Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Social Justice Research 4/2017

06-10-2017

The Role of Genes and Environments in Linking the Need to Evaluate with Political Ideology and Political Extremity

Authors: Aleksander Ksiazkiewicz, Robert F. Krueger

Published in: Social Justice Research | Issue 4/2017

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Understanding the origins of political ideology and political extremity at the individual level is becoming increasingly pressing in the face of polarization in the political domain. Building upon the motivated social cognition model of political ideology, we propose a motivated cognition approach to the study of political extremity with the need to evaluate as a key epistemic motive that contributes to political extremity. Moreover, we hypothesize that the link between the need to evaluate and political extremity may rest largely on shared genetic effects. This hypothesis builds upon existing biology and politics research, which has convincingly demonstrated that genes influence the direction of ideology, but has been largely silent on the role of genes in political extremity. To test our hypothesis, we consider several types of ideological, affective, and partisan extremity alongside conventional measures of political ideology and the need to evaluate in a behavioral genetic framework. Using a twin study methodology, we show for the first that the need to evaluate is heritable, that its phenotypic relationships with ideological extremity and strength are rooted in shared genetic influences, and, unexpectedly, that the relationship between the need to evaluate and some forms of political extremity is largely environmental. In examining the genetic and environmental components of the covariation of the need to evaluate with political ideology and right wing authoritarianism, we find limited support for shared genetic influences. Taken together, these results illustrate the value of adopting a biologically informed motivated cognition approach to the study of political ideology and political extremity.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
We conceive of these five aspects of political extremity as theoretically distinct, though they are positively intercorrelated. See also footnote 6.
 
2
It is also possible to conduct a trivariate analysis to determine whether the need to evaluate accounts for genetic variance in political ideology or political extremity when controlling for other measures, such as the Big Five personality traits (cf. Ksiazkiewicz, Ludeke, & Krueger, 2016). The need to evaluate has modest phenotypic correlations with the Big Five personality traits. In our sample, the correlations are: openness (0.19), conscientiousness (0.02), extraversion (0.23), agreeableness (-0.09), emotional stability (0.01). Focusing on openness and extraversion, these two personality factors are largely unrelated to our measures of political extremity (all phenotypic correlations below 0.1 in absolute value). Openness does have significant phenotypic correlations with political ideology in these data (see Ksiazkiewicz, Ludeke, & Krueger, 2016), but controlling for openness does not substantively change the results discussed below. As such, we do not present analyses formally testing whether the need to evaluate accounts for unique variance in political ideology or political extremity when controlling for Big Five traits.
 
3
Due to a labeling error, the middle response options on the need to evaluate scale were presented out of order. They were presented as “1 = Completely disagree, 2 = Moderately agree, 3 = Slightly agree, 4 = Slightly disagree, 5 = Moderately disagree, 6 = Completely agree.” This adds measurement error to our measure of the need to evaluate, which means that we likely have a more conservative estimate of the heritability of the need to evaluate than we might otherwise. Nonetheless, the scale demonstrates good reliability when coded as written (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.75, as compared to 0.87 in Jarvis & Petty, 1996). The reliability is considerably lower if we assume that participants fixed the poles (i.e., assumed that 1 and 6 were correct and that the intermediate categories were mislabeled; alpha = 0.56) or flipped the poles (assumed that 1 and 6 needed to be flipped; alpha = 0.56). For those participants that explicitly indicated that they were changing the labeling of the scale (e.g., by writing so on their response sheet), the participant response scale is used. Otherwise, we treat the responses as following the provided scale. This procedure produces a good Cronbach’s alpha (0.75).
 
4
There is also a fourth component that is sometimes estimated to represented dominance genetic effects (D), but the simultaneous estimation of A, C, D, and E requires data beyond that provided by MZ and DZ twins.
 
5
We focus here on the Wilson-Patterson measure of ideology, which contains less measurement error than the single-item measure (cf. Ksiazkiewicz, Ludeke, & Krueger, 2016).
 
