Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Cognitive Neurodynamics 2/2012

Open Access 01-04-2012 | Research Article

Traveling EEG slow oscillation along the dorsal attention network initiates spontaneous perceptual switching

Authors: Takashi J. Ozaki, Naoyuki Sato, Keiichi Kitajo, Yoshiaki Someya, Kimitaka Anami, Hiroaki Mizuhara, Seiji Ogawa, Yoko Yamaguchi

Published in: Cognitive Neurodynamics | Issue 2/2012

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

An ambiguous figure such as the Necker cube causes spontaneous perceptual switching (SPS). The mechanism of SPS in multistable perception has not yet been determined. Although early psychological studies suggested that SPS may be caused by fatigue or satiation of orientation, the neural mechanism of SPS is still unknown. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has shown that the dorsal attention network (DAN), which mainly controls voluntary attention, is involved in bistable perception of the Necker cube. To determine whether neural dynamics along the DAN cause SPS, we performed simultaneous electroencephalography (EEG) and fMRI during an SPS task with the Necker cube, with every SPS reported by pressing a button. This EEG–fMRI integrated analysis showed that (a) 3–4 Hz spectral EEG power modulation at fronto-central, parietal, and centro-parietal electrode sites sequentially appeared from 750 to 350 ms prior to the button press; and (b) activations correlating with the EEG modulation traveled along the DAN from the frontal to the parietal regions. These findings suggest that slow oscillation initiates SPS through global dynamics along the attentional system such as the DAN.

Introduction

Seeing is a process of uniquely choosing a single percept from among many possibilities. When seeing a single ambiguous figure that allows multiple percepts, however, an observer’s representation cannot always be embodied uniquely; our percept easily changes from one percept to another without any physical change in the figure. The Necker cube, Rubin’s vase (Leopold and Logothetis 1999), and the dynamic dot quartet (Ramachandran and Anstis 1983) are examples that induce these phenomena. A transient process of this phenomenon is spontaneous perceptual switching (SPS), in which observers switch spontaneously from one percept to another, indicates that complex neural mechanisms underlie multistable perception.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has shown that the dorsal frontoparietal regions are involved in SPS induced by the Necker cube (Inui et al. 2000; Shen et al. 2009). These dorsal frontoparietal regions, also called the “dorsal attention network” (DAN), are related to voluntary attentional control (Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Corbetta et al. 2008). Moreover, experiments have shown the involvement of the DAN in both Necker-cube induced SPS and voluntary attentional control (Slotnick and Yantis 2005), suggesting that these processes are closely related and involve common neural substrates. This hypothesis is consistent with the results of a psychophysical study, which found that SPS induced by the Necker cube could be modulated by voluntary attentional control, whereas binocular rivalry could not (Meng and Tong 2004).
Furthermore, recent effective connectivity fMRI studies with optimized experimental (longer duration of events) and imaging (longer repetition time of fMRI scanning) parameters for effective connectivity analysis, such as Granger causality analysis, indicated that voluntary attentional control may be related to top-down connectivity of neural processing along the DAN (Bressler et al. 2008; Ozaki and Ogawa 2009; Ozaki 2011).
These findings lead to another hypothesis that similar top-down connectivity of neural processing along the DAN may also occur in case of SPS induced by the Necker cube, because the DAN may be a common neural substrate of both voluntary attentional control and SPS induced by the Necker cube, as suggested by previous behavioral and functional neuroimaging studies.
On the other hand, the mechanism by which neural processes initiate SPS still remains unclear. Especially, because of the low temporal resolution of fMRI and the excessively rapid changing behavior of Necker cube induced SPS, fMRI alone could not determine this mechanism even using recently developed effective connectivity analysis techniques.
In contrast, electroencephalography (EEG) has revealed neural correlates of Necker cube induced SPS cube in a fine time scale (Kornmeier and Bach 2004, 2005; Britz et al. 2009). Moreover, some oscillatory EEG activities, especially delta-band (around at 4 Hz) activities, have been shown to correlate with SPS (Nakatani and van Leeuwen 2005, 2006; Shimaoka et al. 2010; Nakatani et al. 2011). Delta-band oscillatory EEG activities have been thought to be mediators of long-range cortical networks such as the DAN (Isoglu-Alkac et al. 1998, 2000; Mathes et al. 2006). These findings suggest that some neural dynamics may be self-organized by slow oscillatory neural activities in the human brain, in agreement with theoretical predictions (Shimizu and Yamaguchi 1987; Yamaguchi and Shimizu 1994). Furthermore, a previous EEG study even indicated that the frontal-to-occipital synchrony or connectivity may be important for SPS induced by the Necker cube (Shimaoka et al. 2010). On a larger level, the fronto-parietal delta-band oscillatory EEG coherence was suggested as an important component for a general attention-demanding cognition (Guntekin and Basar 2010). This line of evidence appears to support our current hypothesis.
Recent advances in imaging techniques have enabled the simultaneous recording of EEG data on a fine time scale during fMRI scanning, allowing us to determine the dynamics of neural activity on a fine time scale (Mizuhara et al. 2004, 2005; Mizuhara and Yamaguchi 2007; Laufs et al. 2008). These findings indicate that simultaneous EEG–fMRI technology may be a promising method for testing our hypothesis.
To determine whether rapidly changing neural dynamics along the DAN from anterior (frontal) to posterior (parietal) regions occur prior to Necker cube induced SPS, we performed a simultaneous EEG–fMRI experiment with EEG–fMRI integrated analysis and supplementary EEG signal source estimates on subjects performing an SPS task with the Necker cube.

Methods

Participants

Twenty right-handed healthy volunteers (21–44 years old, 4 females, normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity) participated in the experiment and each participant gave written informed consent for participation. The experiment and its protocol were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of RIKEN and Ogawa Laboratories for Brain Function Research, respectively.

