Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Organisationspsychologie (GIO) 3/2020

Open Access 06-08-2020 | Hauptbeiträge - Offener Teil

A comparison of business meeting practices in Germany and Spain

Authors: Cornelius J. König, Nale Lehmann-Willenbrock, Annika L. Meinecke, Jasmin Winter

Published in: Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Organisationspsychologie (GIO) | Issue 3/2020

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

This article in the journal Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. (GIO) presents a study on meetings, an important part of contemporary organizational life. What happens in meetings affects individual employee experiences, team processes, and organizational functioning. However, to date little is known regarding cross-cultural differences in meeting practices. This study leverages an organizational sample (N = 488) with a German and a Spanish site to compare how pre-meeting talk, meeting design, voice in meetings, and meeting follow-up actions differ across the two cultures. Hypotheses were derived from prior intercultural theory (i.e., the GLOBE study). Following Open Science principles, the study was pre-registered. Contrary to our expectations, we found no significant differences in meeting practices between monocultural German and Spanish workplace meetings. These findings suggest that cultural differences in workplace attitudes and work practices may be diminishing in an increasingly global workplace. We sketch implications for meeting science and cross-cultural research on business practices more broadly.
Meetings exist in nearly every organization regardless of organizational culture, industry, or size. Organizations devote up to 15% of their personnel budgets to meetings (Rogelberg 2019). They have become a key component of employees’ everyday workplace experiences, with an overall employee average of six hours per week spent in meetings and estimates of up to 80% of managers’ work time consumed by various meetings (for an overview, see Lehmann-Willenbrock et al. 2018; Mroz et al. 2018). Hence, meetings can be viewed as the place where organizational life happens.
Yet, the ways in which organizational life is shaped by meetings, and the way specific meeting practices are applied, may vary across different cultural settings (cf. Gelfand et al. 2017). Previous empirical work in this regard is scarce, but points to potential differences in the way meetings are conducted in different national cultures as well as the need to consider culture when studying business meetings (Angouri 2010; Aritz and Walker 2010; Du-Babcock and Tanaka 2013; van Eerde and Azar 2020; van Eerde and Buengeler 2015; Joardar et al. 2019; Köhler et al. 2012; Lü 2018; Murata 2014; Paletz et al. 2018). While the practice of meetings is widespread across different types of organizations and cultures, the specific aspects of how meetings are designed, how people behave as they enter the meeting space, and what happens during and after meetings may differ depending on the respective cultural setting. Comparisons of cultural practices more broadly, most prominently the GLOBE study (House and Javidan 2004), indicate that this may be the case.
To address this idea, we present empirical findings regarding potential differences in meeting practices when comparing German to Spanish monocultural face-to-face meetings. We draw from cross-cultural differences indicated by the GLOBE study as well as previous findings regarding relevant meeting practices and test our hypotheses in a large organizational sample with employees in Germany and Spain. Our findings contribute much-needed cross-cultural insights to meeting science, relieve the often U.S.-centric study focus in previous work, and offer interesting discussion points for research and practice.

1 Culture and meeting practices

Considering a country as a cultural setting (Hofstede 2001), this study sets out to explore possible meeting differences and similarities between Germany and Spain with regard to pre-meeting small talk, meeting design characteristics, voice in meetings, and meeting follow-up actions. Conceptually, our study is informed by the well-known GLOBE country cluster framework (House and Javidan 2004). The GLOBE project (House and Javidan 2004) addressed the fundamental question of how and why societal cultures differ. The authors were especially interesting in the link between societal culture and organizational practices as well as leadership characteristics. Cultural values and practices were collected from 62 nations across the globe. These cultural values and practices share common ground with previous classifications by Hofstede (2001) and span a total of nine dimensions, namely performance orientation, assertiveness, future orientation, humane orientation, institutional collectivism, in-group collectivism, gender egalitarianism, power distance, and uncertainty avoidance.
Brought to the meeting context, we assume that describing the respective countries in terms of these cultural dimensions helps us to understand similarities and differences in how meetings are designed, how participants behave before and during meetings, and how they follow-up on meetings. Comparing the GLOBE values of Germany and Spain, the most striking differences emerge in in-group collectivism, uncertainty avoidance, power distance, and future orientation (Brodbeck and Frese 2008; O’Connell et al. 2008). In the following, we outline how these four key dimensions relate to our focal outcome variables.