6
As discussed above, we think of each of the extremity measures as capturing one of five theoretically distinct forms of extremity: ideological extremity, ideological strength, ideological breadth, attitude bipolarity, and strength of partisanship. For this reason, we analyze the ten measures of extremity in these five groupings in the results. Nonetheless, these different aspects of extremity are related conceptually to each other (and empirically to the need to evaluate). When the ten measures are combined in an exploratory factor analysis, two significant factors emerge. After rotation, the first factor captures extremity in terms of the Wilson–Patterson measures. The second factor captures extremity of partisanship (extremity of party identification, extremity of feeling thermometers toward parties and candidates). The extremity of single-item ideology and the feeling thermometers toward political groups load somewhat on each factor. Both factors have a significant phenotypic correlation with the need to evaluate (0.26, 0.18) and significant unique environmental correlations with the need to evaluate (0.16, 0.21). Only Factor 1 has a significant genetic correlation (0.52; for Factor 2, rA = 0.06). We present these results here for the interested reader, but focus on the five types of extremity described previously in the main text.
 
7
We kept these three types of feeling thermometers separate analytically because each represents a different type of political attitude theoretically (toward parties, candidates, and political movements). Nonetheless, the extremity of these attitudes is positively correlated. If we average together the three extremity measures, we do find a significant genetic correlation with the need to evaluate (0.30), in support of H3b, though we lose the unique environmental correlation (perhaps because the unique environmental factors that lead to attitude extremity toward parties, candidates, and political movements are not the same). We note this here simply to say that future research should utilize more nuanced measures for different types of attitudinal extremity to examine their relationships with the need to evaluate separately and jointly.
 
8
A similar pattern holds for the direction of political ideology, especially economic ideology (which shares a sizeable genetic component with the need to evaluate), and right wing authoritarianism (for which the covariance with the need to evaluate is primarily through the unique environment). Here, too, research on how these traits co-develop could be theoretically insightful and practically useful for developing effective interventions for pursuing fairness and social justice, insofar as these attitudes are relevant to perpetuating existing hierarchies in society.
 
