Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Social Justice Research 4/2008

01-12-2008

Does Moral Conviction Really Override Concerns About Procedural Justice? A Reexamination of the Value Protection Model

Authors: Jaime L. Napier, Tom R. Tyler

Published in: Social Justice Research | Issue 4/2008

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

A large research literature on procedural justice demonstrates that people are more accepting of decisions that they do not feel are advantageous or fair when those decisions are arrived at using just procedures. Recently, several papers (Skitka, Pers Soc Psychol Bull, 28:588–597, 2002; Skitka and Mullen, Pers Soc Psychol Bull, 28:1419–1429, 2002) have argued that these procedural mechanisms do not have a significant influence when the decision made concerns issues about which those involved have strong moral feelings (“a moral mandate”). A reanalysis of the data in these two studies indicates that, contrary to the strong position taken by the authors, i.e. that “when people have a moral mandate about an outcome, any means justifies the mandated end” (Skitka, Pers Soc Psychol Bull, 28:594, 2002), the justice of decision-making procedures is consistently found to significantly influence people’s reactions to decisions by authorities and institutions even when their moral mandates are threatened.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Footnotes
1
We were not able to generate a dataset or analyze a correlation matrix that included all of the variables listed in Skitka’s original correlation matrix (2002, p. 593) because the matrix was not positive definite. We therefore broke up the data by policy domain for both the data generation and the structural equation models. Additional analyses revealed that this is most likely due to multivariate linear dependencies, where several variables together perfectly predict another variable. Specifically, a non-positive definite matrix occurs when trying to predict outcome fairness in the civil rights domain with pre-threat procedural fairness and the three moral mandates. By entering moral mandates two at a time, we found that moral mandates in abortion and homosexual civil rights accounted for 70% of the variance in outcome fairness ratings of a court decision granting rights to homosexuals; moral mandates in immigration and homosexual civil rights accounted for 51% of the variance in outcome fairness ratings; and strikingly, moral mandates in abortion and immigration accounted for 30% of the variance in outcome fairness ratings of a homosexual rights policy decision. Such results suggest that the operationalizations of the outcome fairness and moral mandate variables in the study may have conceptually overlapped.
 
2
Another way to investigate whether this measure was indeed tapping outcome fairness is to see how judgments of the procedures at Time 1 relate to judgments of outcome fairness at Time 2 when the perceiver does not have a moral mandate. In this case, we would expect to find the generally robust fair-process effect (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Thibaut & Walker, 1975). We selected participants who had a “moral mandate” score between −1 and 1 of the original mandate measure for each domain. The correlations between pre-threat procedural justice ratings and post-threat fairness ratings showed that evaluations of outcome fairness at Time 2 are almost completely unrelated to evaluations of the procedures at Time 1. This offers further support for the speculation that the operationalization of this measure was not tapping perceptions of fairness.
 
3
Our analysis thus only uses one of the three dependent variables evaluated in Skitka (2002). While she uses post-threat outcome fairness, procedural fairness, and moral outrage, we only examine post-threat procedural fairness. As reported in Skitka (2002), we did not find effects of pre-threat ratings of the procedures on post-threat outcome fairness. However, we were hesitant to conclude that pre-threat judgments did not affect post-threat outcome fairness because (1) the operationalization of outcome fairness was empirically indistinguishable from that of moral outrage, and (2) post-threat outcome fairness was not associated with pre-threat fairness ratings even for those without a moral mandate, as reported in Note 2. Using only post-threat procedural fairness ratings is a clear limitation of this study, but we address this issue in Study 2. In that study, we find that the evaluations of the procedures are related to final ratings of both outcome and procedural fairness.
 
4
According to Skitka and Mullen (2002a), 17.3% of the participants were classified as having a moral mandate that Elian should stay in the United States (“anti-decision mandate”), 37.5% of participants had a mandate that Elian should return to Cuba (“pro-decision mandate”), and 45.3% of the participants could be classified as having “weaker or no moral mandate (p. 1423).” Thus, we re-ran our analysis with moral mandate dummy-codes based on these exact guidelines. For the anti-decision mandate dummy-code, the bottom 17.3% of the data were assigned “1” and the middle 45.3% were assigned a “0”. Likewise, the top 37.5% of the data were assigned a “1” for a pro-decision mandate dummy-code and the middle 45.3% were assigned a “0”. In two regression models, we examined the effects of procedural justice, moral mandates (either pro- or anti-decision), and the interaction term on post-resolution measures. The results from these analyses were completely consistent with the results reported in Table 4, such that pre-raid procedural justice influences all of the post-resolution measures and these effects were not qualified by moral mandates.
 