1.1 In-group collectivism and pre-meeting small talk

In cultures with a high degree of in-group collectivism, members show “pride, loyalty and cohesiveness in their organizations or families” (House and Javidan 2004, p. 12). Close personal relationships are valued and communicating with the members of one’s own group is considered very important (Gelfand et al. 2004; Javidan et al. 2006). Comparing Germany and Spain, the GLOBE study disclosed that Spanish in comparison to German respondents reported higher values of in-group collectivism (House et al. 2004).
Relating to workplace meetings, in-group collectivisms enables insights into the relational value that is placed on workplace gatherings. Meeting scholars noted previously that in addition to serving formal goals, meetings also provide a place to socialize, network, and build relationships (Kieffer 1988; Lehmann-Willenbrock et al. 2018; Olien et al. 2015). One way to establish and maintain close workplace relationships is to engage in pre-meeting small talk, defined as the verbal and behavioral interactions that occur before the meeting begins and that do not have an explicit work or task focus (Mirivel and Tracy 2005). Typical small talk topics include the weather, family, or sports. Scholarly interest in pre-meeting talk is slowly growing, but largely driven by efforts stemming from the U.S. (Allen et al. 2014; Yoerger et al. 2015). For example, previous research showed that pre-meeting small talk is related to meeting effectiveness (Allen et al. 2014). Research on small talk in organizations more generally further showed that small talk fulfills important social functions as it can strengthen work group cohesion and build the ground base for other types of more formal interactions (Holmes 2000).
In terms of cross-cultural comparisons, previous research is rather sparse and did not specifically contrast German and Spanish meetings. Extrapolating from research on U.S.-American and German meetings, Germans have been described as “eschewing small talk” (Köhler et al. 2012, p. 175). Regarding within-meeting processes, findings further showed that Germans engage in little relational talk during meetings (Köhler et al. 2012; Lehmann-Willenbrock et al. 2014). Anecdotal evidence for the Germans’ apparent aversion of small talk is also ubiquitous in online articles (e.g., Houghton 2017), and popular management books frequently describe Germans as efficient and direct talkers whereas Spaniards are said to be more chatty (Lewis 2006).
In sum, as respondents from countries scoring high on in-group collectivisms value cohesion within the work group, we assume that they favor pre-meeting small talk more than respondents from low in-group collectivistic countries. We therefore expect pre-meeting small talk to be more prevalent in Spanish than in German meetings. Our first hypothesis posits:
H1
There will be more pre-meeting small talk in Spain than in Germany.

1.2 Uncertainty avoidance and meeting design characteristics

Uncertainty avoidance describes “the extent to which members of an organization or society strive to avoid uncertainty by relying on established social norms, rituals, and bureaucratic practices” (House and Javidan 2004, p. 11). In other words, members of high uncertainty avoidant cultures tend to fear the unknown and therefore prefer rules and regulations that provide a guidance in coping with the unpredictable. For everyday working life in organizations, this means that simple processes and coarse strategies are preferred in low uncertainty cultures, whereas emphasis is placed on formal strategies and detailed processes in countries with high levels of uncertainty (Javidan et al. 2006).
Findings from the GLOBE project disclosed that Germans have a much higher tendency to avoid uncertainty in comparison to Spaniards (House et al. 2004), which is in line with earlier cross-cultural research (Hofstede 2001). Turning to organizational meetings, the meeting leader (i.e., the person responsible for calling and facilitating the meeting) can increase certainty and orderliness by thoroughly preparing for the meeting and structuring the meeting process (Leach et al. 2009; Odermatt et al. 2013). Important meeting design characteristics include using an agenda, making sure that the meeting starts on time, choosing an appropriate meeting space, and only inviting those participants who are necessary to the meeting. Research showed that these design characteristic are vital to meeting effectiveness and meeting satisfaction (Leach et al. 2009).
Previous cross-cultural research showed that German meetings are rather formal (Köhler and Gölz 2015; Meyer 1993). Special attention is given to a detailed meeting agenda, meaning that agenda items are processed sequentially and with high accuracy (Köhler and Gölz 2015). Germans further take care not to exceed the scheduled meeting duration (Meyer 1993), manifested in procedural statements that structure the meeting process (Lehmann-Willenbrock et al. 2014). In Spain, on the other hand, a spirit of adventure and associated flexibility are culturally anchored (O’Connell et al. 2008). Although this does not imply that participants follow no meeting agenda (van Eerde and Buengeler 2015), we assume that overall Spaniards tend to be more comfortable with uncertainty during meetings. Based on these differences in uncertainty avoidance, we hypothesize:
H2
More meeting design characteristics will be found in Germany than in Spain.

