Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Political Behavior 3/2016

19-01-2016 | Original Paper

Are Group Cues Necessary? How Anger Makes Ethnocentrism Among Whites a Stronger Predictor of Racial and Immigration Policy Opinions

Author: Antoine J. Banks

Published in: Political Behavior | Issue 3/2016

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Research shows that group conflict sets ethnocentric thinking into motion. However, when group threat is not salient, can ethnocentrism still influence people’s political decision-making? In this paper, I argue that anger, unrelated to racial and ethnic groups, can activate the attitudes of ethnocentric whites and those that score low in ethnocentrism thereby causing these attitudes to be a stronger predictor of racial and immigration policy opinions. Using an adult national experiment over two waves, I induced several emotions to elicit anger, fear, or relaxation (unrelated to racial or ethnic groups). The experimental findings show that anger increases opposition to racial and immigration policies among whites that score high in ethnocentrism and enhances support for these policies among those that score low in ethnocentrism. Using data from the American National Election Study cumulative file, I find a similar non-racial/ethnic anger effect. The survey findings also demonstrate that non-racial/ethnic fear increases opposition to immigration among whites that don’t have strong out-group attitudes.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
“Tough Times? Blame Minorities!” Southern Poverty Law Center, October 14, 2008.
 
2
In fact, research shows that anger plays an important role in whites’ opinions toward affirmative action (Kinder and Sanders 1996; Kuklinski et al. 1997) and likely underlies people’s opinions toward undocumented immigrants (Lee and Fiske 2006).
 
3
Scholars have devoted little attention to the role of blame attributions in the immigration debate. Much of the research on immigration focuses on the threat immigrants (mostly Hispanic immigrants) pose to American citizens (Brader et al. 2008; Burns and Gimpel 2000; Citrin et al. 1997; Hood and Morris 1997; Hopkins 2010; Huddy and Sears 1995; Rocha et al. 2011; Sniderman et al. 2004). This research oftentimes examines the effects of economic threats (realistic and material) and cultural threats (symbolic and identity) on people’s willingness to support immigration.
 
4
Valentino et al. (2013) find that the U.S. media coverage of immigration focuses more on Hispanics than other immigrant groups like Asians, Africans, or Muslims. Perhaps Americans hold Hispanic immigrants more responsible for the immigration problem than other immigrant groups.
 
5
They should also acquire their attitudes during anger-inducing experiences. An example of an anger-inducing event is the beating of Rodney King by several white police officers. Many people considered this act racially motivated.
 
6
Brader et al. (2008) acknowledge that their anxiety measure “is not well suited to detecting anger as distinct from anxiety because it only contains one anger term. Thus, one could reinterpret the scales as negative and positive affect, rather than as anxiety and enthusiasm” (p. 968).
 
7
Several subjects were dropped from the analysis because they failed to follow instructions. The results are essentially the same if these respondents are included.
 
8
There was a re-contact success rate of 60 %. If some respondents were turned off by the measures of group attitudes in the pre-test, they might opt out of the second wave. This withdrawal of participants could dampen effects. Fortunately, the mortality rate was equivalent across the two waves- no biases occurred between waves 1 and 2 on the feeling thermometer rating of whites (Chi squared 60.2, p = .153), blacks (chi-squared 68.2, p = .569), Hispanics (chi-squared 90.6, p = .175), and Asians (chi-squared 72.7, p = .389).
 
9
The distribution of ethnocentrism (Mean = .13 S.D. = .18) shows that whites on balance are ethnocentric. This result is consistent with Kinder and Kam’s (2009) finding.
 
10
The facial images are presented in the supporting information document. The facial images are of a middle age white woman. Facial expressions are shown to trigger the same emotion in the viewer (Ekman 1993). The pictures are drawn from Ekman’s archive of emotional expressions (Ekman and Friesen 1976).
 