Literature
go back to reference Almond, G. A., Appleby, R. S., & Sivan, E. (2002). Strong religion: The rise of fundamentalisms around the world. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Almond, G. A., Appleby, R. S., & Sivan, E. (2002). Strong religion: The rise of fundamentalisms around the world. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Antoun, R. (2001). Understanding fundamentalism: Christian, Islamic and Jewish movements. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. Antoun, R. (2001). Understanding fundamentalism: Christian, Islamic and Jewish movements. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
go back to reference Bentler, P. M., & Wu, E. J. C. (1995). EQS/Windows user’s guide. Los Angeles: BMDP, Statistical Software. Bentler, P. M., & Wu, E. J. C. (1995). EQS/Windows user’s guide. Los Angeles: BMDP, Statistical Software.
go back to reference Barrett-Howard, E., & Tyler, T. R. (1986). Procedural justice as a criterion in allocation decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 296–304.CrossRef Barrett-Howard, E., & Tyler, T. R. (1986). Procedural justice as a criterion in allocation decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 296–304.CrossRef
go back to reference Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105. Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105.
go back to reference Haidt, J. (2001). The emotion dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834.PubMedCrossRef Haidt, J. (2001). The emotion dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20, 98–116.CrossRef Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20, 98–116.CrossRef
go back to reference Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.PubMedCrossRef Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jost, J. T., Napier, J. L., Thorisdottir, H., Gosling, S. D., Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Do needs to reduce uncertainty and threat predict political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 989–1007. Jost, J. T., Napier, J. L., Thorisdottir, H., Gosling, S. D., Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Do needs to reduce uncertainty and threat predict political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 989–1007.
go back to reference Krueger, A. B., & Malečková, J. (2003). Education, poverty and terrorism: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 17, 119–144.CrossRef Krueger, A. B., & Malečková, J. (2003). Education, poverty and terrorism: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 17, 119–144.CrossRef
go back to reference Lakoff, G. (2002). Moral politics: How liberals and conservatives think. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, G. (2002). Moral politics: How liberals and conservatives think. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Leventhal, G. S. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. J. Gergen, M. S. Greenbers, & R. H. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27–53). New York: Plenum. Leventhal, G. S. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. J. Gergen, M. S. Greenbers, & R. H. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27–53). New York: Plenum.
go back to reference Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. In Critical issues in social justice. New York: Plenum Press. Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. In Critical issues in social justice. New York: Plenum Press.
go back to reference Marty, M. E., & Appleby, R. S. (1994). Fundamentalisms observed. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Marty, M. E., & Appleby, R. S. (1994). Fundamentalisms observed. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Mullen, E., & Skitka, L. J. (2006). Exploring the psychological underpinnings of the moral mandate effect: Motivated reasoning, identification, or affect? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 629–643. Mullen, E., & Skitka, L. J. (2006). Exploring the psychological underpinnings of the moral mandate effect: Motivated reasoning, identification, or affect? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 629–643.
go back to reference Skitka, L. J. (2002). Do the means always justify the ends, or do the ends sometimes justify the means? A value protection model of justice reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 588–597.CrossRef Skitka, L. J. (2002). Do the means always justify the ends, or do the ends sometimes justify the means? A value protection model of justice reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 588–597.CrossRef
go back to reference Skitka, L. J., & Houston, D. A. (2001). When due process is of no consequence: Moral mandates and presumed defendant guilt or innocence. Social Justice Research, 14, 305–326.CrossRef Skitka, L. J., & Houston, D. A. (2001). When due process is of no consequence: Moral mandates and presumed defendant guilt or innocence. Social Justice Research, 14, 305–326.CrossRef
go back to reference Skitka, L. J., & Mullen, E. (2002a). Understanding judgments of fairness in a real-world political context: A test of the value protection model of justice reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1419–1429.CrossRef Skitka, L. J., & Mullen, E. (2002a). Understanding judgments of fairness in a real-world political context: A test of the value protection model of justice reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1419–1429.CrossRef
go back to reference Skitka, L. J., & Mullen, E. (2002b). The dark side of moral conviction. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 2, 35–41.CrossRef Skitka, L. J., & Mullen, E. (2002b). The dark side of moral conviction. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 2, 35–41.CrossRef
go back to reference Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Tyler, T. R. (1988). What is procedural justice? Criteria used by citizens to assess the fairness of legal procedures. Law and Society Review, 22, 103–135.CrossRef Tyler, T. R. (1988). What is procedural justice? Criteria used by citizens to assess the fairness of legal procedures. Law and Society Review, 22, 103–135.CrossRef
go back to reference Tyler, T. R. (1994). Psychological models of the justice motive: Antecedents of distributive and procedural justice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 850–863.CrossRef Tyler, T. R. (1994). Psychological models of the justice motive: Antecedents of distributive and procedural justice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 850–863.CrossRef
go back to reference Tyler, T. R. (2000). Why do people cooperate in groups? Support for structural solutions to social dilemma problems. In M. Van Vugt, M. Snyder, T. R. Tyler, & A. Biel (Eds.), Cooperation in modern society: Promoting the welfare of communities, states and organizations (pp. 64–83). New York: Routledge. Tyler, T. R. (2000). Why do people cooperate in groups? Support for structural solutions to social dilemma problems. In M. Van Vugt, M. Snyder, T. R. Tyler, & A. Biel (Eds.), Cooperation in modern society: Promoting the welfare of communities, states and organizations (pp. 64–83). New York: Routledge.
go back to reference Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2003). The group engagement model: Procedural justice, social identity, and cooperative behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 349–361.PubMedCrossRef Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2003). The group engagement model: Procedural justice, social identity, and cooperative behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 349–361.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tyler, T. R., Boeckmann, R. J., Smith, H. J., & Huo, Y. J. (1997). Social justice in a diverse society. Bolder, CO: Westview Press. Tyler, T. R., Boeckmann, R. J., Smith, H. J., & Huo, Y. J. (1997). Social justice in a diverse society. Bolder, CO: Westview Press.
go back to reference Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 25, pp. 115–191). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 25, pp. 115–191). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Tyler, T. R., & Smith, H. (1998). Social justice and social movements. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th edn., vol. 2, pp. 595–629). New York: McGraw-Hill. Tyler, T. R., & Smith, H. (1998). Social justice and social movements. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th edn., vol. 2, pp. 595–629). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Metadata
Title
Does Moral Conviction Really Override Concerns About Procedural Justice? A Reexamination of the Value Protection Model
Authors
Jaime L. Napier
Tom R. Tyler
Publication date
01-12-2008
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Social Justice Research / Issue 4/2008
Print ISSN: 0885-7466
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6725
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-008-0083-y

Other articles of this Issue 4/2008

Social Justice Research 4/2008 Go to the issue