Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Journal of Business and Psychology 1/2015

01-03-2015

The Effects of Unconsciously Derived Affect on Task Satisfaction and Performance

Authors: Xiaoxiao Hu, Seth Kaplan

Published in: Journal of Business and Psychology | Issue 1/2015

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Purpose

Theoretical and empirical evidence from basic psychological research suggests that much of affective processing occurs outside of conscious awareness and that this unconscious processing can have profound impacts on attitudes and behaviors. However, research in the management domain examining unconscious affective processing is almost completely absent. To address this void, we examined the role of unconsciously derived affect in work-related judgments and behaviors.

Design/Methodology/Approach

In two studies, participants were primed with subliminal affective cues while performing work-related tasks.

Findings

Across the two studies, the results showed that positively valenced stimuli impacted reports of task satisfaction and performance on some, but not all, types of tasks. The effects of this unconsciously derived affect generally remained even when participants were explicitly told that they would be exposed to these primes.

Implications

This study demonstrates the potential importance of unconsciously derived affect for work-related outcomes. The results provide initial evidence that affect that emerges through sources outside of conscious awareness can impact individuals’ feelings about, and performance on, work-related tasks. This finding may help leaders and employees to make the work environment more conducive to serving employee and organizational objectives.

Originality/Value

This is an initial examination of the effects of unconsciously derived affect on reactions to, and performance on, work-relevant tasks.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Footnotes
1
In well-being research, negative affect scores are routinely subtracted from positive affect scores to form an overall affect score (e.g., Diener 2009; Diener et al. 2012; Robinson et al. 1991; Van Schuur and Kruijtbosch 1995). The resulting affect score is called the “affect balance,” which represents one’s overall experienced psychological well-being at a given point in time (Bradburn 1969). Individuals with high affect balance were said to be in an overall affective mood state whereas those with low affect balance were said to have an overall negative affective state. Bradbum’s (1969) approach (i.e., compute the difference score between positive affect and negative affect) to measure overall affect has proven to be valid, in that the difference score correlates more strongly with two other prominent indicators of well-being, happiness and life satisfaction, than does either the positive or negative affect score, respectively (Bradburn 1969; Harding 1982). In addition, it has been suggested that one’s affective state is composed of both positive and negative states, and thus is better represented by a composite index (e.g., the difference score) than by either positive affect or negative affect separately (Robinson et al. 1991). Although previous research has suggested that positive and negative affect are distinct factors (e.g., Warr et al. 1983; Watson and Tellegen 1985, but also see Diener et al. 1985; Russell and Carroll 1999 for support for the bipolarity of positive and negative affect), Headey et al. (1984) argued that this should not prevent the use of the composite index and that the composite affect score can be better understood if compared with the measurement of intelligence: “general intelligence tests usually have separate sections dealing with quantitative ability and verbal ability, and results for the sections are considered separately… However, it is generally found that, if an overall measure of intelligence is required, it is best to combine the two scores” (p. 116). Thus, to describe individuals’ global affective states, composite scores computed based on positive and negative affect seem most appropriate. For these reasons and considerations, we computed the difference score between positive affect and negative affect and used it as a measure of participants’ overall affective state during the study (Bradburn 1969; van Schuur and Kruijtbosch 1995).
 
2
Univariate analyses showed that those in the positive condition liked the proofreading task more than did those in the negative condition (p < 0.05). No significant difference was found for the creative task.
 
3
Creativity has also been operationalized as the product of novelty and usefulness in previous research (Zhou and Oldham 2001). Thus, we also ran an ANOVA using the product of novelty and usefulness as the dependent variable. The result showed no significant difference among the three conditions, F (2,105) = 1.33, p > 0.05.
 
4
Univariate analyses showed that both those in the primed condition (p < 0.05) and those in the primed-and-told condition (p < 0.05) liked the proofreading task more than did those in the control condition. No significant difference was found for the creative tasks.
 
5
Again, we also ran an ANOVA using the product of novelty and usefulness as the dependent variable for each task. Results yielded a significant effect for the brainstorming task, F (2,101) = 3.56, p < 0.05, but a non-significant effect for the problem-solving task, F (2,101) = .07, p > 0.05. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that participants in the primed condition had higher creativity scores than those in the control condition (p < 0.05).
 