1.3 Power distance and voice during meetings

Power distance is defined as “the degree to which members of an organization or society expect and agree that power should be stratified and concentrated at higher levels of an organization or government” (House and Javidan 2004, p. 12). High power distance has been linked to hierarchical thinking, higher levels of authority, and obedience (Carl et al. 2004). Likewise, participatory decision-making is rather seen as a waste of time and not welcomed in high power distance cultures (Carl et al. 2004).
Findings from the GLOBE project revealed that Spain scored slightly higher on power distance than Germany (House et al. 2004). Transferred to organizational meetings, higher power distance implies that the meeting leader is a person of authority who not only structures the meeting process but also is responsible for decision-making. As such, meeting leaders can use the meeting to demonstrate their power and status (e.g., Rogelberg et al. 2014). Painting a more extreme picture, the meeting leader might even control meeting participation and avoid discussing differing views. Creating such a high-power meeting environment likely discourages meeting participants from showing voice. Following definitions by Hirschman (1970) and Morrison (2014), voice is the informal and discretionary communication by an employee of ideas, concerns, information about problems, or opinions about work-related issues. These voice statements challenge the status quo, are directed at the people who can take appropriate action (i.e., typically the supervisor), and are constructive in nature (see also Van Dyne and LePine 1998).
Although German workplace meetings are described as structured and organized, they are typically less hierarchical than assumed (Hedderich 1999). The need to defend formal authority is low in Germany (Köhler and Gölz 2015) and previous research showed that Germans are expected to voice their opinions during meetings and to take on an active role in decision making (Köhler et al. 2012; see also Yin 2002). Seen through this lens, we assume that German meeting participants are more likely to speak up with their own opinions and concerns than Spanish meeting participants who are more inclined to promote group cohesion and in-group collectivism. Our third hypothesis thus states:
H3
There will be more voice in German than in Spanish meetings.

1.4 Future orientation and meeting follow-up actions

Finally, this study sets out to explore the role of future orientation and workplace meetings. Future orientation describes “the degree to which individuals in organizations or societies engage in future-oriented behaviors such as planning, investing in the future, and delaying individual or collective gratification” (House and Javidan 2004, p. 12). Higher levels of future orientation are related to systematic planning and thinking in long-term intervals. Cultures low in future orientation are more opportunistic and less systematic in their planning meaning that people prefer to live in the here and now instead of making detailed plans for the future (Javidan et al. 2006).
The GLOBE project showed that Germany scores higher on future orientation than Spain (House et al. 2004). Workplace meetings are fundamental to organizational functioning and contribute to future orientation by providing structure to decision-making and tracking organizational or departmental goals (Lehmann-Willenbrock et al. 2018). During organizational meetings, we expect future orientation to manifest in detailed action plans that outline who is responsible for which task. Thus, the way meetings are followed-up on indicates future orientation by making sure that a meeting is not an isolated single event but embedded in the employees’ workflow and tasks (Aksoy-Burkert and König 2015). In this sense, meeting follow-up actions seamlessly merge with preparing for the next meeting (Tropman 2003).
Köhler et al. (2012) could show that German meeting are often conducted with the intention to come up with long-term solutions that have a high level of detail. This tendency likely carries over into concrete post meeting action plans. In Latin-European countries like Spain, on the other hand, good interpersonal relationships are seen as more important than schedules (Hickson and Hugh 1995). The tendency to live in the moment and changing plans at short notice likely leads to less follow-up actions in Spanish workplace meetings. Our final hypothesis thus posits:
H4
There will be more meeting follow-up actions after German meetings compared to Spanish meetings.

2 Method

2.1 Open science

Following guidelines for Open Science, all hypotheses, variables, and analyses were pre-registered before conducting this study. The preregistration is available at http://​aspredicted.​org/​blind.​php?​x=​6j2uw3. The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors to any qualified researcher upon request.