11
For the relaxed condition, there is no image. Paul Ekman’s archival of emotional expression does not include an image of someone who is relaxed. The prompt for the relaxed condition states “Now we would like you to describe in general things that make you feel RELAXED. It is okay if you don't remember all the details, just be specific about what exactly it is that makes you RELAXED and what it feels like to be RELAXED. Please describe the events that make you feel the MOST RELAXED, these experiences could have occurred in the past or will happen in the future. If you can, write your description so that someone reading it might even feel RELAXED.”
 
12
I also find that respondents in the anger conditions are more likely to write about race and immigration than subjects in the fear and control conditions. These results are in the supporting information document.
 
13
The racial policy opinions index is a combination of support for government assistance to blacks and affirmative action (preferential treatment of blacks). This measure, along with all other variables, is described in the supporting information document.
 
14
The effect is significant among 3 % of the sample that score high in ethnocentrism and 5 % of the sample that score low in ethnocentrism.
 
15
I also find that the marginal effect of anger is statistically distinguishable from the marginal effect of fear (one-tailed p < .025). To determine if these differences are statistically significant, I take the difference between the marginal effects of anger and fear and calculate its 95 % CI (across the ethnocentrism scale). The test shows that marginal effect of anger is statistically different from the marginal effect of fear among whites that score at the high end of the ethnocentrism scale (.6 and higher) and those that score low in ethnocentrism (−.1 and lower).
 
16
The immigration policy opinion item is whether respondents support prohibiting children of illegal immigrants from attending U.S. public schools. This measure captures whites’ views toward policies that directly benefit illegal immigrants by providing their children with educational opportunities.
 
17
The anger effect is significant among 33 % of respondents in the analysis. That is, the effect is significant among 1 % of the sample that score high in ethnocentrism and 9 % of the sample that score low in ethnocentrism (i.e. racial liberals) The anger effect is also significant among whites that score at the mid-point of the scale.
 
18
The marginal effect of anger is statistically different from the marginal effect of fear (one-tailed p < .05) among whites that score at the very high end of the ethnocentrism scale and those that score low in ethnocentrism.
 
19
I exclude 2008 and 2012 from my analysis because the emotion measures are directed at the presidential candidates, and one of the candidates is Barack Obama, an African American. Since my argument is that non-racial/ethnic anger should heighten the effect of ethnocentrism on racial and immigration policy opinions, it is important that blacks, Latinos, or Asians are not the targets of the emotion measures.
 
20
The measure of ethnocentrism is based upon the feeling thermometers of three groups (whites, blacks and Hispanics). The 1996 ANES only asked participants about these three groups and not Asians. When I use the ethnocentrism measure based upon whites, blacks, Hispanics, and Asians (by excluding 1996 from the analysis), the results are the same.
 
21
The anger effect is significant among 9 % of the sample that are ethnocentric and 8 % of the sample that score low in ethnocentrism.
 
22
There is no significant difference between the marginal effect of anger and fear across the ethnocentrism scale.
 
23
I collapse the dependent variable because some people perceive “keeping the status quo” category as a pro-immigrant stance. That is, people believe that the “status quo” is allowing a large number of immigrants into the country. Hopkins (2010) applies a similar approach when using the same measure of immigration policy opinion. When the variable is coded as a 3-category variable (1 = decrease, .5 = keep the same, and 0 = increase), the results are essentially the same.
 
24
I calculate the predicted probabilities by manipulating the emotion variables while holding all the other independent variables at their observed values in the data and then averaging over all of the cases (See Hanmer and Kalkan 2013 for a more detailed description of this approach).
 
25
The anger effect is significant among 18 % of that sample that are ethnocentric and 13 % of the sample that score low in ethnocentrism. These results are available in the supporting information document.
 