6
We thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing out this possibility.
 
7
We thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting this future direction.
 
8
We conducted regression analyses predicting post-study affect from condition, controlling for pre-study affect. These analyses revealed that, in Study 1, the effect of the condition was not significant at p < 0.05 for either positive affect, F (2,105) = 32.34, or negative affect, F (2,105) = 20.71. In Study 2, the effect of the condition was significant at p < 0.05 for positive affect, F (2,102) = 10.92, but not significant for negative affect, F (2,102) = 12.14.
 
Literature
go back to reference Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779–806. doi:10.1037/a0012815.CrossRefPubMed Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779–806. doi:10.​1037/​a0012815.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Baker, J. R., & Doran, M. S. (2002). Human resource management: In-basket exercises for school administrators. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Pub Inc. Baker, J. R., & Doran, M. S. (2002). Human resource management: In-basket exercises for school administrators. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Pub Inc.
go back to reference Bargh, J. A. (2007). Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes. New York: Psychology Press. Bargh, J. A. (2007). Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes. New York: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Barling, J., & MacEwen, K. E. (1991). Maternal employment experiences, attention problems and behavioral performance: A mediational model. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 12, 495–505. doi:10.1002/job.4030120604.CrossRef Barling, J., & MacEwen, K. E. (1991). Maternal employment experiences, attention problems and behavioral performance: A mediational model. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 12, 495–505. doi:10.​1002/​job.​4030120604.CrossRef
go back to reference Barsade, S., Brief, A., & Spataro, S. (2003). The affective revolution in organizational behavior: the emergence of a paradigm. In J. Greenbrg (Ed.), Organizational Behavior: The State of the Science (pp. 3–52). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Barsade, S., Brief, A., & Spataro, S. (2003). The affective revolution in organizational behavior: the emergence of a paradigm. In J. Greenbrg (Ed.), Organizational Behavior: The State of the Science (pp. 3–52). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Beal, D. J., Weiss, H. M., Barros, E., & MacDermid, S. M. (2005). An episodic process model of affective influences on performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1054–1068.CrossRefPubMed Beal, D. J., Weiss, H. M., Barros, E., & MacDermid, S. M. (2005). An episodic process model of affective influences on performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1054–1068.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bolls, P. D., Lang, A., & Potter, R. F. (2001). The effects of message valence and listener arousal on attention, memory, and facial muscular responses to radio advertisements. Communication Research, 28, 627–651. doi:10.1177/009365001028005003.CrossRef Bolls, P. D., Lang, A., & Potter, R. F. (2001). The effects of message valence and listener arousal on attention, memory, and facial muscular responses to radio advertisements. Communication Research, 28, 627–651. doi:10.​1177/​0093650010280050​03.CrossRef
go back to reference Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago, IL: Aldine. Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago, IL: Aldine.
go back to reference Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (2002). Unconscious motivations: Their activation, operation, and consequences. In A. Tesser, D. Stapel, & J. Wood (Eds.), Self and motivation: Emerging psychological perspectives (pp. 13–41). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRef Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (2002). Unconscious motivations: Their activation, operation, and consequences. In A. Tesser, D. Stapel, & J. Wood (Eds.), Self and motivation: Emerging psychological perspectives (pp. 13–41). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRef
go back to reference Clore, G., & Colcombe, S. (2003). The parallel worlds of affective concepts and feelings. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation (pp. 169–188). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Clore, G., & Colcombe, S. (2003). The parallel worlds of affective concepts and feelings. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation (pp. 169–188). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Dalal, R. S., Lam, H., Weiss, H. M., Welch, E. R., & Hulin, C. L. (2009). A within-person approach to work behavior and performance: Concurrent and lagged citizenship- counterproductivity associations, and dynamic relationships with affect and overall job performance. Academy of Management Journal, 52, 1051–1066. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2009.44636148.CrossRef Dalal, R. S., Lam, H., Weiss, H. M., Welch, E. R., & Hulin, C. L. (2009). A within-person approach to work behavior and performance: Concurrent and lagged citizenship- counterproductivity associations, and dynamic relationships with affect and overall job performance. Academy of Management Journal, 52, 1051–1066. doi:10.​5465/​AMJ.​2009.​44636148.CrossRef