Stimuli and experimental procedure

Each experimental session consisted of three blocks, each with its own condition. In the test condition, a Necker cube (size: 4.5° × 4.5°) was presented continuously with a fixation cross (Fig. 1a). In the control condition, a pair of cube stimuli (4.5° × 4.5°) with different viewpoints was presented alternately. In both the test and control conditions, participants were instructed to gaze at the fixation cross and respond by holding down one of two buttons until the next percept arose, thus allowing the participant to indicate the viewpoint from which he/she perceived the cube (left button, upper viewpoint; right button, lower viewpoint; Fig. 1b). In the rest condition, participants passively viewed a hexagon with the same outline as the Necker cube and normal cube stimuli, but did not make any responses. Each block of condition lasted 3 min 6 s; therefore, each session lasted 9 min 18 s. Each participant was involved in three sessions per experiment.
A pilot study indicated that seeing a pair of cube stimuli was influenced by the experience of seeing a Necker cube; therefore, in these experiments, blocks of the test condition always preceded those of the control condition. To counterbalance the effect of a fixed order of blocks and to avoid overestimation of activation by fMRI, a mask procedure of the activation map was performed (see EEG–fMRI integrated analysis).

Simultaneous EEG data recording with fMRI and preprocessing

During fMRI scanning, scalp EEG was simultaneously recorded using a Brain Cap MR 64 Ag/AgCL electrode cap with two Brain Amp MR (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany) 32 channel amplifiers. Impedances were kept below 5 kΩ at the ground and reference electrodes and below 20 kΩ at other electrodes. EEG data were digitized at a sampling rate of 5,000 Hz and filtered with 1.0 Hz highpass and 250 Hz lowpass filters. Data obtained from the third session of all participants were excluded because of low data quality. Data from the second session of two participants were excluded because of excessive body movements.
Prior to data analysis, the recorded EEG data were preprocessed by Brain Vision Analyzer software (Brain Products) to reduce artifacts derived from the simultaneous EEG–fMRI environment. First, MRI scanning artifacts were reduced by the averaged artifact subtraction (AAS) method (Allen et al. 2000), with the EEG amplifiers and MRI scanner synchronization technique (Mandelkow et al. 2006). Second, ballistocardiographic artifacts were reduced by the AAS method (Allen et al. 1998). Finally, any remaining slice scan timing artifacts and unidentified machinery artifacts were reduced by the phase-shift free band-cutoff filter. Cutoff frequencies were 16.0 and 19.33 Hz, including harmonics of 19.33 Hz in the higher frequency bands. 16.0 Hz was a frequency of a persistent artifact occurring even in case of simultaneous EEG–fMRI recording with a saline phantom and it was suspected as the scanner-specified machinery artifact.

EEG data analysis

Preprocessed EEG data were analyzed by Brain Vision Analyzer software and Matlab software package (Mathworks, Natick, MA). The data were downsampled to a 250 Hz sampling rate and filtered with a 1–40 Hz bandpass filter. The reference used for the EEG data was changed to the average of all electrodes after filtering because ear reference electrodes showed low signal-to-noise ratios, due to ear mufflers that protected participants’ ears from fMRI scanning noises. This process was followed by infomax independent component analysis (ICA) to the filtered data to segregate noises and artifacts from true EEG signals, because any remaining noises or artifacts would cause severe artifacts of EEG-derived regressors for fMRI data analysis (Delorme and Makeig 2004). By visual inspection, we excluded independent components (ICs) with (a) waveforms that correlated with the electrooculograms, (b) abnormally widely distributed frequency spectra, (c) spike-shaped waveforms causing broadly-distributed gamma-band artifacts (Yuval-Greenberg et al. 2008), (d) a reconstructed EEG topography showing prominent left–right symmetry with greater amplitude (Britz et al. 2010). Consequently, 28 ± 3 independent components were excluded from the EEG data of each participant. We next computed wavelet transforms (complex Morlet’s wavelet with a constant ratio of f/σf = 7) of the EEG data to obtain time courses of spectral power variations in the time–frequency domain. Finally, to evaluate event-related modulation, the spectral power variations of the time–frequency domain were averaged and time-locked to the button holding onset latency.
Wavelet-transformed EEG data were statistically analyzed based on the random effects model. All EEG data were normalized relative to the EEG data in the rest condition. For first level analysis, a two-sample, two-tailed t-test was performed on each participant to quantify the modulation in the test condition compared with the control condition. For second level analysis, a one-sample, two-tailed t-test was performed on t-values from all participants using a random effects model, with the significance threshold set at an uncorrected p < 0.05.

EEG signal source estimates

In order to obtain a rough picture of neural dynamics from the EEG data, the standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA) inverse solution was performed with the distributed software package (Pascual-Marqui 2002), onto the EEG data simultaneously recorded during the fMRI scanning.
For the sLORETA inverse solution, first statistically significant oscillatory EEG component in time–frequency domain was extracted as a difference of time course of oscillatory EEG power between the test and control condition. Second, the extracted time course was averaged across all time points in order to stabilize source estimates in terms of time. Finally, the inverse solution was performed with a solution space defined by the relative head-surface-based approximation of the 10/10 electrodes system coordinates (Jurcak et al. 2007), onto the digitized probability brain atlas of the Brain Imaging Center at Montreal Neurological Institute (Collins et al. 1994).

fMRI data acquisition and preprocessing

A Magnetom Allegra 3.0 T head-dedicated MRI scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) was used to acquire fMRI data, based on the blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) effect (Ogawa et al. 1992). For each participant, functional images were scanned using a T2*-weighted single-shot echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence. Scanning parameters were: TR = 1,500 ms, TE = 22 ms, flip angle = 90°, field of view = 230 × 230 mm2, imaging matrix = 64 × 64, and in-plane resolution = 3.0 mm. These images consisted of 29 contiguous transverse slices covering the cerebral cortex (not including the cerebellum) with no gaps, with 375 whole-brain volumes acquired during each session. T1-weighted high-resolution anatomical images were scanned as sagittal slices with the magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence (TR = 2,500 ms, TE = 4.38 ms, voxel size = 1.3 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm).
Acquired functional images were preprocessed by BrainVoyager QX software (Brain Innovation, Maastricht, Netherlands). The first three volumes of each scan were discarded to allow the magnet to stabilize. The remaining 372 volumes were filtered at a three cycle per session temporal frequency to remove any linear trend. The volumes were realigned to the first image, corrected for head motion and differences in slice scan timing, smoothed using a 7.0 mm full-width half maximum Gaussian spatial filter, and normalized to Talairach stereotaxic space (Talairach and Tournoux 1988).