26
These results are available in the supporting information document.
 
Literature
go back to reference Ai, C., & Norton, E. C. (2003). Interaction terms in logit and probit models. Economic Letters, 80, 123–129.CrossRef Ai, C., & Norton, E. C. (2003). Interaction terms in logit and probit models. Economic Letters, 80, 123–129.CrossRef
go back to reference Averill, James R. (1983). Studies of anger and aggression: Implication for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38(11), 1145–1160.CrossRef Averill, James R. (1983). Studies of anger and aggression: Implication for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38(11), 1145–1160.CrossRef
go back to reference Banks, A. J. (2014a). Anger and racial politics: The emotional foundation of racial attitudes in America. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Banks, A. J. (2014a). Anger and racial politics: The emotional foundation of racial attitudes in America. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Banks, A. J. (2014b). The public’s anger: White racial attitudes and opinions toward health care reform. Political Behavior, 36(3), 493–514.CrossRef Banks, A. J. (2014b). The public’s anger: White racial attitudes and opinions toward health care reform. Political Behavior, 36(3), 493–514.CrossRef
go back to reference Banks, A. J., & Valentino, N. A. (2012). Emotional substrates of white racial attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 56(2), 286–297.CrossRef Banks, A. J., & Valentino, N. A. (2012). Emotional substrates of white racial attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 56(2), 286–297.CrossRef
go back to reference Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 45–62.CrossRef Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 45–62.CrossRef
go back to reference Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129–148.CrossRef Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129–148.CrossRef
go back to reference Bower, G. H., & Forgas, J. P. (2001). Mood and social memory. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), The handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 95–120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Bower, G. H., & Forgas, J. P. (2001). Mood and social memory. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), The handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 95–120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Brader, T., Valentino, N. A., & Suhay, E. (2008). What triggers opposition to immigration? Anxiety, group cues, and immigration threat. American Journal of Political Science, 52(4), 959–978.CrossRef Brader, T., Valentino, N. A., & Suhay, E. (2008). What triggers opposition to immigration? Anxiety, group cues, and immigration threat. American Journal of Political Science, 52(4), 959–978.CrossRef
go back to reference Brambor, T., Clark, W. R., & Golder, M. (2006). Understanding interaction models: Improving empirical analyses. Political Analysis, 14(1), 63–82.CrossRef Brambor, T., Clark, W. R., & Golder, M. (2006). Understanding interaction models: Improving empirical analyses. Political Analysis, 14(1), 63–82.CrossRef
go back to reference Braumoeller, B. F. (2004). Hypothesis testing and multiplicative interaction terms. International Organization, 58, 807–820.CrossRef Braumoeller, B. F. (2004). Hypothesis testing and multiplicative interaction terms. International Organization, 58, 807–820.CrossRef
go back to reference Burns, P. R., & Gimpel, J. G. (2000). Economic insecurity, prejudicial stereotypes, and public opinion on immigration policy. Political Science Quarterly., 115(2), 201–225.CrossRef Burns, P. R., & Gimpel, J. G. (2000). Economic insecurity, prejudicial stereotypes, and public opinion on immigration policy. Political Science Quarterly., 115(2), 201–225.CrossRef
go back to reference Citrin, J., Green, D. P., Muste, C., & Wong, Cara. (1997). Public opinion toward immigration reform: The role of economic motivations. Journal of Politics, 59(3), 858–881.CrossRef Citrin, J., Green, D. P., Muste, C., & Wong, Cara. (1997). Public opinion toward immigration reform: The role of economic motivations. Journal of Politics, 59(3), 858–881.CrossRef
go back to reference Claassen, C. (n.d.). Group entitlement, anger and participation in intergroup violence. British Journal of Political Science, forthcoming. Claassen, C. (n.d.). Group entitlement, anger and participation in intergroup violence. British Journal of Political Science, forthcoming.
