Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Political Behavior 3/2016

28-01-2016 | Original Paper

Rolling off the Tongue into the Top-of-the-Head: Explaining Language Effects on Public Opinion

Author: Efrén O. Pérez

Published in: Political Behavior | Issue 3/2016

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Growing evidence shows that mass opinion varies by interview language, yet modest theory exists to explain this result. I propose a framework where language impacts survey response by making some political concepts more mentally accessible. I claim that concepts vary by how associated they are with certain languages, which means people are more likely to acquire a construct when it is tied to the tongue one speaks. Hence, recalling concepts from memory should be easier when the language a construct is linked to matches the tongue one interviews in, thereby intensifying people’s opinions. I test my theory by manipulating the interview language in two U.S. surveys of English/Spanish bilingual Latino adults. I generally find that language influences the accessibility of concepts. For example, subjects report higher opinion levels for concepts that are tied more to their interview language, such as American identity among English interviewees. Subjects who interview in English are also less likely to refuse completing items measuring knowledge about U.S. politics, and more likely to answer them quickly. Items reflecting constructs that are highly labile (e.g. anti-Obama affect) or very crystallized (e.g., partisanship) do not display these patterns. I then rule out that language effects are mostly mediated by a heightened sense of anxiety, anger, pride or efficacy that emerges when bilingual subjects interview in one of their languages.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
In turn, “hubristic” pride results from attributions to internal, stable, uncontrollable causes (e.g., I won because I’m always great). I focus on “authentic” pride because respondents do control the concepts they retrieve and use to form opinions.
 
2
Stereotype threat might also mediate the impact of language via depletion of people’ executive functioning (Steele et al. 2002), a possibility that future research should further explore.
 
3
37 % of subjects report usually completing GfK surveys in Spanish, while 63 % usually complete them in English.
 
4
31 % of subjects report usually completing GfK surveys in Spanish, while 69 % usually complete them in English.
 
5
Subjects completed these items when they first enrolled to GfK’s respondent panel. As might be expected (Abrajano and Alvarez 2010; Alba and Nee 2003), about half of bilinguals in both samples are foreign-born and Mexican origin, with the median education in both samples being “some college.” See table A in the supporting information (SI) for more characteristics and any nuances between them across samples.
 
6
Table B (SI) reports a non-linear regression predicting assignment to an experimental condition as a function of several covariates, suggesting my treatment was effectively randomized. Table C (SI) shows these covariates are balanced across my experimental cells. These results imply that my bilingual respondents are alike in all respects, save for the interview language treatment.
 
7
A full comparison of both treatments can be found in table D (SI).
 
8
The response times I analyze are indexed in seconds, rather than milliseconds, because GfK can only collect response times in seconds at this time. This will make it harder to uncover any accessibility effects by language.
 
9
For Study 1: Latino identity (α = .54); National origin identity (α = .86); American identity (α = .81). For Study 2: Latino identity (α = .54); National origin identity (α = .87); American identity (α = .74).
 
10
In both studies, most of the concepts that I measure are theorized to be linked to English. I did this by design, since U.S. polls typically ask about American constructs, which are tied to English. Still, as I describe in the text, I measure one Spanish-linked construct often asked about in U.S. academic surveys (i.e., national origin identity), and designed two items tapping knowledge about Latin American history in order to better test my hypotheses.
 
11
Since my knowledge items are dichotomous, I report average tetrachoric correlations (rho) rather than alphas (α). For Study 1: Traditional U.S. knowledge (average rho = .58); Latino knowledge (average rho = .54).
 
12
For Study 2: Traditional U.S. knowledge (average rho = .37); Latin American knowledge (average rho = .53).
 
13
One might expect these items to precede my opinion measures. But asking people about their efficacy and emotions right after my language manipulation risks an artificial increase in the correlation between interview language and emotions, which works against finding mediation effects (e.g., the more variance in anxiety that interview language explains, the less variance in anxiety to explain opinions). Also, gauging opinions should have a trivial effect on measuring efficacy and emotion, since any sense of these has already occurred by the time opinions are reported.
 