EEG–fMRI integrated analysis

To determine the correlation between electrophysiological oscillatory activities and hemodynamic neural activations as neural substrates of SPS, two general linear models (GLM) were computed to obtain activation maps. One model was based on the block effects defined by a box-car function of three experimental blocks (test, control and rest, ‘model 1’). The other model was based on variations of EEG spectral power at a specified frequency, the onsets of participants’ responses, and block effects. In the first model, each regressor was computed by convolution of each box-car function with the canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) in the blocked-design manner as a “block-effect” regressor (‘model 2’). The latter model utilized three steps to compute regressors. First, an EEG based regressor was obtained by convolution of the time course of the normalized EEG spectral power variation at a specified frequency and electrode with the canonical HRF. Second, a response-onset based regressor was computed by convolution of the time course of the button onset timings with the canonical HRF. Finally, the arithmetic sum of the EEG based regressor, the response-onset based regressor, and the block-effect regressor was computed as an “EEG-motor-block” combined effects regressor (Mizuhara et al. 2004, 2005; Mizuhara and Yamaguchi 2007). The response-onset based regressor was included in the EEG-motor-block effect regressor since explicit motor responses are not included in the EEG data. Thus, information about motor responses should be obtained from behavioral data, independent of EEG data.
After two models were specified, one activation map was computed by model 1 as a block-effect map, and the other activation map was computed by model 2 as an EEG-motor-block effect map. For both maps, a linear contrast as [test > control] was used to compute voxel-wise statistics (random effects model, uncorrected p < 0.005, volume threshold as 40 mm3). Finally, to counterbalance the effect of the fixed order of the experimental blocks and to avoid overestimation of activation, the model 2-derived map was masked by the model 1-derived map (Mizuhara et al. 2004, 2005; Mizuhara and Yamaguchi 2007).

Results

Behavior

In the experiment, twenty participants performed an SPS task with the Necker cube during fMRI scanning. Data from four participants were discarded because of excessive artifacts in EEG and/or fMRI data caused by body movements or problems with machinery. Thus, data from 16 participants were analyzed and evaluated.
A histogram of the perceptual duration in the test condition showed a trend of gamma distribution; the histogram could be fitted by a gamma distribution Ga(k, θ) where k = 1.62 and θ = 2,494.9 (Fig. 1c). The correlation of determination of the fitted gamma distribution (R2) was 0.908, consistent with the results of a previous psychological study (Murata et al. 2003). In contrast, R2 between the distribution of the response duration and that of the stimulus duration in the control condition was 0.796. This result indicates that the participants adequately performed the task in the control condition and successfully followed viewpoint changes of the cube stimuli. The mode value of the response time to changes in the control stimuli was 357 ms. This latency can be regarded as a stimulus onset latency of SPS induced by the Necker cube.

EEG oscillatory dynamics prior to SPS

To determine the detailed time course of neural dynamics related to SPS during multistable perception, we performed a time–frequency domain analysis of spectral power in the simultaneously-recorded EEG and fMRI data in an event-related manner. The time–frequency domain plots time-locked by the button-press response showed a significantly greater modulation at lower frequency bands associated with SPS in the test than in the control condition. Topographic maps of the EEG spectral power at 3.0 Hz from 1,400 to 200 ms before the button was pressed showed significant modulations at the left frontal, right parietal and centro-parietal electrodes in the test compared with the control condition (Fig. 2a).
We chose three electrodes showing prominent modulation at approximately 3–4 Hz narrow frequency band: FC3, P6, and CP2. Time–frequency domain plots of the EEG data recorded at these three electrodes demonstrated significant modulations at 2.5–4.4 Hz with a peak at 750 ms before the button press for FC3 electrode (abbreviated as “early FC3 component”: Fig. 2b, top), at 2.6–3.4 Hz with a peak at 600 ms before the button press for P6 electrode (“middle P6 component”: Fig. 2b, middle), and at 2.8–4.0 Hz with a peak at 350 ms before the button press for CP2 electrode (“late CP2 component”: Fig. 2b, bottom). These oscillatory EEG components appeared to correlate with SPS.
From the behavioral data in the control condition, 357 ms before the button press was estimated as an “assumed onset of the percept of a cube” in both the test and control condition. The relationship of the latencies of the three oscillatory EEG components implied that the early FC3 component and the middle P6 component arose prior to the assumed percept of the Necker cube, and that only the late CP6 component occurred almost at the same time with the percept.

Low spatial resolution neural dynamics from the anterior to posterior cortical regions estimated from the oscillatory EEG data

Prior to EEG–fMRI integrated analysis with high temporal resolution, time-varying electromagnetic source estimates were performed by using the standardized low-resolution electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA) method. For the source estimates, each oscillatory EEG component was extracted as follows: 1,100–750 ms before the button press at 2.5–4.4 Hz for the early FC3 component, 800–400 ms at 2.6–3.4 Hz for the middle P6 component, and 750–200 ms at 2.8–4.0 Hz for the late CP2 component based on the p-value plot in time–frequency domain (Fig. 2).
Figure 3 showed results of the sLORETA source estimates for the three oscillatory EEG components. Despite its low spatial resolution, the results indicated neural dynamics before and/or at SPS. For the early FC3 component, the sLORETA solution showed broader bilateral frontal distribution covering and focal bilateral occipital distribution. For the middle P6 component, the source estimates solution showed broader bilateral post-central-parietal distribution. Finally, for the late CP2 component, the source estimate solution indicated broader bilateral parietal distribution.
In total, the three sLORETA solutions indicated a qualitative trend in which neural dynamics before and/or at SPS traveled from the anterior to posterior cortical regions, while the solution for the early FC3 component included narrow but rather intensive occipital distribution.