2.2 Participants

Data were gathered in a large international aerospace company, which has offices in Germany and Spain. The participating company supported this research project because informal complaints of too many meetings were very common, including complaints about problems when interacting with colleagues from the other country in meetings. Official meeting guidelines did not exist. Participation was voluntary and participants retained the right to opt out of the data gathering at any point. The sampling procedure was approved by the company’s work council. A link to an online questionnaire was distributed among 1640 employees, 800 of whom worked at the German site and 840 at the Spanish site. Participants were primarily engineers.
Five hundred and twenty-five employees responded (a response rate of 31%). After excluding 30 participants who had no German (or Spanish) nationality, six participants who mentioned that they did not follow the instructions, and one participant with an unrealistic old age of 82, the final sample consisted of 488 participants (254 German and 234 Spanish respondents). Of these, 86 were women and 397 men, which is consistent with the typical gender distribution of the workforce in this particular industry. Participants were on average 43.65 years old (SD = 9.81) and worked for the company for 15.59 years (SD = 10.12).
Meetings in this organization were fairly large and time consuming. They contained about nine participants on average (M = 8.7 meeting attendees, SD = 4.4), which is consistent with informal accounts of the meeting culture in this organization. On average, meetings took 79.8 min (SD = 49.4).

2.3 Measures

All questionnaires were in English, which is the official language of the company where we collected the data. Participants were given the following instruction: “For the following questions, please think of a typical meeting with your manager on site that he/she leads. We define meeting as a routine team gathering that was scheduled in advance, takes place on a regular basis, has a minimum of three participants and a duration of 15 min up to three hours. When answering the questions, please refer only to a typical LOCAL face-to-face team meeting with colleagues from your country and your manager on site”. Thus, we specifically asked participants to focus on monocultural meetings in order to avoid possible confounding effects.
Pre-meeting small talk
was assessed with the eight item small talk subscale of the pre-meeting talk scale (Allen et al. 2014). A sample item is “Before a meeting begins, to what extent do you do the following: Discuss a sporting event”. The response format ranged from 1 (not at all) to 5 (to a great extent).
Meeting design characteristics
were measured with a seven item scale from the Zurich Meeting Questionnaire (ZMQ, Odermatt et al. 2016). Items either were answered on a dichotomous yes/no scale (e.g., for the item “The meeting ends at the agreed time”) or on a five-point scale (e.g., for the item “The goals of the meeting are clearly defined”, with the scale ranging from 0 to 0.25, to 0.5, to 0.75, and to 1). An index (see Streiner 2003) was created by summing up all items.
Voice
was measured with the scale developed by Allen and Rogelberg (2013). It contains seven items, and respondents have to describe how often the meeting leader shows a specific behavior during the meeting, for example “Ensure that each employee has an opportunity to speak”. Response options ranged from 1 (never) to 5 (always).
Meeting follow-up actions
We developed our own measurement for meeting follow-up actions as follows. First, we searched for potential topics that were mentioned in (a) meeting guide books and articles (e.g., Carlozzi 1999; Hartmann et al. 2002; Haynes 2006; Tobia and Becker 1990; Tropman 2003) and (b) in the academic literature (Aksoy-Burkert and König 2015). Second, we discussed our insights with participants at a research colloquium. Two members of the author team then developed seven items that can be found in the Appendix. Participants answered on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (to a great extent).
To test the homogeneity of our new scale, we conducted a principal component analysis, extracting a single factor. Factor loadings varied between 0.65 and 0.80, and the factor explained 55.5% of the variance, which supports a one-factor solution. An additional reliability analysis revealed a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.86. for the full sample (αGerman sample = 0.85; αSpanish sample = 0.87).
Additional variables
To ensure that our sample was similar to previous studies, we also aimed at replicating previously shown relationships of the variables pre-meeting small talk, meeting design characteristics, and voice in meetings with a main outcome variable in the field of meeting research, i.e. meeting effectiveness (cf. Allen et al. 2014; Odermatt et al. 2016; Allen and Rogelberg 2013). Meeting effectiveness was assessed with a three-item measure from Leach et al. (2009), that asks for ratings of the effectiveness of a typical meeting in terms of goal achievement (e.g., “achieving your own work goals”). Participants rated the items on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (extremely ineffective) to 5 (extremely effective). Finally, we asked for meeting length and number of meeting attendees as well as for some demographic information.

3 Results

Table 1 shows the correlations between all study variables. Both meeting design characteristics and voice in meetings significantly correlated with meeting effectiveness (r = 0.46 for meeting design characteristics, similar to Odermatt et al. 2016; r = 0.47 for voice in meetings, similar to Allen and Rogelberg 2013). Furthermore, meeting follow-up actions also correlated with meeting effectiveness (r = 0.57). However, pre-meeting small talk was unrelated to meeting effectiveness (r = 0.02, unlike Allen et al. (2014), who found a correlation of 0.28).
Table 1
Correlations Among Study Variables
 