14
The point of these efforts, then, is not to create translations that are exact in length, word for word. Such a strategy is likely to yield translations of similar length, but with different and often grammatically incorrect meanings (Jacobson et al. 1960). Instead, the aim is to develop translated items that mean the same thing to different people (Pérez 2009).
 
15
Indeed, scholars regularly find that Latino identity is more widespread among individuals who have deeper roots in the U.S. and are more conversant in English (Portes and Rumbaut 2006; Abrajano and Alvarez 2010), suggesting a relatively stronger tie between it and the English language.
 
16
Although I use a regression approach to estimate my treatment effects, t-tests yield comparable results.
 
17
I use one-tailed tests given the explicitly directional nature of my main hypothesis (H1), as well as accumulated lab work showing accessibility effects in specific directions on cognitive outcomes, such as answering questions.
 
18
Reflecting prior research (Ross et al. 2002, p. 1041), these findings are largely inconsistent with a social desirability explanation. For example, if social desirability was responsible for these opinion gaps, we should have observed English interviewees express weaker (not stronger) Latino identity than Spanish interviewees.
 
19
I used a 30 s threshold for answering knowledge items in Study 1 in order to keep this analysis consistent with an identical one using knowledge items from Study 2, where subjects were explicitly given 30 s to answer each knowledge item. As my analysis will show, knowledge item RTs in Study 1 are largely unaffected by slow responders.
 
20
I study refusal rates rather than “don’t know” (DK) rates because people offer DKs for many reasons, including (a) not fully knowing an answer (b) incompletely knowing an answer (Gibson and Caldeira 2009); (c) refraining from guessing an answer (Mondak 2001); and (d) being unmotivated to try answering correctly (Prior and Lupia 2008). Refusal rates are less affected by these problems, so I use them to test my accessibility hypothesis.
 
21
Indeed, in Study 1, the two items measuring American identity were positively worded. Not surprisingly in hindsight, the estimated intercept for American identity in Study 1 was about .75, which suggests a high degree of this attachment among Spanish interviewees, making it harder for English interviewees to report an even higher level of this attachment.
 
22
This estimate is from an ordered probit model. The three knowledge scale items were: “Whose responsibility is it to determine if a law is constitutional or not?”; “How much of a majority is required for the US Senate and House to override a presidential veto?”; and “How long is the term of office for a United States Senator?”.
 
23
Specifically, I estimated an ordered probit model with the following coefficients: Latin American knowledge = −.144 English + .110 > 30 s + .613 English> 30 s + .464 Education + .015 Age.
 
24
Indeed, in both Study 1 and 2 the intercept for this construct is above .80 on a 0–1 range.
 
25
Although limited in number, the two reliable RT differences by interview language in Table 2 weakly impact people’s survey responses, further hinting at the role of accessibility in producing these results (see Table E, SI).
 
26
Since my RR variables have much more limited variation than my RT scores, I do not further examine whether RR differences subsequently impact knowledge reports.
 
27
One might reasonably wonder whether an emotion like pride is conceptually and empirically synonymous with group consciousness, linked fate, or solidarity, since all of these reflect positive feelings to a degree (Dawson 1994; McClain et al. 2009; Sanchez 2006; Leach et al. 2008). True, group attachments can induce pride and other emotions in group members (Mackie et al. 2008). But my conceptualization and measure of pride focus on this feeling among individuals and what each person accomplishes during an interview. Still, I would expect, but cannot test here, that language impacts people’s sense of group consciousness, linked fate, and/or solidarity insofar as these flow from a specific group identity.
 
28
A better strategy here is to design a sufficiently powered experiment that blocks subjects’ assigned interview language on their PL or IG, thus retaining the experiment’s causal leverage. I leave this possibility open for future research.
 