Cortical activations correlating with EEG oscillatory dynamics prior to SPS revealed by EEG–fMRI integrated analysis

The result of the sLORETA solutions indicated a trend of neural dynamics traveling from the anterior to posterior cortical regions. However, it cannot precisely localize details of neural activity yielding such neural dynamics. In the current study, we applied EEG–fMRI integrated analysis to the fMRI data in order to localize the neural activity underlying the neural dynamics indicated by the sLORETA solutions.
To obtain fMRI activation maps with EEG–fMRI integrated analysis, we first utilized a conventional blocked design analysis. Consistent with previous fMRI results, our activation map with a linear contrast as [test > control] showed considerable activation at the premotor, inferior frontal, medial frontal, dorsal parietal, and occipital regions of both hemispheres (Inui et al. 2000). This activation map was used to compensate for the effect of experimental blocks, since the block order was fixed, not randomized (see Materials and Methods). Figure 4 showed the activation map of the effect of experimental blocks. A summary of activated clusters is shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Talairach coordinates of brain clusters
Cluster name
Talairach coordinate (mm)
Volume (mm3)
x
y
z
R MFG
36
41
32
473
R aIC
36
12
8
1,089
R mFC
9
−1
50
1,037
R PrCeS
27
−11
52
1,210
R PoCeG
57
−16
33
812
R PCC
14
−32
47
528
R IPS
29
−48
52
563
R Cal
27
−48
5
159
L MFG
−32
31
32
591
L aFO
−33
15
8
904
L mFC
−4
0
50
855
L pFO
−41
−4
10
895
L PrCeG
−41
−8
43
513
L PCC
−8
−32
47
520
L Cal
−7
−75
6
664
L Extr
−22
−76
−7
58
ACC
2
14
33
1,047
R right, L left, MFG middle frontal gyrus, aIC anterior insular cortex, mFC medial frontal cortex, PrCeS precentral sulcus, PoCeG postcentral gyrus, PCC posterior cingulated cortex, IPS intraparietal sulcus, Cal calcarine sulcus, aFO anterior frontal operculum, pFO posterior frontal operculum, PrCeG precentral gyrus, Extr extrastriate cortex, ACC anterior cingulated cortex
We subsequently performed an EEG–fMRI integrated analysis based on the time variation of the spectral power of the EEG data recorded at the FC3, P6 and CP2 electrodes, counterbalancing the effects of experimental blocks and motor responses in order to avoid overestimation of activation (see Materials and Methods). The activation maps with a linear contrast as [test > control] showed focused activations at each of the three electrodes (see the caption of Table 2 for definitions of the activation sites): the FC3 group activation sites at 2.5–4.4 Hz with a peak at 750 ms included the R hFEF, R IPS, R PoCeC, R aIC, L aFO and L pFO sites; the P6 group activation sites at 2.6–3.4 Hz with a peak at 600 ms included the R hFEF, L IPS, L PoCeC, R PoCeC, R vPrCeG and L vPrCeS sites; and the CP2 group activation sites at 2.8–4.0 Hz with a peak at 350 ms included the R IPS and L IPS sites. All activation sites are shown in Fig. 5, and their details are described in Table 2.
Table 2
Talairach coordinates of clusters corresponding to the FC3, P6, and CP2 electrodes
ROI name
Talairach coordinate (mm)
Volume (mm3)
x
y
z
FC3-group
 R aIC
34
12
9
47
 R hFEF
26
−12
53
119
 R PoCeC
53
−24
33
233
 R IPS
28
−49
52
163
 L aFO
−34
16
8
52
 L pFO
−41
−4
10
88
P6-group
 R vPrCeG
53
3
27
107
 R hFEF
25
−12
53
209
 R PoCeC-2
54
−23
32
209
 R PoCeC-1
48
−28
40
104
 L vPrCeS
−47
8
14
378
 L PoCeC
−43
−19
29
97
 L IPS
−23
−55
48
192
CP2-group
 R IPS
28
−49
52
343
 L IPS
−21
−56
49
151
R right, L left, aIC anterior insular cortex, hFEF human homologue of the frontal eye field (at the conjunction of the precentral sulcus and superior frontal sulcus), PoCeC postcentral cortex, IPS intraparietal sulcus, aFO anterior frontal operculum, pFO posterior frontal operculum, vPrCeG ventral precentral gyrus, vPrCeS ventral precentral sulcus

Discussion

Our results demonstrate that rapid neural dynamics arose before observers were aware of SPS induced by the Necker cube. EEG data simultaneously recorded during fMRI scanning and source estimates of the data showed rapid dynamics of neural substrates electrophysiologically related to SPS. In addition, our EEG–fMRI integrated analysis indicated that the oscillation-related regions were activated in accord with a sequence of 3–4 Hz EEG oscillatory modulations more in the test than in the control condition. This integrated analysis also demonstrated that a sequence of three groups of activations correlated with 3–4 Hz slow oscillatory modulations traveled from the anterior to posterior cortical regions sequentially prior to the presumable latency of SPS (Fig. 6). These findings are consistent with a previous study reporting the frontal-to-occipital synchrony or connectivity before SPS induced by the Necker cube (Shimaoka et al. 2010).
A large-scale meta-analysis of the precise functional neuroanatomy of the DAN indicated that the following brain regions should be included in the DAN (Toro et al. 2008): the IPS, ventral IPS, inferior parietal lobule, hFEF, inferior precentral sulcus (iPrCeS), supplementary motor area (SMA), pre SMA, dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), ventral occipital cortex, aIC, ventral hFEF, thalamus, and right cerebellum. In contrast, we found that the oscillation-related regions included the hFEF, IPS, vPrCeS/vPrCeS (that are included in the iPrCeS), aIC, FO, and PoCeC. Comparisons of these two sets of results indicated that almost all of the oscillation-related regions are included within the DAN, whereas only the FO and bilateral PoCeC were not. Thus, these oscillation-related regions can be regarded as parts of the DAN.