1
2
3
4
5
6
1. Country
     
2. Pre-meeting small talk
0.04
0.83
    
3. Meeting design characteristics
−0.06
−0.02
a
   
4. Voice in meetings
0.05
0.01
0.30**
0.88
  
5. Follow-up actions
−0.02
0.00
0.50**
0.32**
0.86
 
6. Meeting effectiveness
0.06
0.02
0.46**
0.47**
0.57**
0.90
N = 488. Country coding: 1 = Germany, 2 = Spain. Cronbach’s alphas are shown in the diagonal (in italics) where applicable
**p < 0.01, two-tailed
aCronbach’s alpha not applicable because meeting design characteristics is an index and not a scale (see Streiner 2003)
Table 2 shows the means and standard deviations of the four main variables for both countries. As can be seen, the means of both samples were fairly similar. A MANOVA showed no significant differences between German and Spanish meetings, Wilks’ λ = 0.99, F(4, 483) = 1.10, p = 0.36. Additional t-tests, separately run for each variable, did not result in significant differences either (see last row of Table 2). Hence, all our study hypotheses were refuted by the findings.
Table 2
Differences in Study Variables Among German and Spanish Respondents
 
Germany
Spain
Cohen’s d
t-tests
t value
Pre-meeting small talka
1.91 (0.62)
1.96 (0.64)
−0.08
p= 0.42
−0.86
Meeting design characteristicsb
5.05 (1.31)
4.90 (1.31)
0.20
p= 0.20
1.28
Voice in meetingsc
3.75 (0.80)
3.82 (0.78)
−0.09
p= 0.30
−1.03
Follow-up actions
3.36 (0.79)
3.33 (0.79)
0.04
p= 0.68
0.42
nGerman sample = 254, nSpanish sample = 234. Standard deviations are shown in parentheses. df = 486
aIn the original study by Allen et al. (2014), the mean was 2.03 (SD = 0.95), thus very similar
bIn Study 1 of the original study by Odermatt et al. (2016), the mean was 5.80 (SD = 1.50), in their Study 2 5.68 (SD = 1.46), and in their Study 3 4.90 (SD = 1.06), which indicates that the means of our samples were in the expected range
cIn the original study by Allen and Rogelberg (2013), the mean was 3.47 (SD = 0.95), thus only slightly lower than our means

4 Discussion

In sum, this study showed that meeting practices, in terms of pre-meeting small talk, design characteristics, voice, and meeting follow-up actions, did not differ meaningfully across the two national cultures—i.e., Germany and Spain—investigated here. Counter to previous assumptions and our hypotheses, these findings suggest more similarities than differences across cultures, based on our survey data gathered in a globally operating company.

4.1 Theoretical and practical implications

Previous work has emphasized cross-cultural differences in workplace values, perceptions, norms, and practices (e.g., Gibson and Zellmer-Bruhn 2002; Köhler and Gölz 2015; van Eerde and Buengeler 2015). We expected differences in meeting experiences across the German and Spanish meetings included in our sample accordingly, but the results tell a different story. One possible explanation for the lack of cross-cultural differences observed in the present study concerns the globalization argument. Specifically, differences in employees’ everyday experiences across cultures may be diminishing given the contemporary global workplace. Work related practices such as human resources management, marketing, and organizational development are becoming more similar across the globe, and as a result, individual work experiences tend to become more similar than different across different national cultures (e.g., Evans et al. 2011). Meetings in particular may be a practice that is becoming more aligned across cultures due to globalization trends. Yet, we would like to acknowledge that our findings are only beginning to address the cross-cultural research gap in meeting science and clearly more research is needed to fully understand the magnitude of this global trend.
Another explanation for the lack of cross-cultural differences found in our study may lie in the interplay between national and organizational culture. Our decision to survey employees of the same organization, with sites in both Germany and Spain, is in line with previous cross-cultural research, particularly Hofstede’s (2001) pioneering work on cultural dimensions, which was based on a large international sample of employees working at IBM. When considering different national branches of the same organization, as in the present investigation, corporate practices related to meetings may have likened business practices across different cultural contexts. In other words, organizational culture might have trumped national culture. This view can be linked to a more fundamental discussion on the role of national culture recently put forward in cross-cultural research. Taras et al. (2016) performed a meta-analysis of more than 500 studies and found that national boundaries are not the most ideal aggregate for describing cultures. They found more variation within rather than between countries and consequently suggest to focus on cultures of socio-economic classes, professions, and free versus oppressed societies instead of using national boundaries as a proxy for culture (Taras et al. 2016). Combining these findings with the findings of the present study suggests that more traditional conceptualizations of national culture only have limited effects in modern workplaces—in particular, if we want to understand how employees describe and experience their meetings. Future meeting research should explore cross-cultural differences using samples from different organizations and industries to more fully explore this idea. At this point, we can only speculate as to whether organizational culture might be of greater impact than national culture in determining meeting practices.
In terms of practical implications, the current study contributes to a growing body of evidence-based meeting practices. Specifically, our findings highlight that meeting design characteristics, the opportunity to actively participate in the meeting by showing voice, and meeting follow-up actions are important building blocks for increasing meeting effectiveness, both in Germany and Spain. Turning to intercultural similarities and differences in meeting practices, our findings suggest that meetings are conducted in a more similar style than expected. For organizations (like the one where we collected the data) in which complains about cross-cultural meetings are common, our study implies that there is a danger of misattribution. Although dissatisfaction with meetings might be attributed to cross-cultural differences, such attributions might not be appropriate, and organizations should rather focus on establishing better meeting practices (e.g., more meeting preparation and more meeting follow-up actions). In particular, trainings efforts targeted at leading more effective meetings could incorporate reflection components that help participants value and understand that cultural preferences for meeting practices are rather secondary in contemporary workplaces. Our findings suggest that meeting practices between cultures are just as similar as meeting practices within a specific culture.