go back to reference Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48(4), 384–392.CrossRef Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48(4), 384–392.CrossRef
go back to reference Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.W. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Human interaction laboratory. San Francisco: University of California Medical Center. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.W. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Human interaction laboratory. San Francisco: University of California Medical Center.
go back to reference Gilens, M. (1999). Why Americans hate welfare: Race, media, and the politics of antipoverty policy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Gilens, M. (1999). Why Americans hate welfare: Race, media, and the politics of antipoverty policy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Gurin, P., Miller, A. H., & Gurin, G. (1980). Stratum identification and consciousness. Social Psychological Quarterly, 43(1), 30–47.CrossRef Gurin, P., Miller, A. H., & Gurin, G. (1980). Stratum identification and consciousness. Social Psychological Quarterly, 43(1), 30–47.CrossRef
go back to reference Hanmer, M. J., & Kalkan, K. O. (2013). Behind the curve: Clarifying the best approach to calculating predicted probabilities and marginal effects from limited dependent variable models. American Journal of Political Science, 57(1), 263–277.CrossRef Hanmer, M. J., & Kalkan, K. O. (2013). Behind the curve: Clarifying the best approach to calculating predicted probabilities and marginal effects from limited dependent variable models. American Journal of Political Science, 57(1), 263–277.CrossRef
go back to reference Hood, M. V, I. I. I., & Morris, I. L. (1997). Amigos o enemigos?: Racial context, racial attitudes, and white public opinion towards immigration. Social Science Quarterly., 78(2), 309–323. Hood, M. V, I. I. I., & Morris, I. L. (1997). Amigos o enemigos?: Racial context, racial attitudes, and white public opinion towards immigration. Social Science Quarterly., 78(2), 309–323.
go back to reference Hopkins, D. (2010). Politicized places: Explaining where and when immigrants provoke local opposition. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 40–60.CrossRef Hopkins, D. (2010). Politicized places: Explaining where and when immigrants provoke local opposition. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 40–60.CrossRef
go back to reference Huddy, L., & Sears, D. O. (1995). Opposition to bilingual education: Prejudice or the defense of realistic interests? Social Psychological Quarterly, 58(2), 133–143.CrossRef Huddy, L., & Sears, D. O. (1995). Opposition to bilingual education: Prejudice or the defense of realistic interests? Social Psychological Quarterly, 58(2), 133–143.CrossRef
go back to reference Kam, C. D., & Franzese, R. J. (2007). Modeling and interpreting interactive hypotheses in regression analysis. Michigan: University of Michigan Press. Kam, C. D., & Franzese, R. J. (2007). Modeling and interpreting interactive hypotheses in regression analysis. Michigan: University of Michigan Press.
go back to reference Kam, C. D., & Kinder, D. R. (2012). Ethnocentrism as a short-term force in the 2008 American presidential election. American Journal of Political Science, 56(2), 326–340.CrossRef Kam, C. D., & Kinder, D. R. (2012). Ethnocentrism as a short-term force in the 2008 American presidential election. American Journal of Political Science, 56(2), 326–340.CrossRef
go back to reference Kinder, D. R., & Kam, C. D. (2009). US against them: ethnocentric foundations of American opinion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Kinder, D. R., & Kam, C. D. (2009). US against them: ethnocentric foundations of American opinion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Kinder, Donald R., & Sanders, Lynn M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kinder, Donald R., & Sanders, Lynn M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Kuklinski, J. H., Sniderman, P. M., Knight, K., Piazza, T., Tetlock, P. E., Lawrence, G. R., & Mellers, B. (1997). Racial prejudice and attitudes toward affirmative action. American Journal of Political Science, 41(2), 402–419.CrossRef Kuklinski, J. H., Sniderman, P. M., Knight, K., Piazza, T., Tetlock, P. E., Lawrence, G. R., & Mellers, B. (1997). Racial prejudice and attitudes toward affirmative action. American Journal of Political Science, 41(2), 402–419.CrossRef