go back to reference Diener, E. (2009). Assessing well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRef Diener, E. (2009). Assessing well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.CrossRefPubMed Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Diener, E., Fujita, F., Tay, L., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2012). Purpose, mood, and pleasure in predicting satisfaction judgments. Social Indicators Research, 105, 333–341.CrossRef Diener, E., Fujita, F., Tay, L., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2012). Purpose, mood, and pleasure in predicting satisfaction judgments. Social Indicators Research, 105, 333–341.CrossRef
go back to reference Elsbach, K. D., & Pratt, M. G. (2008). The physical environment in organizations. In J. P. Walsh & A. P. Brief (Eds.), Annals of the Academy of Management (pp. 181–224). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis. Elsbach, K. D., & Pratt, M. G. (2008). The physical environment in organizations. In J. P. Walsh & A. P. Brief (Eds.), Annals of the Academy of Management (pp. 181–224). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
go back to reference Harding, S. D. (1982). Psychological well-being in Great Britain: An evaluation of the Bradburn Affect Balance Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 3, 167–175.CrossRef Harding, S. D. (1982). Psychological well-being in Great Britain: An evaluation of the Bradburn Affect Balance Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 3, 167–175.CrossRef
go back to reference Hassin, R. R., Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (2005). The new unconscious. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hassin, R. R., Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (2005). The new unconscious. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Headey, B., Holstrom, E., & Wearing, A. (1984). Well-being and ill-being: Different dimensions? Social Indicators Research, 14, 115–140.CrossRef Headey, B., Holstrom, E., & Wearing, A. (1984). Well-being and ill-being: Different dimensions? Social Indicators Research, 14, 115–140.CrossRef
go back to reference Johnson, S. K., & Johnson, C. S. (2009). The secret life of mood: Causes and consequences of unconscious affect at work. In C. E. Hartel (Ed.), Emotions in groups, organizations and cultures (pp. 103–121). Bingley, UK: JAI Press.CrossRef Johnson, S. K., & Johnson, C. S. (2009). The secret life of mood: Causes and consequences of unconscious affect at work. In C. E. Hartel (Ed.), Emotions in groups, organizations and cultures (pp. 103–121). Bingley, UK: JAI Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Liddell, B. J., Williams, L. M., Rathjen, J., Shevrin, H., & Gordon, E. (2004). A temporal dissociation of subliminal versus supraliminal fear perception: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 479–486. doi:10.1162/089892904322926809.CrossRefPubMed Liddell, B. J., Williams, L. M., Rathjen, J., Shevrin, H., & Gordon, E. (2004). A temporal dissociation of subliminal versus supraliminal fear perception: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 479–486. doi:10.​1162/​0898929043229268​09.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nielsen, L., & Kaszniak, A. W. (2007). Conceptual, theoretical, and methodological issues in inferring subjective emotional experience: Recommendations for researchers. In J. J. B. Allen & J. Coan (Eds.), The Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. Nielsen, L., & Kaszniak, A. W. (2007). Conceptual, theoretical, and methodological issues in inferring subjective emotional experience: Recommendations for researchers. In J. J. B. Allen & J. Coan (Eds.), The Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Oldham, G. R., Cummings, A., Mischel, L. J., Schmidtke, J. M., & Zhou, J. (1995). Listen while you work? Quasi-experimental relations between personal-stereo handset use and employee work responses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 547–564. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.80.5.547.CrossRef Oldham, G. R., Cummings, A., Mischel, L. J., Schmidtke, J. M., & Zhou, J. (1995). Listen while you work? Quasi-experimental relations between personal-stereo handset use and employee work responses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 547–564. doi:10.​1037/​0021-9010.​80.​5.​547.CrossRef