Literature
go back to reference Abrajano, M. A., & Alvarez, R. M. (2010). New faces, new voices: The Hispanic electorate in America. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRef Abrajano, M. A., & Alvarez, R. M. (2010). New faces, new voices: The Hispanic electorate in America. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Abrajano, M., & Panagopoulos, C. (2011). Does language matter? The impact of Spanish versus English-language GOTV efforts on Latino turnout. American Politics Research, 39(4), 643–663.CrossRef Abrajano, M., & Panagopoulos, C. (2011). Does language matter? The impact of Spanish versus English-language GOTV efforts on Latino turnout. American Politics Research, 39(4), 643–663.CrossRef
go back to reference Alba, R., & Nee, V. (2003). Remaking the American mainstream: Assimilation and contemporary immigration. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRef Alba, R., & Nee, V. (2003). Remaking the American mainstream: Assimilation and contemporary immigration. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Bailey, S. C., Hasnain-Wynia, R., Chen, A. H., Sarkar, U., Schoua-Glusberg, A., Lindquist, L. A., et al. (2012). Developing multilingual prescription instructions for patients with limited-English proficiency. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved, 23, 81–87.CrossRef Bailey, S. C., Hasnain-Wynia, R., Chen, A. H., Sarkar, U., Schoua-Glusberg, A., Lindquist, L. A., et al. (2012). Developing multilingual prescription instructions for patients with limited-English proficiency. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved, 23, 81–87.CrossRef
go back to reference Baker, C. (2001). Foundations of bilingual education and bilingualism. New York: Multilingual Matters. Baker, C. (2001). Foundations of bilingual education and bilingualism. New York: Multilingual Matters.
go back to reference Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.CrossRef Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.CrossRef
go back to reference Binder, M., Kogan, V., Kousser, T., & Panagopoulos, C. (2014). Mobilizing Latino voters: The impact of language and co-ethnic policy leadership. American Politics Research, 42(4), 677–699.CrossRef Binder, M., Kogan, V., Kousser, T., & Panagopoulos, C. (2014). Mobilizing Latino voters: The impact of language and co-ethnic policy leadership. American Politics Research, 42(4), 677–699.CrossRef
go back to reference Brader, T. (2005). Striking a responsive chord: How political ads motivate and persuade voters by appealing to emotions. American Journal of Political Science, 49, 388–405.CrossRef Brader, T. (2005). Striking a responsive chord: How political ads motivate and persuade voters by appealing to emotions. American Journal of Political Science, 49, 388–405.CrossRef
go back to reference Brader, T., & Marcus, G. E. (2013). Emotion and political psychology. In L. Huddy, D. O. Sears, & J. S. Levy (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Brader, T., & Marcus, G. E. (2013). Emotion and political psychology. In L. Huddy, D. O. Sears, & J. S. Levy (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Bullock, J. G., & Ha, S. E. (2011). Mediation analysis is harder than it looks. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science. New York: Cambridge University Press. Bullock, J. G., & Ha, S. E. (2011). Mediation analysis is harder than it looks. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Cho, G. (2000). The role of the heritage language in social interactions and relationships: Reflections from a language minority group. Bilingual Research Journal, 24, 369–384.CrossRef Cho, G. (2000). The role of the heritage language in social interactions and relationships: Reflections from a language minority group. Bilingual Research Journal, 24, 369–384.CrossRef
go back to reference Citrin, J., Wong, C., & Duff, B. (2001). The meaning of American national identity: Pattern of ethnic conflict and consensus. In R. D. Ashmore, L. Jussim, & D. Wilder (Eds.), Social identity, intergroup conflict, and conflict resolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Citrin, J., Wong, C., & Duff, B. (2001). The meaning of American national identity: Pattern of ethnic conflict and consensus. In R. D. Ashmore, L. Jussim, & D. Wilder (Eds.), Social identity, intergroup conflict, and conflict resolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159.CrossRef Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159.CrossRef
go back to reference Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.CrossRef Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.CrossRef
go back to reference Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608–630.CrossRef Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608–630.CrossRef
go back to reference Davila, A. (2001). Latinos, inc.: the marketing and making of a people. Berkeley: University of California Press. Davila, A. (2001). Latinos, inc.: the marketing and making of a people. Berkeley: University of California Press.
go back to reference Dawson, M. C. (1994). Behind the mule: Race and class in African-American politics. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Dawson, M. C. (1994). Behind the mule: Race and class in African-American politics. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
