Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Political Behavior 4/2009

01-12-2009 | Original Paper

The Unmet Potential of Interdisciplinary Research: Political Psychological Approaches to Voting and Public Opinion

Authors: James N. Druckman, James H. Kuklinski, Lee Sigelman

Published in: Political Behavior | Issue 4/2009

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Since its founding, political science has embraced interdisciplinary research. Yet there exist few, if any, systematic assessments of the success of these endeavors. We assess what is often seen as a paradigm of interdisciplinary collaboration: political psychological research on voting and public opinion. Surprisingly, we find little evidence of true interdisciplinary work; instead, we uncover misused concepts and scant evidence of conceptual or disciplinary integration. We conclude with suggestions for how to improve interdisciplinary research on voting and public opinion, and more generally.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Footnotes
1
Consistent with this implication, it has become fairly common for scholars with a Ph.D. in psychology to hold their main academic appointment in a department of political science, but it is rare for scholars with a Ph.D. in political science to hold their main academic appointment in a department of psychology. Herbert Simon was one such rare case, but we are hard-pressed to think of others.
 
2
This is particularly true for heuristics, media priming, and on-line processing; because motivated reasoning has emerged more recently in political science (e.g., Taber and Lodge 2006), the centrality of its status is less certain. One of the central concepts we are not considering is emotion; our impression is that a thorough examination of extant political science research might reveal dynamics similar to what we observe for our four concepts (for discussion, see Druckman and McDermott 2008, pp. 301–302).
 
3
Alternatively, Lenz (2009) argues that media priming is in fact akin to learning.
 
4
Lodge et al. (1989) focus much of their attention on Kelley and Mirer’s (1974, p. 574) model, in which “The voter canvasses his likes and dislikes of the leading candidates and major parties involved in an election. Weighing each like and dislike equally, he votes for the candidate toward whom he has the greatest number of net favorable attitudes.” However, Lodge et al. (1989, p. 417) also note the contrast with rational choice, stating “It is increasingly evident that citizens are not well informed and cannot engage in the computations required by most theories of voter rationality. We add to this the caveat that candidate memory is distorted by systematic biases that stem directly from impression-driven processing. However, from a cognitive perspective, these consequences of impression-driven evaluation are a natural and (dare we say) ‘reasonable’ way to compensate for the severe limitations on human information processing that render models of unbounded rationality psychologically unrealistic.”
 
5
The basic idea of motivated reasoning was not new to political science (e.g., Sears and Whitney 1973), but there had previously been no grounded theory.
 
6
One is, however, more likely to engage in motivated reasoning when one’s OL tally is strong (Lodge and Taber 2000, p. 211).
 
7
However, for further development of the model in yet to be published papers, see Lodge et al. (2008), and Taber et al. (2009). Also, see Braman and Nelson (2007), which was published after 2006, the cut-off year in our content analysis.
 
8
That other individual difference variables significantly matter is made clear by the aforementioned research by McGraw and her colleagues.
 
9
Another possible hurdle to integration is that the methods and measures used in psychology often differ from those employed in political science. For example, response latency measures are more common in psychology, and many psychological measures are captured with lengthier batteries of questions (e.g., the original need-to-evaluate measure uses 16 items, versus the two or three used on the National Election Study). It is important for scholars from each discipline to consider such differences when collaborating and integrating.
 
10
Psychologists do explore other concepts that clearly fall within the realm of political behavior, such as collective guilt, intergroup conflict, and ideology/values (e.g., Schwartz 1994; Branscombe and Doosje 2004; Jost et al. 2008). However, much of this work has not penetrated very deeply into political science.
 
11
Some recent political science studies that explore over-time processes include Mutz and Reeves (2005); Gerber et al. (2007); Chong and Druckman (2008).
 
12
Interestingly, the three examples we explore—heuristics, media priming, and on-line processing/motivated reasoning—all entered the political science literature due to collaborative projects involving political scientists and psychologists (e.g., Sniderman and Tetlock, Iyengar and Kinder, Lodge and McGraw). There are some positive signs of further movement in this direction. For example, applications for the Summer Institute for the Study of Political Psychology are now largely split between the two disciplines (personal communication); the National Election Studies board of overseers is now made up of scholars from multiple disciplines; and psychologists and political behavior scholars have shown considerable recent interest in the phenomena of implicit and explicit priming (e.g., Althaus and Kim 2006; Lodge et al. 2008).
 