Literature
go back to reference Alford, John, Funk, Carolyn, & Hibbing, John. (2005). Are political orientations genetically transmitted? American Political Science Review, 99(2), 153–167.CrossRef Alford, John, Funk, Carolyn, & Hibbing, John. (2005). Are political orientations genetically transmitted? American Political Science Review, 99(2), 153–167.CrossRef
go back to reference Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press. Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press.
go back to reference Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other ‘‘authoritarian personality’’. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 47–92.CrossRef Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other ‘‘authoritarian personality’’. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 47–92.CrossRef
go back to reference Amodio, D. M., Jost, J. T., Master, S. L., & Yee, C. M. (2007). Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1246–1247.CrossRefPubMed Amodio, D. M., Jost, J. T., Master, S. L., & Yee, C. M. (2007). Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1246–1247.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ansolabehere, S., Rodden, J., & Snyder, J. M., Jr. (2008). The strength of issues: Using multiple measures to gauge preference stability, ideological constraint, and issue voting. American Political Science Review, 102(2), 215–232.CrossRef Ansolabehere, S., Rodden, J., & Snyder, J. M., Jr. (2008). The strength of issues: Using multiple measures to gauge preference stability, ideological constraint, and issue voting. American Political Science Review, 102(2), 215–232.CrossRef
go back to reference Barber, M., & McCarty, N. (2015). Causes and Consequences of Polarization. In N. Persily (Ed.), Solutions to political polarization in America (pp. 15–58). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Barber, M., & McCarty, N. (2015). Causes and Consequences of Polarization. In N. Persily (Ed.), Solutions to political polarization in America (pp. 15–58). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Bell, E., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2009). The origins of political attitudes and behaviours: An analysis using twins. Canadian Journal of Political Science, 42(04), 855–879.CrossRef Bell, E., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2009). The origins of political attitudes and behaviours: An analysis using twins. Canadian Journal of Political Science, 42(04), 855–879.CrossRef
go back to reference Bizer, G. Y., Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Wheeler, S. C., Rucker, D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). The impact of personality on cognitive, behavioral, and affective political processes: The effects of the need to evaluate. Journal of Personality, 72, 995–1027.CrossRefPubMed Bizer, G. Y., Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Wheeler, S. C., Rucker, D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). The impact of personality on cognitive, behavioral, and affective political processes: The effects of the need to evaluate. Journal of Personality, 72, 995–1027.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Boker, S., Neale, M., Maes, H., Wilde, M., Spiegel, M., Brick, T., et al. (2011). OpenMx: An open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika, 76(2), 306–317.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Boker, S., Neale, M., Maes, H., Wilde, M., Spiegel, M., Brick, T., et al. (2011). OpenMx: An open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika, 76(2), 306–317.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences. Journal of Neurobiology, 54(1), 4–45.CrossRefPubMed Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences. Journal of Neurobiology, 54(1), 4–45.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology, 29(6), 807–840.CrossRef Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology, 29(6), 807–840.CrossRef
go back to reference Converse, Philip E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press. Converse, Philip E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press.
go back to reference Crowson, H. M. (2009). Are all conservatives alike? A study of the psychological correlates of cultural and economic conservatism. The Journal of Psychology, 143(5), 449–463.CrossRefPubMed Crowson, H. M. (2009). Are all conservatives alike? A study of the psychological correlates of cultural and economic conservatism. The Journal of Psychology, 143(5), 449–463.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Druckman, J. N., Peterson, E., & Slothuus, R. (2013). How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. American Political Science Review, 107(1), 57–79.CrossRef Druckman, J. N., Peterson, E., & Slothuus, R. (2013). How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. American Political Science Review, 107(1), 57–79.CrossRef
go back to reference Druckman, J. N., & Leeper, T. J. (2012). Learning more from political communication experiments: Pretreatment and its effects. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 875–896.CrossRef Druckman, J. N., & Leeper, T. J. (2012). Learning more from political communication experiments: Pretreatment and its effects. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 875–896.CrossRef
go back to reference Dvir-Gvirsman, S. (2014). One-track minds? Cognitive needs, media diet, and overestimation of public support for one’s views. Media Psychology, 18(4), 475–498.CrossRef Dvir-Gvirsman, S. (2014). One-track minds? Cognitive needs, media diet, and overestimation of public support for one’s views. Media Psychology, 18(4), 475–498.CrossRef