go back to reference Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Lee, T. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Not an outgroup, not yet an ingroup: Immigrants in the stereotype content model. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 751–768.CrossRef Lee, T. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Not an outgroup, not yet an ingroup: Immigrants in the stereotype content model. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 751–768.CrossRef
go back to reference Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159.CrossRef Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159.CrossRef
go back to reference Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 115–137.CrossRef Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 115–137.CrossRef
go back to reference Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 602–616.CrossRef Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 602–616.CrossRef
go back to reference MacKuen, M., Wolak, J., Keele, L., & Marcus, G. E. (2010). Civic engagements: Resolute partisanship or reflective deliberation. American Journal of Political Science, 54(2), 440–458.CrossRef MacKuen, M., Wolak, J., Keele, L., & Marcus, G. E. (2010). Civic engagements: Resolute partisanship or reflective deliberation. American Journal of Political Science, 54(2), 440–458.CrossRef
go back to reference Marcus, G. E., Russell Neuman, W., & MacKuen, M. (2000). Affective intelligence and political judgment. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Marcus, G. E., Russell Neuman, W., & MacKuen, M. (2000). Affective intelligence and political judgment. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Mendelberg, T. (2001). The race card: campaign strategy, implicit messages, and the norm of equality. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Mendelberg, T. (2001). The race card: campaign strategy, implicit messages, and the norm of equality. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
go back to reference Mendelberg, T. (2008). Racial priming revived. Perspective on Politics, 6(1), 109–123. Mendelberg, T. (2008). Racial priming revived. Perspective on Politics, 6(1), 109–123.
go back to reference Merolla, J., Ramarkrishnan, S. K., & Haynes, Chris. (2013). ‘Illegal’, ‘Undocumented’, or ‘Unauthorized’: Equivalency frames, issue frames, and public opinion on immigration. Perspectives on Politics, 11(3), 789–807.CrossRef Merolla, J., Ramarkrishnan, S. K., & Haynes, Chris. (2013). ‘Illegal’, ‘Undocumented’, or ‘Unauthorized’: Equivalency frames, issue frames, and public opinion on immigration. Perspectives on Politics, 11(3), 789–807.CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, A. H., Gurin, P., Gurin, G., & Malanchuk, O. (1981). Group consciousness and political participation. American Journal of Political Science, 25(3), 494–511.CrossRef Miller, A. H., Gurin, P., Gurin, G., & Malanchuk, O. (1981). Group consciousness and political participation. American Journal of Political Science, 25(3), 494–511.CrossRef
go back to reference Nelson, T. E., & Kinder, D. R. (1996). Issue frames and group-centrism in American public opinion. Journal of Politics, 58(4), 1055–1078.CrossRef Nelson, T. E., & Kinder, D. R. (1996). Issue frames and group-centrism in American public opinion. Journal of Politics, 58(4), 1055–1078.CrossRef
go back to reference Rocha, R. R., Longoria, T., Wrinkle, R. D., Knoll, B. R., Polinard, J. L., & Wenzel, J. P. (2011). Ethnic context and immigration policy preferences among latinos and anglos. Social Science Quarterly, 92, 1–19.CrossRef Rocha, R. R., Longoria, T., Wrinkle, R. D., Knoll, B. R., Polinard, J. L., & Wenzel, J. P. (2011). Ethnic context and immigration policy preferences among latinos and anglos. Social Science Quarterly, 92, 1–19.CrossRef
go back to reference Schildkraut, D. J. (2011). Americanism in the twenty-first century: Public opinion in the age of immigration. Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press. Schildkraut, D. J. (2011). Americanism in the twenty-first century: Public opinion in the age of immigration. Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Sears, D. O., & Kinder, D. (1971). Racial tensions and voting in Los Angeles. In W. Hirsch (Ed.), Los Angeles: Viability and prospects for metropolitan leadership (pp. 51–88). New York: Praeger. Sears, D. O., & Kinder, D. (1971). Racial tensions and voting in Los Angeles. In W. Hirsch (Ed.), Los Angeles: Viability and prospects for metropolitan leadership (pp. 51–88). New York: Praeger.
