journal of Politics, 73(2), 316–330.
go back to reference Weinschenk, A. C. (2012). Partisan pocketbooks: The politics of personal financial evaluations. Social Science Quarterly, 93(4), 968–987. Weinschenk, A. C. (2012). Partisan pocketbooks: The politics of personal financial evaluations. Social Science Quarterly, 93(4), 968–987.
go back to reference Wells, C., Reedy, J., Gastil, J., & Lee, C. (2009). Information distortion and voting choices: The origins and effects of factual beliefs in initiative elections. Political Psychology, 30(6), 953–969. Wells, C., Reedy, J., Gastil, J., & Lee, C. (2009). Information distortion and voting choices: The origins and effects of factual beliefs in initiative elections. Political Psychology, 30(6), 953–969.
go back to reference Wlezien, C., Franklin, M., & Twiggs, D. (1997). Economic perceptions and vote choice: Disentangling the endogeneity. Political Behavior, 19(1), 7–17. Wlezien, C., Franklin, M., & Twiggs, D. (1997). Economic perceptions and vote choice: Disentangling the endogeneity. Political Behavior, 19(1), 7–17.
go back to reference Wooldridge, J. M. (2009). Introductory econometrics: A modern approach. Boston: Cengage Learning. Wooldridge, J. M. (2009). Introductory econometrics: A modern approach. Boston: Cengage Learning.
go back to reference Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Zaller, J. (2004). Floating voters in U.S. presidential elections, 1948–2000. In W. E. Saris & P. M. Sniderman (Eds.), Studies in public opinion: Attitudes, nonattitudes, measurement error, and change. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Zaller, J. (2004). Floating voters in U.S. presidential elections, 1948–2000. In W. E. Saris & P. M. Sniderman (Eds.), Studies in public opinion: Attitudes, nonattitudes, measurement error, and change. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Metadata
Title
Partisanship, Political Awareness, and Retrospective Evaluations, 1956–2016
Author
Philip Edward Jones
Publication date
23-03-2019
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Political Behavior / Issue 4/2020
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-019-09543-y

Other articles of this Issue 4/2020

Political Behavior 4/2020 Go to the issue