go back to reference Fazekas, Z., & Littvay, L. (2015). The importance of context in the genetic transmission of US party identification. Political Psychology, 36(4), 361–377.CrossRef Fazekas, Z., & Littvay, L. (2015). The importance of context in the genetic transmission of US party identification. Political Psychology, 36(4), 361–377.CrossRef
go back to reference Federico, C. M. (2004). Predicting attitude extremity: The interactive effects of schema development and the need to evaluate—and their mediation by evaluative integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1281–1294.CrossRefPubMed Federico, C. M. (2004). Predicting attitude extremity: The interactive effects of schema development and the need to evaluate—and their mediation by evaluative integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1281–1294.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Federico, C. M. (2007). Expertise, evaluative motivation, and the structure of citizens’ ideological commitments. Political Psychology, 28, 535–562.CrossRef Federico, C. M. (2007). Expertise, evaluative motivation, and the structure of citizens’ ideological commitments. Political Psychology, 28, 535–562.CrossRef
go back to reference Federico, C. M., & Hunt, C. V. (2013). Political information, political involvement, and reliance on ideology in political evaluation. Political Behavior, 35, 89–112.CrossRef Federico, C. M., & Hunt, C. V. (2013). Political information, political involvement, and reliance on ideology in political evaluation. Political Behavior, 35, 89–112.CrossRef
go back to reference Federico, C. M., & Schneider, M. (2007). Political expertise and the use of ideology: Moderating effects of evaluative motivation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71, 221–252.CrossRef Federico, C. M., & Schneider, M. (2007). Political expertise and the use of ideology: Moderating effects of evaluative motivation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71, 221–252.CrossRef
go back to reference Feldman, S., & Johnston, C. (2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology, 35(3), 337–358.CrossRef Feldman, S., & Johnston, C. (2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology, 35(3), 337–358.CrossRef
go back to reference Friesen, A., & Ksiazkiewicz, A. (2015). Do political attitudes and religiosity share a genetic path? Political Behavior, 37(4), 791–818.CrossRef Friesen, A., & Ksiazkiewicz, A. (2015). Do political attitudes and religiosity share a genetic path? Political Behavior, 37(4), 791–818.CrossRef
go back to reference Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., Dowling, C. M., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Personality and political attitudes: Relationships across issue domains and political contexts. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 111–133.CrossRef Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., Dowling, C. M., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Personality and political attitudes: Relationships across issue domains and political contexts. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 111–133.CrossRef
go back to reference Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.CrossRefPubMed Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hatemi, P. K., Funk, C. L., Medland, S. E., Maes, H. M., Silberg, J. L., Martin, N. G., et al. (2009). Genetic and environmental transmission of political attitudes over a life time. Journal of Politics, 71(3), 1141–1156.CrossRef Hatemi, P. K., Funk, C. L., Medland, S. E., Maes, H. M., Silberg, J. L., Martin, N. G., et al. (2009). Genetic and environmental transmission of political attitudes over a life time. Journal of Politics, 71(3), 1141–1156.CrossRef
go back to reference Hatemi, P. K., Gillespie, N. A., Eaves, L. J., Maher, B. S., Webb, B. T., Heath, A. C., et al. (2011). A genome-wide analysis of liberal and conservative political attitudes. Journal of Politics, 73(1), 271–285.CrossRef Hatemi, P. K., Gillespie, N. A., Eaves, L. J., Maher, B. S., Webb, B. T., Heath, A. C., et al. (2011). A genome-wide analysis of liberal and conservative political attitudes. Journal of Politics, 73(1), 271–285.CrossRef
go back to reference Hatemi, P. K., Medland, S. E., Klemmensen, R., Oskarsson, S., Littvay, L., Dawes, C. T., et al. (2014). Genetic influences on political ideologies: Twin analyses of 19 measures of political ideologies from five democracies and genome-wide findings from three populations. Behavior Genetics, 44(3), 282–294.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hatemi, P. K., Medland, S. E., Klemmensen, R., Oskarsson, S., Littvay, L., Dawes, C. T., et al. (2014). Genetic influences on political ideologies: Twin analyses of 19 measures of political ideologies from five democracies and genome-wide findings from three populations. Behavior Genetics, 44(3), 282–294.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hetherington, M. J., & Rudolph, T. J. (2015). Why Washington won’t work: Polarization, political trust, and the governing crisis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Hetherington, M. J., & Rudolph, T. J. (2015). Why Washington won’t work: Polarization, political trust, and the governing crisis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Hetherington, M. J., & Weiler, J. D. (2009). Authoritarianism and polarization in American politics. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Hetherington, M. J., & Weiler, J. D. (2009). Authoritarianism and polarization in American politics. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(3), 297–307.CrossRefPubMed Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(3), 297–307.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., Xu, X., & Peterson, J. B. (2010). Compassionate liberals and polite conservatives: Associations of agreeableness with political ideology and moral values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 655–664.CrossRefPubMed Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., Xu, X., & Peterson, J. B. (2010). Compassionate liberals and polite conservatives: Associations of agreeableness with political ideology and moral values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 655–664.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2009). Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 714–725.CrossRef Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2009). Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 714–725.CrossRef