go back to reference Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory or social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory or social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Small, D. A., & Lerner, J. S. (2008). Emotional policy: Personal sadness and anger shape judgments about a welfare case. Political Psychology, 29(2), 149–168.CrossRef Small, D. A., & Lerner, J. S. (2008). Emotional policy: Personal sadness and anger shape judgments about a welfare case. Political Psychology, 29(2), 149–168.CrossRef
go back to reference Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838.CrossRef Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838.CrossRef
go back to reference Smith, C. A., Haynes, K. N., Lazarus, R. S., & Pope, L. K. (1993). In search of the Hot’ cognitions: Attributions, appraisals, and their relation to emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(5), 916–929.CrossRef Smith, C. A., Haynes, K. N., Lazarus, R. S., & Pope, L. K. (1993). In search of the Hot’ cognitions: Attributions, appraisals, and their relation to emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(5), 916–929.CrossRef
go back to reference Sniderman, P. M., Hagendoorn, L., & Prior, M. (2004). Predisposing factors and situational triggers: Exclusionary reactions to immigrant minorities. American Political Science Review, 98(1), 35–49.CrossRef Sniderman, P. M., Hagendoorn, L., & Prior, M. (2004). Predisposing factors and situational triggers: Exclusionary reactions to immigrant minorities. American Political Science Review, 98(1), 35–49.CrossRef
go back to reference Suthammanont, C., Peterson, D. A. M., Owens, C. T., & Leighley, J. E. (2010). Taking threat seriously: Prejudice, principle, and attitudes toward racial policies. Political Behavior, 32(2), 231–253.CrossRef Suthammanont, C., Peterson, D. A. M., Owens, C. T., & Leighley, J. E. (2010). Taking threat seriously: Prejudice, principle, and attitudes toward racial policies. Political Behavior, 32(2), 231–253.CrossRef
go back to reference Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Valentino, N. A., Banks, A. J., Hutchings, V. L., & Davis, A. K. (2009). Selective exposure in the internet age: The interaction between anxiety and information utility. Political Psychology, 30(4), 591–613.CrossRef Valentino, N. A., Banks, A. J., Hutchings, V. L., & Davis, A. K. (2009). Selective exposure in the internet age: The interaction between anxiety and information utility. Political Psychology, 30(4), 591–613.CrossRef
go back to reference Valentino, N. A., Brader, T., Groenendyk, E. W., Gregorowicz, K., & Hutchings, V. L. (2011). Election night’s alright for fighting: The role of emotion in political participation. Journal of Politics, 73(1), 156–170.CrossRef Valentino, N. A., Brader, T., Groenendyk, E. W., Gregorowicz, K., & Hutchings, V. L. (2011). Election night’s alright for fighting: The role of emotion in political participation. Journal of Politics, 73(1), 156–170.CrossRef
go back to reference Valentino, N. A., Brader, T., & Jardina, A. E. (2013). Immigration opposition among U.S. whites: General ethnocentrism or media priming of attitudes about latinos? Political Psychology, 34(2), 149–166.CrossRef Valentino, N. A., Brader, T., & Jardina, A. E. (2013). Immigration opposition among U.S. whites: General ethnocentrism or media priming of attitudes about latinos? Political Psychology, 34(2), 149–166.CrossRef
go back to reference Valentino, N. A., Hutchings, V. L., & White, I. K. (2002). Cues that matter: How political ads prime racial attitudes during campaigns. American Political Science Review, 96(1), 75–90.CrossRef Valentino, N. A., Hutchings, V. L., & White, I. K. (2002). Cues that matter: How political ads prime racial attitudes during campaigns. American Political Science Review, 96(1), 75–90.CrossRef
go back to reference White, I. K. (2007). When race matters and when it doesn’t: Racial group differences in response to racial cues. American Political Science Review, 101(2), 339–354.CrossRef White, I. K. (2007). When race matters and when it doesn’t: Racial group differences in response to racial cues. American Political Science Review, 101(2), 339–354.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Are Group Cues Necessary? How Anger Makes Ethnocentrism Among Whites a Stronger Predictor of Racial and Immigration Policy Opinions
Author
Antoine J. Banks
Publication date
19-01-2016
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Political Behavior / Issue 3/2016
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-016-9330-3

Other articles of this Issue 3/2016

Political Behavior 3/2016 Go to the issue