go back to reference Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans need to know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press. Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans need to know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (2002). Self and social identity. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 161–186.CrossRef Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (2002). Self and social identity. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 161–186.CrossRef
go back to reference Ellis, C., & Stimson, J. A. (2009). Symbolic ideology in the American electorate. Electoral Studies, 28, 388–402.CrossRef Ellis, C., & Stimson, J. A. (2009). Symbolic ideology in the American electorate. Electoral Studies, 28, 388–402.CrossRef
go back to reference Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1013–1027.CrossRef Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1013–1027.CrossRef
go back to reference Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229–238.CrossRef Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Fraga, L. R., Garcia, J. A., Hero, R. E., Jones-Correa, M., Martinez-Ebers, V., & Segura, G. M. (2011). Latino lives: making it home. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Fraga, L. R., Garcia, J. A., Hero, R. E., Jones-Correa, M., Martinez-Ebers, V., & Segura, G. M. (2011). Latino lives: making it home. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
go back to reference Garcia, J. A. (2003). Latino politics in America: Community, culture, and interests. Lanham: Rowman and Littlefield. Garcia, J. A. (2003). Latino politics in America: Community, culture, and interests. Lanham: Rowman and Littlefield.
go back to reference Garcia, J. A. (2009). Language of interview: exploring patterns and consequences of changing language during interview. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the western political science association. Vancouver, B.C. Garcia, J. A. (2009). Language of interview: exploring patterns and consequences of changing language during interview. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the western political science association. Vancouver, B.C.
go back to reference Garcia Bedolla, L. (2005). Fluid borders: Latino power, identity, and politics in Los Angeles. Berkeley: University of California Press. Garcia Bedolla, L. (2005). Fluid borders: Latino power, identity, and politics in Los Angeles. Berkeley: University of California Press.
go back to reference Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2009). Knowing the Supreme Court? A reconsideration of public ignorance of the High Court. The Journal of Politics, 71(2), 429–441.CrossRef Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2009). Knowing the Supreme Court? A reconsideration of public ignorance of the High Court. The Journal of Politics, 71(2), 429–441.CrossRef
go back to reference Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: on land and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66(3), 325–331.CrossRef Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: on land and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66(3), 325–331.CrossRef
go back to reference Grant, H. M., Bredahl, L. C., Clay, J., Ferrie, J., Groves, J. E., McDorman, T. A., & Dark, V. J. (1998). Context-dependent memory for meaningful material: Information for students. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 617–623.CrossRef Grant, H. M., Bredahl, L. C., Clay, J., Ferrie, J., Groves, J. E., McDorman, T. A., & Dark, V. J. (1998). Context-dependent memory for meaningful material: Information for students. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 617–623.CrossRef
go back to reference Green, D., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partsian hearts and minds: political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven: Yale University Press. Green, D., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partsian hearts and minds: political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.CrossRef Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.CrossRef
go back to reference Harkness, J. A., Pennell, B. E., & Schoua-Glusberg, A. (2004). Survey questionnaire translation and assessment. In S. Presser, J. M. Rothgeb, M. P. Couper, J. T. Lessler, E. Martin, J. Martin, & E. Singer (Eds.), Methods for testing and evaluating survey questionnaires. Hoboken: Wiley. Harkness, J. A., Pennell, B. E., & Schoua-Glusberg, A. (2004). Survey questionnaire translation and assessment. In S. Presser, J. M. Rothgeb, M. P. Couper, J. T. Lessler, E. Martin, J. Martin, & E. Singer (Eds.), Methods for testing and evaluating survey questionnaires. Hoboken: Wiley.
go back to reference Hattam, V. (2007). In the shadow of race: Jews, Latinos, and immigrant politics in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Hattam, V. (2007). In the shadow of race: Jews, Latinos, and immigrant politics in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Heider, E. R. (1972). Universals in color naming and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93(1), 10–20.CrossRef Heider, E. R. (1972). Universals in color naming and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93(1), 10–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Ho, D. E., Imai, K., King, G., & Stuart, E. A. (2007). Matching as nonparametric preprocessing for reducing model dependence in parametric causal inference. Political Analysis, 15, 199–236.CrossRef Ho, D. E., Imai, K., King, G., & Stuart, E. A. (2007). Matching as nonparametric preprocessing for reducing model dependence in parametric causal inference. Political Analysis, 15, 199–236.CrossRef