4.2 Limitations and future directions

One limitation concerns our sample, because all participants came from one company. Although variance due to organizational differences is reduced (which is the logic Hofstede [e.g., 2001] uses to explain the advantages of his strategy to collect data only within IBM), it is possible that a cross-culturally shared culture within this company might have overshadowed existing differences between Spain and Germany as discussed above. It should be noted that qualitative remarks from people within the company have, however, stressed how different meetings are with members of the respective other countries. The fact that these differences could not quantitatively be established in our present investigation speaks to the subtlety or nuances of cross-cultural differences and points to the need for additional qualitative research (e.g., Köhler and Gölz 2015).
A second limitation that warrants discussion concerns our measurement of meeting experiences in the two countries. Our choice to rely on attendee perceptions of various meeting aspects aligns well with prior research (e.g., Allen et al. 2014; Leach et al. 2009; Rogelberg et al. 2006). However, self-reports may be subject to several biases (Podsakoff et al. 2003). We cannot fully rule out that cultural effects in response style may have masked possible differences between the two cultures. Two commonly recognized biases in this regard include extreme responding (i.e., the tendency to select the end-points of a scale) and acquiescent responding (i.e., the tendency to agree with questions), which showed to differ as a function of national culture (Johnson et al. 2005). Over and above particular response styles, it might have been the case that the Spanish and German respondents in our study had different cultural representations of the study constructs and corresponding response scales. Looking at pre-meeting small talk, for example, meeting attendees from Spain might have a different understanding of what “a great extent” of small talk actually looks like (e.g., in terms of time spent on small talk) in comparison to meeting attendees from Germany. To circumvent these problems, we suggest that future work may pursue a more behavior-focused approach to meeting interactions and emergent interaction patterns in meetings across different cultural settings (Lehmann-Willenbrock et al. 2014; see also Kauffeld and Lehmann-Willenbrock 2012).
Future research can utilize the new scale developed in this study for capturing meeting follow-up actions. As past research emphasized the importance of action planning for meeting effectiveness (e.g., Kauffeld and Lehmann-Willenbrock 2012), our new and reliable follow-up actions scale can be a helpful tool for both research and practice to improve meeting outcomes and promote the return on investment in organizational meetings (e.g., Lehmann-Willenbrock et al. 2018). This argument is empirically supported by the significant correlation between meeting follow-up actions and meeting effectiveness: If meeting leaders care for follow-up actions, meeting attendees perceive the meeting as more productive.
Future research should also try to explain why pre-meeting small talk is sometimes uncorrelated with meeting effectiveness (as in our study) and sometimes correlated (as in Allen et al. 2014). Maybe the effectiveness of pre-meeting small talk depends on the meeting culture of the respective company. Meetings in the participating organization in which we collected the data for the present investigation were held rather often and were typically long (i.e., more than an hour). This may have reduced the need to engage in small talk in order to construct a group identity. Future research should incorporate such design characteristics and compare the role of pre-meeting small talk across a more diverse set of industries and jobs.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Our product recommendations

Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Organisationspsychologie

Die Zeitschrift beleuchtet organisationspsychologische Fragestellungen an den Schnittstellen von Organisation, Team und Individuum.