go back to reference Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D., Iyer, R., & Haidt, J. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 537–544.CrossRef Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D., Iyer, R., & Haidt, J. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 537–544.CrossRef
go back to reference Iyengar, S., Sood, G., & Lelkes, Y. (2012). Affect, not ideology: A social identity perspective on polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(3), 405–431.CrossRef Iyengar, S., Sood, G., & Lelkes, Y. (2012). Affect, not ideology: A social identity perspective on polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(3), 405–431.CrossRef
go back to reference Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.CrossRef Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.CrossRef
go back to reference Jarvis, W. B. G., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 172–194.CrossRef Jarvis, W. B. G., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 172–194.CrossRef
go back to reference Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: Its structure, functions and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 307–337.CrossRefPubMed Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: Its structure, functions and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 307–337.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375.CrossRefPubMed Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jost, J. T., Kay, A. C., & Thorisdottir, H. (Eds.). (2009). Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. New York: Oxford University Press. Jost, J. T., Kay, A. C., & Thorisdottir, H. (Eds.). (2009). Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Jost, J. T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Simon, L. (1999). Effects of epistemic motivation on conservatism, intolerance and other system-justifying attitudes. In L. I. Thompson, J. M. Levine, & D. M. Messick (Eds.), Shared cognition in organizations: The management of knowledge (pp. 91–116). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Jost, J. T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Simon, L. (1999). Effects of epistemic motivation on conservatism, intolerance and other system-justifying attitudes. In L. I. Thompson, J. M. Levine, & D. M. Messick (Eds.), Shared cognition in organizations: The management of knowledge (pp. 91–116). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Jost, J. T., Nam, H. H., Amodio, D. M., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2014). Political neuroscience: The beginning of a beautiful friendship. Political Psychology, 35, 3–42. doi:10.1111/pops.12162.CrossRef Jost, J. T., Nam, H. H., Amodio, D. M., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2014). Political neuroscience: The beginning of a beautiful friendship. Political Psychology, 35, 3–42. doi:10.​1111/​pops.​12162.CrossRef
go back to reference Jost, J. T., Napier, J. L., Thorisdottir, H., Gosling, S. D., Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Are needs to manage uncertainty and threat associated with political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 989–1007.CrossRefPubMed Jost, J. T., Napier, J. L., Thorisdottir, H., Gosling, S. D., Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Are needs to manage uncertainty and threat associated with political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 989–1007.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: Its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(2), 126–136.CrossRefPubMed Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: Its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(2), 126–136.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., & Riemann, R. (2012). Left or right? Sources of political orientation: The roles of genetic factors, cultural transmission, assortative mating, and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 633–645.CrossRefPubMed Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., & Riemann, R. (2012). Left or right? Sources of political orientation: The roles of genetic factors, cultural transmission, assortative mating, and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 633–645.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kemmelmeier, M. (1997). Need for closure and political orientation among German university students. Journal of Social Psychology, 137, 787–789.CrossRef Kemmelmeier, M. (1997). Need for closure and political orientation among German university students. Journal of Social Psychology, 137, 787–789.CrossRef
go back to reference Krosnick, J. A., Boninger, D. S., Chuang, Y. C., Berent, M. K., & Carnot, C. G. (1993). Attitude strength: One construct or many related constructs? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1132–1151.CrossRef Krosnick, J. A., Boninger, D. S., Chuang, Y. C., Berent, M. K., & Carnot, C. G. (1993). Attitude strength: One construct or many related constructs? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1132–1151.CrossRef