go back to reference Hong, Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55(7), 709–720.CrossRef Hong, Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55(7), 709–720.CrossRef
go back to reference Huddy, L., Feldman, S., & Cassese, E. (2007). On the distinct political effects of anxiety and anger. In W. N. Neuman, G. E. Marcus, A. N. Crigler, & M. MacKuen (Eds.), The affect effect: Dynamics of emotion in political thinking and behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Huddy, L., Feldman, S., & Cassese, E. (2007). On the distinct political effects of anxiety and anger. In W. N. Neuman, G. E. Marcus, A. N. Crigler, & M. MacKuen (Eds.), The affect effect: Dynamics of emotion in political thinking and behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Imai, K., Keele, L., Tingley, D., & Yamamoto, T. (2011). Unpacking the black box of causality: Learning about causal mechanisms from experimental and observational studies. American Political Science Review, 105(4), 765–789.CrossRef Imai, K., Keele, L., Tingley, D., & Yamamoto, T. (2011). Unpacking the black box of causality: Learning about causal mechanisms from experimental and observational studies. American Political Science Review, 105(4), 765–789.CrossRef
go back to reference Jacobson, E., Hideya, K., & Gullahorn, J. E. (1960). Cross-cultural contributions to attitude research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24(2), 205–223.CrossRef Jacobson, E., Hideya, K., & Gullahorn, J. E. (1960). Cross-cultural contributions to attitude research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24(2), 205–223.CrossRef
go back to reference Jaspal, R. (2009). Language and social identity: a psychosocial approach. PsychTalk, 64, 17–20. Jaspal, R. (2009). Language and social identity: a psychosocial approach. PsychTalk, 64, 17–20.
go back to reference King, D. (2000). Making Americans: immigration, race, and the origins of the diverse democracy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. King, D. (2000). Making Americans: immigration, race, and the origins of the diverse democracy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Krosnick, J. A., & Smith, W. R. (1994). Attitude strength. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior. San Diego: Academic Press. Krosnick, J. A., & Smith, W. R. (1994). Attitude strength. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior. San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Leach, C. W., van Zomeren, M., Zebel, S., Vliek, M. L. W., Pennekamp, S. F., Doosje, B., et al. (2008). Group-level self-definition and self-investment: a hierarchical (multicomponent) model of in-group identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 144–165.CrossRef Leach, C. W., van Zomeren, M., Zebel, S., Vliek, M. L. W., Pennekamp, S. F., Doosje, B., et al. (2008). Group-level self-definition and self-investment: a hierarchical (multicomponent) model of in-group identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 144–165.CrossRef
go back to reference Lee, T., & Pérez, E. O. (2014). The persistent connection between language-of-interview and Latino political opinion. Political Behavior, 36(2), 401–425.CrossRef Lee, T., & Pérez, E. O. (2014). The persistent connection between language-of-interview and Latino political opinion. Political Behavior, 36(2), 401–425.CrossRef
go back to reference Lien, P., Conway, M. M., & Wong, J. (2004). The politics of Asian Americans. New York: Routledge. Lien, P., Conway, M. M., & Wong, J. (2004). The politics of Asian Americans. New York: Routledge.
go back to reference Lodge, M., & Taber, C. (2013). The rationalizing voter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Lodge, M., & Taber, C. (2013). The rationalizing voter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Mackie, D. M., Smith, E. R., & Ray, D. G. (2008). Intergroup emotions and intergroup relations. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(5), 1866–1880.CrossRef Mackie, D. M., Smith, E. R., & Ray, D. G. (2008). Intergroup emotions and intergroup relations. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(5), 1866–1880.CrossRef
go back to reference Marcus, G. E., Neuman, W. R., & MacKuen, M. (2000). Affective intelligence and political judgment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Marcus, G. E., Neuman, W. R., & MacKuen, M. (2000). Affective intelligence and political judgment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Marian, V., & Fausey, C. M. (2006). Language-dependent memory in bilingual learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1025–1047.CrossRef Marian, V., & Fausey, C. M. (2006). Language-dependent memory in bilingual learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1025–1047.CrossRef