Neural dynamics along the DAN prior to SPS

Our findings indicate that a sequence of three prominent neural activities occur prior to the Necker cube induces SPS. These neural activities showed local maxima at 750 (FC3), 600 (P6), and 350 (CP2) ms before the button was pressed, indicating that these are peak latencies in response to SPS. Since almost all of the oscillation-related regions correlating the sequence of neural activities were within the DAN (Toro et al. 2008), and since this sequence traveled sequentially from anterior to posterior regions, our results confirmed our hypothesis, that rapidly changing neural dynamics arising along the DAN from frontal to parietal regions arise prior to SPS induced by the Necker cube.
Electrophysiology has shown that right parietal activity arose 50 ms before the onset of the Necker cube stimuli (Britz et al. 2009). Similarly, our findings indicate that parietal activity appears beginning approximately 400 ms before, or approximately 50 ms before the presumable onset latency of each percept of the Necker cube. Functional neuroimaging of BOLD signal latency analysis showed that right inferior frontal activation occurred 800 ms before button pressing in response to SPS by the dynamic dot quartet, whereas the left FO, the bilateral MT+, the right inferior parietal region and the left sensorimotor cortex were not activated prior to button pressing (Sterzer and Kleinschmidt 2007), suggesting that serial processes underlie multistable perception, but not providing any details of these processes.
In contrast to these earlier studies, our findings provide novel evidence of the details of serial processes underlying SPS. Several recent studies have described neural dynamics along the DAN. For example, the monkey FEF has been shown to transfer information to the V4 during an attentional task (Gregoriou et al. 2009). Moreover, functional neuroimaging has shown that, during voluntary control of visuospatial attention, the human FEF sends causal flows to the posterior parietal cortex, including the IPS and V4 (Bressler et al. 2008; Ozaki and Ogawa 2009). Neural dynamics along the DAN may be flexibly switched in accordance with a task demand in voluntary attentional control (Ozaki and Ogawa 2009).
Despite the low time resolution of fMRI, voluntary attentional control may be illustrated by a model of neural dynamics from the anterior (the FEF and other frontal areas) to the posterior (the IPS and other parietal or occipital areas) regions along the DAN. Functional neuroimaging of resting-state networks (RSN) has also revealed intrinsic neural dynamics along the DAN, functionally dissociating the DAN from other brain regions (Fox et al. 2006; Vincent et al. 2007). Moreover, simultaneous EEG–fMRI assays of RSNs suggested that these intrinsic neural dynamics may have rapid microstates on a fine time scale (Britz et al. 2010). Our hypothesis and results are consistent with these findings and models.
Finally, one may argue that not voluntary but involuntary attentional control can contribute to SPS and that SPS should be related to some neural correlates of involuntary attentional control. However, many previous studies have revealed that cortical regions related to involuntary attentional control are clearly dissociable from those related to voluntary attentional control, because neural substrates of involuntary attentional control are mainly in the ventral part of the cerebral cortex while those of voluntary attentional control are mainly in the dorsal part (see Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Corbetta et al. 2008 for review).
In summary, our results suggest that intrinsic neural dynamics along the DAN traveling from anterior to posterior regions, similar to the dynamics observed during voluntary attentional control, can change a status of voluntary attentional control and may subsequently trigger SPS (Fig. 7).

Interaction of the neural dynamics in the DAN revealed by the EEG–fMRI integrated analysis with representative cortical regions related to the Necker cube perception

Our EEG–fMRI integrated analysis indicated that neural dynamics before SPS travels along the DAN from the anterior to posterior cortical regions. This finding suggests that some interaction between such a traveling neural dynamics and functional structure of the DAN may occur before SPS.
As far as we have searched, there are only a few functional neuroimaging studies investigating functional localization of the neural correlates of SPS induced by the Necker cube. An earlier fMRI study by Inui and colleagues indicated that the bilateral dorsal premotor and parietal cortex would be important for the Necker cube perception (Inui et al. 2000). On the other hand, a recent study by Knapen and colleagues reported that the fronto-parietal pathway in the right hemisphere would play an important role in embodying the bistable percept itself (Knapen et al. 2011). However, these previous reports could not provide any evidence of the importance of traveling neural dynamics from the anterior to posterior cortical regions.
Perception of the Necker cube also can be related to the language function. A neuropsychological study reported that hemispatial neglect patients with higher verbal intelligence can copy the Necker cube better than patients with lower verbal intelligence (Seki et al. 2000). This fact supports a finding that the left ventral frontal cortex adjacent to the Broca’s language area was included in the FC3 group. Taken together with the finding of Knapen’s group (Knapen et al. 2011), the left ventral frontal activity reported in the current study must be more important for not the bistable perception, but SPS itself.
Thus, our results indicated that the anterior regions of the DAN and some parts of the left ventral frontal cortex supposed to be related to the language function were first activated, and that broadly-distributed regions of the DAN were activated, and that finally the bilateral posterior parietal regions within the DAN were activated. Such a neural dynamics traveling along the DAN may reflect a time course of fine-scale temporal structure of cognitive dynamics underlying SPS induced by the Necker cube.

Origin of multistability as a fundamental framework of SPS

Unlike the attention studies cited above, our study did not include any explicit voluntary attentional control. Nonetheless, our hypothesis and results indicate that the neural dynamics along the DAN from frontal to parietal regions appeared prior to SPS during multistable perception, as well as during voluntary attentional control. These findings suggest that similar mechanisms of the DAN underlie both SPS induced by the Necker cube and voluntary attentional control.
Two possible mechanisms of the DAN may be involved in Necker cube induced SPS: flexibility and/or fluctuation of the network. In regard to flexibility, there is evidence that neural oscillations across various frequency bands mediate self-organization in the human brain. For example, neural oscillations across broad frequency bands recorded by magnetoencelphalography (MEG) make small-world networks emerge dynamically and flexibly in response to task demands (Bassett et al. 2006). Indeed, theoretical studies have proposed that oscillatory phenomena involve a similar mechanism of self-organization of neural oscillators through synchronization (Shimizu and Yamaguchi 1987; Yamaguchi and Shimizu 1994). This hypothesis suggests that the FC3-, P6- and CP2-groups correlating with various kinds of oscillations may be embodied by a process of self-organization of neural oscillators and/or synchronization.
In contrast, theoretical studies have suggested various approaches to the fluctuation of networks. For example, neuronal fluctuations may be a source of stochastic behavior, i.e. multistability (Deco and Romo 2008). These fluctuations may lead to stochastic transition of a status of systems, i.e. stochastic resonance (Kitajo et al. 2003; Ward et al. 2006). Our findings suggest that the three activation groups appearing prior to SPS are mediated by neural oscillations, and that, as a network, they may easily transit from one to another by means such as stochastic resonance, together with some intrinsic noises in the human brain. Moreover, our findings suggest that such a transition through the three activation groups may result in a sequence of neural dynamics along the DAN from frontal to parietal regions.
There is considerable evidence about intrinsic and noise-induced fluctuations in the human brain (Fox et al. 2005; Fox and Raichle 2007; He and Raichle 2009), especially in the DAN (Fox et al. 2006; Vincent et al. 2007). This evidence indicates that intrinsic and noise-induced fluctuations in the DAN may give rise to sub-networks and embody neural dynamics along the DAN as a global flow. This would lead to fluctuations in voluntary attentional control, thus triggering SPS, especially by the Necker cube.
In contrast to binocular rivalry, ambiguous perception may be modulated by attentional or intentional control, although both are multistable (Meng and Tong 2004). Our findings may explain the difference between ambiguous perception by single figures and binocular rivalry. That is, ambiguous perception is mediated by self-organization and/or stochastic resonance of neural oscillations involved in attentional control that travel along the DAN, whereas binocular rivalry, although a similar stochastic process, arises mainly in the visual cortex and partly in a fronto-parietal circuit but not in the DAN (Sterzer and Rees 2008).
In conclusion, simultaneous EEG and fMRI evaluations of participants performing a multistable perception task with the Necker cube identified intrinsic neural dynamics driven by 3–4 Hz slow oscillations along the DAN from frontal to parietal regions prior to SPS induced by the Necker cube, thus confirming our working hypothesis. Our results indicate that such intrinsic neural dynamics may trigger SPS, and that they can be driven by self-organization and/or stochastic resonance of slow neural oscillations. These findings provide a novel explanation about why ambiguous perceptions can be modulated by voluntary attentional control whereas binocular rivalry cannot: the former is driven by a stochastic process in the DAN, whereas the latter is driven by a similar stochastic process but not within the DAN.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Yoko Takahashi and Ryohei Miyota for technical support. This study was partially supported by a MEXT (Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology) Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas “The study on the neural dynamics for understanding communication in terms of complex hetero systems (No.4103)” (21120005) and Hamano Life Science Research Foundation.