go back to reference Ksiazkiewicz, A., Ludeke, S., & Krueger, R. (2016). The role of cognitive style in the link between genes and political ideology. Political Psychology. doi:10.1111/pops.12318. Ksiazkiewicz, A., Ludeke, S., & Krueger, R. (2016). The role of cognitive style in the link between genes and political ideology. Political Psychology. doi:10.​1111/​pops.​12318.
go back to reference Leader Maynard, J., & Mildenberger, M. (2016). Convergence and divergence in the study of ideology: A critical review. British Journal of Political Science. doi:10.1017/S0007123415000654. Leader Maynard, J., & Mildenberger, M. (2016). Convergence and divergence in the study of ideology: A critical review. British Journal of Political Science. doi:10.​1017/​S000712341500065​4.
go back to reference Leeper, T. J. (2014). Cognitive style and the survey response. Public Opinion Quarterly, 78(4), 974–983.CrossRef Leeper, T. J. (2014). Cognitive style and the survey response. Public Opinion Quarterly, 78(4), 974–983.CrossRef
go back to reference Ludeke, S. G., Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2013). “Obedience to traditional authority:” A heritable factor underlying authoritarianism, conservatism and religiousness. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(4), 375–380.CrossRef Ludeke, S. G., Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2013). “Obedience to traditional authority:” A heritable factor underlying authoritarianism, conservatism and religiousness. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(4), 375–380.CrossRef
go back to reference Malka, A., Soto, C. J., Inzlicht, M., & Lelkes, Y. (2014). Do needs for security and certainty predict cultural and economic conservatism? A cross-national analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 1031–1051.CrossRefPubMed Malka, A., Soto, C. J., Inzlicht, M., & Lelkes, Y. (2014). Do needs for security and certainty predict cultural and economic conservatism? A cross-national analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 1031–1051.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mason, L. (2015). “I Disrespectfully Agree”: The differential effects of partisan sorting on social and issue polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(1), 128–145.CrossRef Mason, L. (2015). “I Disrespectfully Agree”: The differential effects of partisan sorting on social and issue polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(1), 128–145.CrossRef
go back to reference McCarty, N., Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (2006). Polarized America: The dance of ideology and unequal riches. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. McCarty, N., Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (2006). Polarized America: The dance of ideology and unequal riches. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Medland, S. E., & Hatemi, P. K. (2009). Political science, biometric theory, and twin studies: A methodological introduction. Political Analysis, 17(2), 191–214.CrossRef Medland, S. E., & Hatemi, P. K. (2009). Political science, biometric theory, and twin studies: A methodological introduction. Political Analysis, 17(2), 191–214.CrossRef
go back to reference Mondak, J. J. (2010). Personality and the foundations of political behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Mondak, J. J. (2010). Personality and the foundations of political behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Nir, L. (2011). Motivated reasoning and public opinion perception. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75(3), 504–532.CrossRef Nir, L. (2011). Motivated reasoning and public opinion perception. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75(3), 504–532.CrossRef
go back to reference Osborne, D., Wootton, L. W., & Sibley, C. G. (2013). Are liberals agreeable or not? Politeness and compassion differentially predict political conservatism via distinct ideologies. Social Psychology, 44, 354–360.CrossRef Osborne, D., Wootton, L. W., & Sibley, C. G. (2013). Are liberals agreeable or not? Politeness and compassion differentially predict political conservatism via distinct ideologies. Social Psychology, 44, 354–360.CrossRef
go back to reference Oskarsson, S., Cesarini, D., Dawes, C. T., Fowler, J. H., Johannesson, M., Magnusson, P. K. E., et al. (2015). Linking genes and political orientations: Testing the cognitive ability as media hypothesis. Political Psychology, 36(6), 649–665.CrossRef Oskarsson, S., Cesarini, D., Dawes, C. T., Fowler, J. H., Johannesson, M., Magnusson, P. K. E., et al. (2015). Linking genes and political orientations: Testing the cognitive ability as media hypothesis. Political Psychology, 36(6), 649–665.CrossRef
go back to reference Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, M. V., Miller, J. L., Scalora, M., et al. (2008). Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science, 321(5896), 1667–1670.CrossRefPubMed Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, M. V., Miller, J. L., Scalora, M., et al. (2008). Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science, 321(5896), 1667–1670.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prior, M. (2013). Media and political polarization. Annual Review of Political Science, 16, 101–127.CrossRef Prior, M. (2013). Media and political polarization. Annual Review of Political Science, 16, 101–127.CrossRef
go back to reference Rico, G., & Jennings, M. K. (2016). The formation of left-right identification: Pathways and correlates of parental influence. Political Psychology, 37(2), 237–252.CrossRef Rico, G., & Jennings, M. K. (2016). The formation of left-right identification: Pathways and correlates of parental influence. Political Psychology, 37(2), 237–252.CrossRef