go back to reference Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2004). Self-construal and emotion in bicultural bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 190–201.CrossRef Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2004). Self-construal and emotion in bicultural bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 190–201.CrossRef
go back to reference Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). Language context guides memory content. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 925–933.CrossRef Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). Language context guides memory content. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 925–933.CrossRef
go back to reference Marian, V., & Neisser, U. (2000). Language-dependent recall of autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 361–368.CrossRef Marian, V., & Neisser, U. (2000). Language-dependent recall of autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 361–368.CrossRef
go back to reference Martínez, G., Marín, B., & Schoua-Glusberg, A. (2006). Translating from English to Spanish: The 2002 National Survey of Family Growth. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 28, 531–545.CrossRef Martínez, G., Marín, B., & Schoua-Glusberg, A. (2006). Translating from English to Spanish: The 2002 National Survey of Family Growth. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 28, 531–545.CrossRef
go back to reference McClain, P. D., Carew, J. D., Walton, E, Jr, & Watts, C. S. (2009). Group membership, group identity, and group consciousness: Measures of racial identity in American politics? Annual Review of Political Science, 12, 471–485.CrossRef McClain, P. D., Carew, J. D., Walton, E, Jr, & Watts, C. S. (2009). Group membership, group identity, and group consciousness: Measures of racial identity in American politics? Annual Review of Political Science, 12, 471–485.CrossRef
go back to reference McDermott, R. (2011). Internal and external validity. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science. New York: Cambridge University Press. McDermott, R. (2011). Internal and external validity. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Milroy, L. (1982). Language and group identity. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 3, 207–216.CrossRef Milroy, L. (1982). Language and group identity. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 3, 207–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Mondak, J. J. (2001). Developing valid knowledge scales. American Journal of Political Science, 45(1), 224–238.CrossRef Mondak, J. J. (2001). Developing valid knowledge scales. American Journal of Political Science, 45(1), 224–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Mora, G. C. (2014). Making Hispanics: How activists, bureaucrats, and media constructed a new American. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Mora, G. C. (2014). Making Hispanics: How activists, bureaucrats, and media constructed a new American. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Mutz, D. C. (2011). Population-based survey experiments. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRef Mutz, D. C. (2011). Population-based survey experiments. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Niemi, R. G., Craig, S. C., & Mattei, F. (1991). Measuring internal political efficacy in the 1988 National Election Study. American Political Science Review, 85(4), 1407–1413.CrossRef Niemi, R. G., Craig, S. C., & Mattei, F. (1991). Measuring internal political efficacy in the 1988 National Election Study. American Political Science Review, 85(4), 1407–1413.CrossRef
go back to reference Pérez, E. O. (2009). Lost in translation? Item validity in bilingual political surveys. The Journal of Politics, 71(4), 1530–1548.CrossRef Pérez, E. O. (2009). Lost in translation? Item validity in bilingual political surveys. The Journal of Politics, 71(4), 1530–1548.CrossRef
go back to reference Pérez, E. O. (2011). The origins and implications of language effects in multilingual surveys: A MIMIC approach with application to Latino political attitudes. Political Analysis, 19(4), 434–454.CrossRef Pérez, E. O. (2011). The origins and implications of language effects in multilingual surveys: A MIMIC approach with application to Latino political attitudes. Political Analysis, 19(4), 434–454.CrossRef
go back to reference Pérez, E. O. (2015a). Speaking in tongues: explaining and anticipating language effects on survey response. Grant proposal submitted to the National Science Foundation. Pérez, E. O. (2015a). Speaking in tongues: explaining and anticipating language effects on survey response. Grant proposal submitted to the National Science Foundation.
go back to reference Pérez, E. O. (2015b). The language-opinion connection. In R. M. Alvarez & L. Atkeson (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of polling and polling methods. New York: Oxford University Press. Pérez, E. O. (2015b). The language-opinion connection. In R. M. Alvarez & L. Atkeson (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of polling and polling methods. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2006). Immigrant America: A portrait. Berkeley: University of California Press. Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2006). Immigrant America: A portrait. Berkeley: University of California Press.