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 2.0 International License (https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Literature
go back to reference Allen PJ, Polizzi G, Krakow K, Fish DR, Lemieux L (1998) Identification of EEG events in the MR scanner: the problem of pulse artifact and a method for its subtraction. Neuroimage 8:229–239PubMedCrossRef Allen PJ, Polizzi G, Krakow K, Fish DR, Lemieux L (1998) Identification of EEG events in the MR scanner: the problem of pulse artifact and a method for its subtraction. Neuroimage 8:229–239PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Allen PJ, Josephs O, Turner R (2000) A method for removing imaging artifact from continuous EEG recorded during functional MRI. Neuroimage 12:230–239PubMedCrossRef Allen PJ, Josephs O, Turner R (2000) A method for removing imaging artifact from continuous EEG recorded during functional MRI. Neuroimage 12:230–239PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bassett DS, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Achard S, Duke T, Bullmore E (2006) Adaptive reconfiguration of fractal small-world human brain functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:19518–19523PubMedCrossRef Bassett DS, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Achard S, Duke T, Bullmore E (2006) Adaptive reconfiguration of fractal small-world human brain functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:19518–19523PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bressler SL, Tang W, Sylvester CM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M (2008) Top-down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. J Neurosci 28:10056–10061PubMedCrossRef Bressler SL, Tang W, Sylvester CM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M (2008) Top-down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. J Neurosci 28:10056–10061PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Britz J, Landis T, Michel CM (2009) Right parietal brain activity precedes perceptual alternation of bistable stimuli. Cereb Cortex 19:55–65PubMedCrossRef Britz J, Landis T, Michel CM (2009) Right parietal brain activity precedes perceptual alternation of bistable stimuli. Cereb Cortex 19:55–65PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Britz J, van de Ville D, Michel CM (2010) BOLD correlates of EEG topography reveal rapid resting-state network dynamics. Neuroimage 52:1162–1170 Britz J, van de Ville D, Michel CM (2010) BOLD correlates of EEG topography reveal rapid resting-state network dynamics. Neuroimage 52:1162–1170
go back to reference Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC (1994) Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr 18:192–205PubMedCrossRef Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC (1994) Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr 18:192–205PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215PubMedCrossRef Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL (2008) The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58:306–324PubMedCrossRef Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL (2008) The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58:306–324PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21PubMedCrossRef Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fox MD, Raichle ME (2007) Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:700–711PubMedCrossRef Fox MD, Raichle ME (2007) Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:700–711PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:9673–9678PubMedCrossRef Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:9673–9678PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME (2006) Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:10046–10051PubMedCrossRef Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME (2006) Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:10046–10051PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R (2009) High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science 324:1207–1210PubMedCrossRef Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R (2009) High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science 324:1207–1210PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Guntekin B, Basar E (2010) A new interpretation of P300 responses upon analysis of coherences. Cogn Neurodyn 4:107–118PubMedCrossRef Guntekin B, Basar E (2010) A new interpretation of P300 responses upon analysis of coherences. Cogn Neurodyn 4:107–118PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference He BJ, Raichle ME (2009) The fMRI signal, slow cortical potential and consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 13:302–309PubMedCrossRef He BJ, Raichle ME (2009) The fMRI signal, slow cortical potential and consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 13:302–309PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Inui T, Tanaka S, Okada T, Nishizawa S, Katayama M, Konishi J (2000) Neural substrates for depth perception of the Necker cube; a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in human subjects. Neurosci Lett 282:145–148PubMedCrossRef Inui T, Tanaka S, Okada T, Nishizawa S, Katayama M, Konishi J (2000) Neural substrates for depth perception of the Necker cube; a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in human subjects. Neurosci Lett 282:145–148PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Isoglu-Alkac U, Basar-Eroglu C, Ademoglu A, Demiralp T, Miener M, Stadler M (1998) Analysis of the electroencephalographic activity during the Necker cube reversals by means of the wavelet transform. Biol Cybern 79:437–442PubMedCrossRef Isoglu-Alkac U, Basar-Eroglu C, Ademoglu A, Demiralp T, Miener M, Stadler M (1998) Analysis of the electroencephalographic activity during the Necker cube reversals by means of the wavelet transform. Biol Cybern 79:437–442PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Isoglu-Alkac U, Basar-Eroglu C, Ademoglu A, Demiralp T, Miener M, Stadler M (2000) Alpha activity decreases during the perception of Necker cube reversals: an application of wavelet transform. Biol Cybern 82:313–320PubMedCrossRef Isoglu-Alkac U, Basar-Eroglu C, Ademoglu A, Demiralp T, Miener M, Stadler M (2000) Alpha activity decreases during the perception of Necker cube reversals: an application of wavelet transform. Biol Cybern 82:313–320PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jurcak