Appendix

Appendix

The meeting follow-up actions scale was introduced with “During and after a typical meeting, how often do the following actions occur?” and included the following items, answered on a scale from from 1 (not at all) to 5 (to a great extent):
  • Specific actions were agreed
  • It is clearly defined who is responsible for the execution of the actions
  • Deadlines for the actions are set
  • Meeting minutes are written up
  • The minutes are distributed shortly after the meeting
  • The execution of the agreed actions is monitored
  • There are milestones between the meetings to monitor the execution of the agreed actions.
Literature
go back to reference Aksoy-Burkert, F., & König, C. J. (2015). Meeting training: a suggestion. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 69–89). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Aksoy-Burkert, F., & König, C. J. (2015). Meeting training: a suggestion. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 69–89). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Brodbeck, F. C., & Frese, M. (2008). Societal culture and leadership in Germany. In J. S. Chhokar, F. C. Brodbeck & R. J. House (Eds.), Culture and leadership across the world: The GLOBE book of in depth studies of 25 societies (pp. 147–214). Mahwah: Erlbaum. Brodbeck, F. C., & Frese, M. (2008). Societal culture and leadership in Germany. In J. S. Chhokar, F. C. Brodbeck & R. J. House (Eds.), Culture and leadership across the world: The GLOBE book of in depth studies of 25 societies (pp. 147–214). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Carl, D., Vipin, G., & Javidan, M. (2004). Power distance. In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, M. Javidan, P. W. Dorfman & V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, leadership, and organizations: the GLOBE study of 62 societies (pp. 513–563). Thousand Oaks: SAGE. Carl, D., Vipin, G., & Javidan, M. (2004). Power distance. In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, M. Javidan, P. W. Dorfman & V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, leadership, and organizations: the GLOBE study of 62 societies (pp. 513–563). Thousand Oaks: SAGE.
go back to reference Carlozzi, C. L. (1999). Make your meetings count. Journal of Accountancy, 187(2), 53–55. Carlozzi, C. L. (1999). Make your meetings count. Journal of Accountancy, 187(2), 53–55.
go back to reference van Eerde, W. A., & Buengeler, C. (2015). Meeting preparation and design characteristics. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 177–202). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef van Eerde, W. A., & Buengeler, C. (2015). Meeting preparation and design characteristics. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 177–202). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Evans, P., Pucik, V., & Bjorkman, I. (2011). The global challenge: international human resource management (2nd edn.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Evans, P., Pucik, V., & Bjorkman, I. (2011). The global challenge: international human resource management (2nd edn.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
go back to reference Gelfand, M. J., Bhawuk, D. P. S., Nishii, L. H., & Bechtold, D. J. (2004). Individualism and collectivism. In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, M. Javidan, P. W. Dorfman & V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies (pp. 437–512). Thousand Oaks: SAGE. Gelfand, M. J., Bhawuk, D. P. S., Nishii, L. H., & Bechtold, D. J. (2004). Individualism and collectivism. In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, M. Javidan, P. W. Dorfman & V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies (pp. 437–512). Thousand Oaks: SAGE.
go back to reference Hartmann, M., Röpnack, R., & Baumann, H.-W. (2002). Immer diese Meetings! [Always these meetings!]. Weinheim: Beltz. Hartmann, M., Röpnack, R., & Baumann, H.-W. (2002). Immer diese Meetings! [Always these meetings!]. Weinheim: Beltz.
go back to reference Haynes, M. E. (2006). Meeting skills for leaders: a practical guide for more productive meetings (3rd edn.). Boston: Course Technology. Haynes, M. E. (2006). Meeting skills for leaders: a practical guide for more productive meetings (3rd edn.). Boston: Course Technology.
go back to reference Hickson, D. J., & Hugh, D. S. (1995). Management worldwide: the impact of societal culture on organizations around the globe. Harmondsworth: Penguin. Hickson, D. J., & Hugh, D. S. (1995). Management worldwide: the impact of societal culture on organizations around the globe. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
go back to reference Hirschman, A. O. (1970). Exit, voice, and loyalty: responses to decline in firms, organizations, and states. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Hirschman, A. O. (1970). Exit, voice, and loyalty: responses to decline in firms, organizations, and states. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2nd edn.). Thousand Oaks: SAGE. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2nd edn.). Thousand Oaks: SAGE.