go back to reference Sargent, M. J. (2004). Less thought, more punishment: Need for cognition predicts support for punitive responses to crime. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(11), 1485–1493.CrossRef Sargent, M. J. (2004). Less thought, more punishment: Need for cognition predicts support for punitive responses to crime. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(11), 1485–1493.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? Journal of Social Issues, 50(4), 19–45.CrossRef Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? Journal of Social Issues, 50(4), 19–45.CrossRef
go back to reference Settle, J. E., Dawes, C. T., & Fowler, J. H. (2009). The heritability of partisan attachment. Political Research Quarterly, 62(3), 601–613.CrossRef Settle, J. E., Dawes, C. T., & Fowler, J. H. (2009). The heritability of partisan attachment. Political Research Quarterly, 62(3), 601–613.CrossRef
go back to reference Sibley, C. G., Osborne, D., & Duckitt, J. (2012). Personality and political orientation: Meta-analysis and test of a threat-constraint model. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(6), 664–677.CrossRef Sibley, C. G., Osborne, D., & Duckitt, J. (2012). Personality and political orientation: Meta-analysis and test of a threat-constraint model. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(6), 664–677.CrossRef
go back to reference Sidanius, J. (1988). Political sophistication and political deviance: A structural equation examination of context theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 37–51.CrossRef Sidanius, J. (1988). Political sophistication and political deviance: A structural equation examination of context theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 37–51.CrossRef
go back to reference Stoker, L., & Bass, J. (2013). Political socialization: Ongoing questions and new directions. In G. C. Edwards, L. R. Jacobs, & R. Y. Shapiro (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of American public opinion and the media. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199545636.003.0028. Stoker, L., & Bass, J. (2013). Political socialization: Ongoing questions and new directions. In G. C. Edwards, L. R. Jacobs, & R. Y. Shapiro (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of American public opinion and the media. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.​1093/​oxfordhb/​9780199545636.​003.​0028.
go back to reference Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.CrossRef Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.CrossRef
go back to reference Terrizzi, J. A., Jr., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 587–592.CrossRef Terrizzi, J. A., Jr., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 587–592.CrossRef
go back to reference Thurber, J. A., & Yoshinaka, A. (2016). American gridlock: The sources, character, and impact of political polarization. New York: Cambridge University Press. Thurber, J. A., & Yoshinaka, A. (2016). American gridlock: The sources, character, and impact of political polarization. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Verhulst, B., Eaves, L. J., & Hatemi, P. K. (2012). Correlation not causation: The relationship between personality traits and political ideologies. American Journal of Political Science, 56(1), 34–51.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Verhulst, B., Eaves, L. J., & Hatemi, P. K. (2012). Correlation not causation: The relationship between personality traits and political ideologies. American Journal of Political Science, 56(1), 34–51.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Wilson, G. D., & Patterson, J. R. (1968). A new measure of conservatism. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7(4), 264–269.CrossRefPubMed Wilson, G. D., & Patterson, J. R. (1968). A new measure of conservatism. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7(4), 264–269.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Xu, X., Mar, R. A., & Peterson, J. B. (2013). Does cultural exposure partially explain the association between personality and political orientation? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1497–1517.CrossRefPubMed Xu, X., Mar, R. A., & Peterson, J. B. (2013). Does cultural exposure partially explain the association between personality and political orientation? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1497–1517.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Xu, X., Plaks, J. E., & Peterson, J. B. (2016). From dispositions to goals to ideology: Toward a synthesis of personality and social psychological approaches to political orientation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(5), 267–280.CrossRef Xu, X., Plaks, J. E., & Peterson, J. B. (2016). From dispositions to goals to ideology: Toward a synthesis of personality and social psychological approaches to political orientation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(5), 267–280.CrossRef
go back to reference Zakrisson, I. (2005). Construction of a short version of the Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA) scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 863–872.CrossRef Zakrisson, I. (2005). Construction of a short version of the Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA) scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 863–872.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
The Role of Genes and Environments in Linking the Need to Evaluate with Political Ideology and Political Extremity
Authors
Aleksander Ksiazkiewicz
Robert F. Krueger
Publication date
06-10-2017
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Social Justice Research / Issue 4/2017
Print ISSN: 0885-7466
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6725
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-017-0292-3

Other articles of this Issue 4/2017

Social Justice Research 4/2017 Go to the issue