go back to reference Prior, M., & Lupia, A. (2008). Money, time, and political knowledge: Distinguishing quick recall and political learning skills. American Journal of Political Science, 52, 169–183.CrossRef Prior, M., & Lupia, A. (2008). Money, time, and political knowledge: Distinguishing quick recall and political learning skills. American Journal of Political Science, 52, 169–183.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(3), 192–233.CrossRef Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(3), 192–233.CrossRef
go back to reference Ross, M., Xun, W. Q. E., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). Language and the bicultural self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(8), 1040–1050.CrossRef Ross, M., Xun, W. Q. E., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). Language and the bicultural self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(8), 1040–1050.CrossRef
go back to reference Sanchez, G. R. (2006). The role of group consciousness in Latino public opinion. Political Research Quarterly, 59(3), 435–446.CrossRef Sanchez, G. R. (2006). The role of group consciousness in Latino public opinion. Political Research Quarterly, 59(3), 435–446.CrossRef
go back to reference Schildkraut, D. J. (2005). The rise and fall of political engagement among Latinos: The role of identity and perceptions of discrimination. Political Behavior, 27(3), 285–312.CrossRef Schildkraut, D. J. (2005). The rise and fall of political engagement among Latinos: The role of identity and perceptions of discrimination. Political Behavior, 27(3), 285–312.CrossRef
go back to reference Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(3), 515–530.CrossRef Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(3), 515–530.CrossRef
go back to reference Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company.
go back to reference Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: the psychology of stereotype and identity threat. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 379–440.CrossRef Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: the psychology of stereotype and identity threat. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 379–440.CrossRef
go back to reference Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). The nature of pride. In J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research. New York: Guilford. Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). The nature of pride. In J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research. New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Trafimow, D., Silverman, E. S., Fan, R. M., & Fun Law, J. S. (1997). The effects of language and priming on the relative accessibility of the private self and the collective self. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 28(1), 107–123.CrossRef Trafimow, D., Silverman, E. S., Fan, R. M., & Fun Law, J. S. (1997). The effects of language and priming on the relative accessibility of the private self and the collective self. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 28(1), 107–123.CrossRef
go back to reference Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373.CrossRef Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373.CrossRef
go back to reference Wallace, S. J. (2014). Examining Latino support for descriptive representation: the role of identity and discrimination. Social Science Quarterly, 95(2), 311–327.CrossRef Wallace, S. J. (2014). Examining Latino support for descriptive representation: the role of identity and discrimination. Social Science Quarterly, 95(2), 311–327.CrossRef
go back to reference Welch, S., Comer, J., & Steinman, M. (1973). Interviewing in a Mexican-American community: An investigation of some potential sources of response bias. Public Opinion Quarterly, 37(1), 115–126.CrossRef Welch, S., Comer, J., & Steinman, M. (1973). Interviewing in a Mexican-American community: An investigation of some potential sources of response bias. Public Opinion Quarterly, 37(1), 115–126.CrossRef
go back to reference Wong, J., Ramakrishnan, S. K., Lee, T., & Junn, J. (2011). Asian American political participation: Emerging constituents and their political identities. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Wong, J., Ramakrishnan, S. K., Lee, T., & Junn, J. (2011). Asian American political participation: Emerging constituents and their political identities. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
go back to reference Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Zaller, J., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response: Answering questions versus revealing preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 36(3), 579–616.CrossRef Zaller, J., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response: Answering questions versus revealing preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 36(3), 579–616.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Rolling off the Tongue into the Top-of-the-Head: Explaining Language Effects on Public Opinion
Author
Efrén O. Pérez
Publication date
28-01-2016
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Political Behavior / Issue 3/2016
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-015-9329-1

Other articles of this Issue 3/2016

Political Behavior 3/2016 Go to the issue