go back to reference Holmes, J. (2000). Doing collegiality and keeping control at work: small talks in government deparments. In J. Coupland (Ed.), Small talk (pp. 32–61). Harlow: Pearson. Holmes, J. (2000). Doing collegiality and keeping control at work: small talks in government deparments. In J. Coupland (Ed.), Small talk (pp. 32–61). Harlow: Pearson.
go back to reference House, R. J., & Javidan, M. (2004). Overview of GLOBE. In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, M. Javidan, P. W. Dorfman & V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies (pp. 9–28). Thousand Oaks: SAGE. House, R. J., & Javidan, M. (2004). Overview of GLOBE. In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, M. Javidan, P. W. Dorfman & V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies (pp. 9–28). Thousand Oaks: SAGE.
go back to reference House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. (Eds.). (2004). Culture, leadership, and organizations: the GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks: SAGE. House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. (Eds.). (2004). Culture, leadership, and organizations: the GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks: SAGE.
go back to reference Kieffer, G. D. (1988). The strategy of meetings. New York: Warner. Kieffer, G. D. (1988). The strategy of meetings. New York: Warner.
go back to reference Köhler, T., & Gölz, M. (2015). Meetings across cultures: cultural differences in meeting expectations and processe. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 119–152). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Köhler, T., & Gölz, M. (2015). Meetings across cultures: cultural differences in meeting expectations and processe. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 119–152). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Lewis, R. (2006). When cultures collide: leading across cultures. London: Brealey. Lewis, R. (2006). When cultures collide: leading across cultures. London: Brealey.
go back to reference O’Connell, J. J., Prieto, J. M., & Gutierrez, C. (2008). Managerial culture and leadership in Spain. In J. S. Chhokar, F. C. Brodbeck & R. J. House (Eds.), Culture and leadership across the world: the GLOBE book of in depth studies of 25 societies (pp. 623–654). Mahwah: Erlbaum. O’Connell, J. J., Prieto, J. M., & Gutierrez, C. (2008). Managerial culture and leadership in Spain. In J. S. Chhokar, F. C. Brodbeck & R. J. House (Eds.), Culture and leadership across the world: the GLOBE book of in depth studies of 25 societies (pp. 623–654). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Odermatt, I., Kleinmann, M., König, C. J., & Giger, K. P. (2013). Erfolgreiche Meetingvorbereitung: Worauf kommt es an? [Successful meeting preparation: What matters?]. Report Psychologie, 38(1), 8–16. Odermatt, I., Kleinmann, M., König, C. J., & Giger, K. P. (2013). Erfolgreiche Meetingvorbereitung: Worauf kommt es an? [Successful meeting preparation: What matters?]. Report Psychologie, 38(1), 8–16.
go back to reference Olien, J. L., Rogelberg, S. G., Lehmann-Willenbrock, N., & Allen, J. A. (2015). Exploring meeting science: key questions and answers. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 12–19). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Olien, J. L., Rogelberg, S. G., Lehmann-Willenbrock, N., & Allen, J. A. (2015). Exploring meeting science: key questions and answers. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 12–19). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Rogelberg, S. G. (2019). The surprising science of workplace meetings. New York: Oxford University Press. Rogelberg, S. G. (2019). The surprising science of workplace meetings. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Tobia, P. M., & Becker, M. C. (1990). Making the most of meeting time. Training & Development Journal, 44(8), 34–38. Tobia, P. M., & Becker, M. C. (1990). Making the most of meeting time. Training & Development Journal, 44(8), 34–38.
go back to reference Tropman, J. E. (2003). Making meetings work: achieving high quality group decisions (2nd edn.). Newbury Park: Sage.CrossRef Tropman, J. E. (2003). Making meetings work: achieving high quality group decisions (2nd edn.). Newbury Park: Sage.CrossRef
go back to reference Yoerger, M. A., Francis, K., & Allen, J. A. (2015). So much more than “chit-chat”: a closer look at premeeting talk. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 153–173). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Yoerger, M. A., Francis, K., & Allen, J. A. (2015). So much more than “chit-chat”: a closer look at premeeting talk. In J. A. Allen, N. Lehmann-Willenbrock & S. G. Rogelberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of meeting science (pp. 153–173). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
A comparison of business meeting practices in Germany and Spain
Authors
Cornelius J. König
Nale Lehmann-Willenbrock
Annika L. Meinecke
Jasmin Winter
Publication date
06-08-2020
Publisher
Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11612-020-00531-z

Other articles of this Issue 3/2020

Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Organisationspsychologie (GIO) 3/2020 Go to the issue