go back to reference Quirin, M., Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2009). When nonsense sounds happy or sad: The implicit positive and negative affect test (IPANAT). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 500–516. doi:10.1037/a0016063.CrossRefPubMed Quirin, M., Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2009). When nonsense sounds happy or sad: The implicit positive and negative affect test (IPANAT). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 500–516. doi:10.​1037/​a0016063.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ravaja, N., Kallinen, K., Saari, T., & Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (2004). Suboptimal exposure to facial expressions when viewing video messages from a small screen: effects on emotion, attention, and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied, 10, 120–131. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.10.2.120.CrossRefPubMed Ravaja, N., Kallinen, K., Saari, T., & Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (2004). Suboptimal exposure to facial expressions when viewing video messages from a small screen: effects on emotion, attention, and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied, 10, 120–131. doi:10.​1037/​1076-898X.​10.​2.​120.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Robinson, J. P., Shaver, P. R., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Robinson, J. P., Shaver, P. R., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. B. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 3–30.CrossRefPubMed Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. B. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 3–30.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sohlberg, S., Billinghurst, A., & Nylén, S. (1998). Moderation of mood change after subliminal symbiotic stimulation. Four experiments contributing to the further demystification of Silverman’s “mommy and I are one” findings. Journal of Research in Personality, 32, 33–54. doi:10.1006/jrpe. 1997.2199.CrossRef Sohlberg, S., Billinghurst, A., & Nylén, S. (1998). Moderation of mood change after subliminal symbiotic stimulation. Four experiments contributing to the further demystification of Silverman’s “mommy and I are one” findings. Journal of Research in Personality, 32, 33–54. doi:10.​1006/​jrpe. 1997.2199.CrossRef
go back to reference Van Schuur, W. H., & Kruijtbosch, M. (1995). Measuring subjective well-being: Unfolding the bradburn affect balance scale. Social Indicators Research, 36, 49–74. doi:10.1007/BF01079396.CrossRef Van Schuur, W. H., & Kruijtbosch, M. (1995). Measuring subjective well-being: Unfolding the bradburn affect balance scale. Social Indicators Research, 36, 49–74. doi:10.​1007/​BF01079396.CrossRef
go back to reference Velmans, M. (2009). How to define consciousness: And how not to define consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16, 139–156. Velmans, M. (2009). How to define consciousness: And how not to define consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16, 139–156.
go back to reference Warr, P., Barter, J., & Brownbridge, G. (1983). On the independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 644–651.CrossRef Warr, P., Barter, J., & Brownbridge, G. (1983). On the independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 644–651.CrossRef
go back to reference Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at work. Research in Organizational Behavior, 18, 1–74. Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at work. Research in Organizational Behavior, 18, 1–74.
go back to reference Westen, D. (1998). Unconscious thought, feeling and motivation: The end of a century-long debate. In R. F. Bornstein & J. M. Masling (Eds.), Empirical perspectives on the psychoanalytic unconscious (pp. 1–43). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRef Westen, D. (1998). Unconscious thought, feeling and motivation: The end of a century-long debate. In R. F. Bornstein & J. M. Masling (Eds.), Empirical perspectives on the psychoanalytic unconscious (pp. 1–43). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRef
go back to reference Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–24.CrossRefPubMed Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–24.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Winkielman, P., Berridge, K. C., & Wilbarger, J. L. (2005). Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 121–135. doi:10.1177/0146167204271309.CrossRefPubMed Winkielman, P., Berridge, K. C., & Wilbarger, J. L. (2005). Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 121–135. doi:10.​1177/​0146167204271309​.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Winkielman, P., Zajonc, R. B., & Schwarz, N. (1997). Subliminal affective priming resists attributional interventions. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 433–465.CrossRef Winkielman, P., Zajonc, R. B., & Schwarz, N. (1997). Subliminal affective priming resists attributional interventions. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 433–465.CrossRef
go back to reference Zhou, J., & Oldham, G. R. (2001). Enhancing creative performance: Effects of expected developmental assessment strategies and creative personality. Journal of Creative Behavior, 35, 151–167.CrossRef Zhou, J., & Oldham, G. R. (2001). Enhancing creative performance: Effects of expected developmental assessment strategies and creative personality. Journal of Creative Behavior, 35, 151–167.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
The Effects of Unconsciously Derived Affect on Task Satisfaction and Performance
Authors
Xiaoxiao Hu
Seth Kaplan
Publication date
01-03-2015
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Journal of Business and Psychology / Issue 1/2015
Print ISSN: 0889-3268
Electronic ISSN: 1573-353X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-013-9331-8

Other articles of this Issue 1/2015

Journal of Business and Psychology 1/2015 Go to the issue