V, Tsuzuki D, Dan I (2007) 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems. Neuroimage 34:1600–1611PubMedCrossRef Jurcak V, Tsuzuki D, Dan I (2007) 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems. Neuroimage 34:1600–1611PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kitajo K, Nozaki D, Ward LM, Yamamoto Y (2003) Behavioral stochastic resonance within the human brain. Phys Rev Lett 90:218103PubMedCrossRef Kitajo K, Nozaki D, Ward LM, Yamamoto Y (2003) Behavioral stochastic resonance within the human brain. Phys Rev Lett 90:218103PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Knapen T, Brascamp J, Pearson J, van Ee R, Blake R (2011) The role of frontal and parietal brain areas in bistable perception. J Neurosci 31:10293–10301PubMedCrossRef Knapen T, Brascamp J, Pearson J, van Ee R, Blake R (2011) The role of frontal and parietal brain areas in bistable perception. J Neurosci 31:10293–10301PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kornmeier J, Bach M (2004) Early neural activity in Necker-cube reversal: evidence for low-level processing of a gestalt phenomenon. Psychophysiology 41:1–8PubMedCrossRef Kornmeier J, Bach M (2004) Early neural activity in Necker-cube reversal: evidence for low-level processing of a gestalt phenomenon. Psychophysiology 41:1–8PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kornmeier J, Bach M (2005) The Necker cube—an ambiguous figure disambiguated in early visual processing. Vision Res 45:955–960PubMedCrossRef Kornmeier J, Bach M (2005) The Necker cube—an ambiguous figure disambiguated in early visual processing. Vision Res 45:955–960PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Laufs H, Daunizeau J, Carmichael DW, Kleinschmidt A (2008) Recent advances in recording electrophysiological data simultaneously with magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 40:515–528PubMedCrossRef Laufs H, Daunizeau J, Carmichael DW, Kleinschmidt A (2008) Recent advances in recording electrophysiological data simultaneously with magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 40:515–528PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Leopold DA, Logothetis NK (1999) Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception. Trends Cogn Sci 3:254–264PubMedCrossRef Leopold DA, Logothetis NK (1999) Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception. Trends Cogn Sci 3:254–264PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mandelkow H, Halder P, Boesiger P, Brandeis D (2006) Synchronization facilitates removal of MRI artefacts from concurrent EEG recordings and increases usable bandwidth. Neuroimage 32:1120–1126PubMedCrossRef Mandelkow H, Halder P, Boesiger P, Brandeis D (2006) Synchronization facilitates removal of MRI artefacts from concurrent EEG recordings and increases usable bandwidth. Neuroimage 32:1120–1126PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mathes B, Struber D, Stadler MA, Basar-Eroglu C (2006) Voluntary control of Necker cube reversals modulates the EEG delta- and gamma-band response. Neurosci Lett 402:145–149PubMedCrossRef Mathes B, Struber D, Stadler MA, Basar-Eroglu C (2006) Voluntary control of Necker cube reversals modulates the EEG delta- and gamma-band response. Neurosci Lett 402:145–149PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meng M, Tong F (2004) Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. J Vis 4:539–551PubMedCrossRef Meng M, Tong F (2004) Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. J Vis 4:539–551PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mizuhara H, Yamaguchi Y (2007) Human cortical circuits for central executive function emerge by theta phase synchronization. Neuroimage 36:232–244PubMedCrossRef Mizuhara H, Yamaguchi Y (2007) Human cortical circuits for central executive function emerge by theta phase synchronization. Neuroimage 36:232–244PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mizuhara H, Wang LQ, Kobayashi K, Yamaguchi Y (2004) A long-range cortical network emerging with theta oscillation in a mental task. NeuroReport 15:1233–1238PubMedCrossRef Mizuhara H, Wang LQ, Kobayashi K, Yamaguchi Y (2004) A long-range cortical network emerging with theta oscillation in a mental task. NeuroReport 15:1233–1238PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mizuhara H, Wang LQ, Kobayashi K, Yamaguchi Y (2005) Long-range EEG phase synchronization during an arithmetic task indexes a coherent cortical network simultaneously measured by fMRI. Neuroimage 27:553–563PubMedCrossRef Mizuhara H, Wang LQ, Kobayashi K, Yamaguchi Y (2005) Long-range EEG phase synchronization during an arithmetic task indexes a coherent cortical network simultaneously measured by fMRI. Neuroimage 27:553–563PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Murata T, Matsui N, Miyauchi S, Kakita Y, Yanagida T (2003) Discrete stochastic process underlying perceptual rivalry. NeuroReport 14:1347–1352PubMed Murata T, Matsui N, Miyauchi S, Kakita Y, Yanagida T (2003) Discrete stochastic process underlying perceptual rivalry. NeuroReport 14:1347–1352PubMed
go back to reference Nakatani H, van Leeuwen C (2005) Individual differences in perceptual switching rates; the role of occipital alpha and frontal theta band activity. Biol Cybern 93:343–354PubMedCrossRef Nakatani H, van Leeuwen C (2005) Individual differences in perceptual switching rates; the role of occipital alpha and frontal theta band activity. Biol Cybern 93:343–354PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nakatani H, van Leeuwen C (2006) Transient synchrony of distant brain areas and perceptual switching in ambiguous figures. Biol Cybern 94:445–457PubMedCrossRef Nakatani H, van Leeuwen C (2006) Transient synchrony of distant brain areas and perceptual switching in ambiguous figures. Biol Cybern 94:445–457PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nakatani H, Orlandi N, van Leeuwen C (2011) Precisely timed oculomotor and parietal EEG activity in perceptual switching. Cogn Neurodyn 5:399–409PubMedCrossRef Nakatani H, Orlandi N, van Leeuwen C (2011) Precisely timed oculomotor and parietal EEG activity in perceptual switching. Cogn Neurodyn 5:399–409PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ogawa S, Tank DW, Menon R, Ellermann JM, Kim SG, Merkle H, Ugurbil K (1992) Intrinsic signal changes accompanying sensory stimulation: functional brain mapping with magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:5951–5955PubMedCrossRef Ogawa S, Tank DW, Menon R, Ellermann JM, Kim SG, Merkle H, Ugurbil K (1992) Intrinsic signal changes accompanying sensory stimulation: functional brain mapping with magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:5951–5955PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ozaki TJ (2011) Frontal-to-parietal top-down causal streams along the dorsal attention network exclusively mediate voluntary orienting of attention. PLoS ONE 6:e20079PubMedCrossRef Ozaki TJ (2011) Frontal-to-parietal top-down causal streams along the dorsal attention network exclusively mediate voluntary orienting of attention. PLoS ONE 6:e20079PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ozaki TJ, Ogawa S (2009) Causality analysis defines neural streams of orienting and holding of attention. NeuroReport 20:1371–1375PubMedCrossRef Ozaki TJ, Ogawa S (2009) Causality analysis defines neural streams of orienting and holding of attention. NeuroReport 20:1371–1375PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pascual-Marqui RD (2002) Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol 24(Suppl D):5–12 Pascual-Marqui RD (2002) Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol 24(Suppl D):5–12
go back to reference Seki K, Ishiai S, Koyama Y, Sato S, Hirabayashi H, Inaki K (2000) Why are some patients with severe neglect able to copy a cube? The significance of verbal intelligence. Neuropsychologia 38:1466–1472PubMedCrossRef Seki K, Ishiai S, Koyama Y, Sato S, Hirabayashi H, Inaki K (2000) Why are some patients with severe neglect able to copy a cube? The significance of verbal intelligence. Neuropsychologia 38:1466–1472PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shen L, Zeng ZL, Huang PY, Li Q, Mu J, Huang XQ, Lui S, Gong QY, Xie P (2009) Temporal cortex participates in spontaneous perceptual reversal. NeuroReport 20:647–651PubMedCrossRef Shen L, Zeng ZL, Huang PY, Li Q, Mu J, Huang XQ, Lui S, Gong QY, Xie P (2009) Temporal cortex participates in spontaneous perceptual reversal. NeuroReport 20:647–651PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shimaoka D, Kitajo K, Kaneko K, Yamaguchi Y (2010) Transient process of cortical activity during Necker cube perception: from local clusters to global synchrony. Nonlinear Biomed Phys 4(Suppl 1):S7PubMedCrossRef Shimaoka D, Kitajo K, Kaneko K, Yamaguchi Y (2010) Transient process of cortical activity during Necker cube perception: from local clusters to global synchrony. Nonlinear Biomed Phys 4(Suppl 1):S7PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shimizu H, Yamaguchi Y (1987) Synergetic computer and holonics—information dynamics of a semantic computer. Phys Scr 36:970–985CrossRef Shimizu H, Yamaguchi Y (1987) Synergetic computer and holonics—information dynamics of a semantic computer. Phys Scr 36:970–985CrossRef
go back to reference Slotnick SD, Yantis S (2005) Common neural substrates for the control and effects of visual attention and perceptual bistability. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 24:97–108PubMedCrossRef Slotnick SD, Yantis S (2005) Common neural substrates for the control and effects of visual attention and perceptual bistability. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 24:97–108PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sterzer P, Kleinschmidt A (2007) A neural basis for inference in perceptual ambiguity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:323–328PubMedCrossRef Sterzer P, Kleinschmidt A (2007) A neural basis for inference in perceptual ambiguity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:323–328PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sterzer P, Rees G (2008) A neural basis for percept stabilization in binocular rivalry. J Cogn Neurosci 20:389–399PubMedCrossRef Sterzer P, Rees G (2008) A neural basis for percept stabilization in binocular rivalry. J Cogn Neurosci 20:389–399PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Thieme Medical Publisher, New York Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Thieme Medical Publisher, New York
go back to reference Toro R, Fox PT, Paus T (2008) Functional coactivation map of the human brain. Cereb Cortex 18:2553–2559PubMedCrossRef Toro R, Fox PT, Paus T (2008) Functional coactivation map of the human brain. Cereb Cortex 18:2553–2559PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, Van Essen DC, Zempel JM, Snyder LH, Corbetta M, Raichle ME (2007) Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 447:83–86PubMedCrossRef Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, Van Essen DC, Zempel JM, Snyder LH, Corbetta M, Raichle ME (2007) Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 447:83–86PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ward LM, Doesburg SM, Kitajo K, MacLean SE, Roggeveen AB (2006) Neural synchrony in stochastic resonance, attention, and consciousness. Can J Exp Psychol 60:319–326PubMedCrossRef Ward LM, Doesburg SM, Kitajo K, MacLean SE, Roggeveen AB (2006) Neural synchrony in stochastic resonance, attention, and consciousness. Can J Exp Psychol 60:319–326PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yamaguchi Y, Shimizu H (1994) Pattern-recognition with figure-ground separation by generation of coherent oscillations. Neural Netw 7:49–63CrossRef Yamaguchi Y, Shimizu H (1994) Pattern-recognition with figure-ground separation by generation of coherent oscillations. Neural Netw 7:49–63CrossRef
go back to reference Yuval-Greenberg S, Tomer O, Keren AS, Nelken I, Deouell LY (2008) Transient induced gamma-band response in EEG as a manifestation of miniature saccades. Neuron 58:429–441PubMedCrossRef Yuval-Greenberg S, Tomer O, Keren AS, Nelken I, Deouell LY (2008) Transient induced gamma-band response in EEG as a manifestation of miniature saccades. Neuron 58:429–441PubMedCrossRef
Metadata
Title
Traveling EEG slow oscillation along the dorsal attention network initiates spontaneous perceptual switching
Authors
Takashi J. Ozaki
Naoyuki Sato
Keiichi Kitajo
Yoshiaki Someya
Kimitaka Anami
Hiroaki Mizuhara
Seiji Ogawa
Yoko Yamaguchi
Publication date
01-04-2012
Publisher
Springer Netherlands
Published in
Cognitive Neurodynamics / Issue 2/2012
Print ISSN: 1871-4080
Electronic ISSN: 1871-4099
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11571-012-9196-y

Other articles of this Issue 2/2012

Cognitive Neurodynamics 2/2012 Go to the issue