Literature
go back to reference Allen, B., O’Loughlin, P., Jasperson, A., & Sullivan, J. L. (1994). The media and the Gulf War: Framing, priming, and the spiral of silence. Polity, 27, 255–284. doi:10.2307/3235175.CrossRef Allen, B., O’Loughlin, P., Jasperson, A., & Sullivan, J. L. (1994). The media and the Gulf War: Framing, priming, and the spiral of silence. Polity, 27, 255–284. doi:10.​2307/​3235175.CrossRef
go back to reference Bartels, L. M. (1996). Uninformed votes: Information effects in presidential elections. American Journal of Political Science, 40, 194–230. doi:10.2307/2111700.CrossRef Bartels, L. M. (1996). Uninformed votes: Information effects in presidential elections. American Journal of Political Science, 40, 194–230. doi:10.​2307/​2111700.CrossRef
go back to reference Bartels, L. M. (2003). Democracy with attitudes. In M. B. MacKuen & G. Rabinowitz (Eds.), Electoral democracy. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Bartels, L. M. (2003). Democracy with attitudes. In M. B. MacKuen & G. Rabinowitz (Eds.), Electoral democracy. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
go back to reference Berelson, B. R., Lazarsfeld, P. F., & McPhee, W. N. (1954). Voting: A study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Berelson, B. R., Lazarsfeld, P. F., & McPhee, W. N. (1954). Voting: A study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Bizer, G. Y., Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Wheller, S. C., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). The impact of personality on cognitive, behavioral, and affective political processes: The effects of need to evaluate. Journal of Personality, 72, 995–1027. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00288.x.CrossRef Bizer, G. Y., Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Wheller, S. C., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). The impact of personality on cognitive, behavioral, and affective political processes: The effects of need to evaluate. Journal of Personality, 72, 995–1027. doi:10.​1111/​j.​0022-3506.​2004.​00288.​x.CrossRef
go back to reference Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (Eds.). (2004). Collective guilt. New York: Cambridge University Press. Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (Eds.). (2004). Collective guilt. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2005). Individual differences in attitude change. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes and attitude change. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2005). Individual differences in attitude change. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes and attitude change. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press. Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Chong, D. (2007). The value of information and endorsements. Unpublished paper, Northwestern University. Chong, D. (2007). The value of information and endorsements. Unpublished paper, Northwestern University.
go back to reference Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2008). Dynamic public opinion: Framing effects over time. Presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, Boston, MA, August 28–August 31. Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2008). Dynamic public opinion: Framing effects over time. Presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, Boston, MA, August 28–August 31.
go back to reference Committee on Facilitating Interdisciplinary Research. (2005). Facilitating interdisciplinary research. Washington: National Academies Press. Committee on Facilitating Interdisciplinary Research. (2005). Facilitating interdisciplinary research. Washington: National Academies Press.
go back to reference Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press. Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press.
go back to reference Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: HarperCollins. Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: HarperCollins.
go back to reference Druckman, J. N. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (ir)relevance of framing effects. The American Political Science Review, 98, 671–686. doi:10.1017/S0003055404041413. Druckman, J. N. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (ir)relevance of framing effects. The American Political Science Review, 98, 671–686. doi:10.​1017/​S000305540404141​3.
go back to reference Druckman, J. N., & Lupia, A. (2006). Mind, will, and choice: Lessons From experiments in contextual variation”. In R. E. Goodin & C. Tilly (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of contextual political analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Druckman, J. N., & Lupia, A. (2006). Mind, will, and choice: Lessons From experiments in contextual variation”. In R. E. Goodin & C. Tilly (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of contextual political analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace. Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace.
go back to reference Edwards, W., Lindman, H., & Savage, L. J. (1963). Bayesian statistical inference for psychological research. Psychological Review, 38, 193–242. doi:10.1037/h0044139.CrossRef Edwards, W., Lindman, H., & Savage, L. J. (1963). Bayesian statistical inference for psychological research. Psychological Review, 38, 193–242. doi:10.​1037/​h0044139.CrossRef
go back to reference Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Fazio, R. H. (2000). Accessible attitudes as tools for object appraisal: Their costs and benefits. In G. R. Maio & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate: Functions of attitudes. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Fazio, R. H. (2000). Accessible attitudes as tools for object appraisal: Their costs and benefits. In G. R. Maio & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate: Functions of attitudes. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Federico, C. M., & Schneider, M. C. (2007). Political expertise and the use of ideology: Moderating effects of evaluative motivation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71, 221–252. doi:10.1093/poq/nfm010.CrossRef Federico, C. M., & Schneider, M. C. (2007). Political expertise and the use of ideology: Moderating effects of evaluative motivation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71, 221–252. doi:10.​1093/​poq/​nfm010.CrossRef
go back to reference Ferejohn, J. A., & Kuklinski, J. H. (Eds.). (1990). Information and democratic processes. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Ferejohn, J. A., & Kuklinski, J. H. (Eds.). (1990). Information and democratic processes. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
go back to reference Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
go back to reference Fiorina, M. (1981). Retrospective voting in American elections. New Haven: Yale University Press. Fiorina, M. (1981). Retrospective voting in American elections. New Haven: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Gerber, A., Gimpel, J. G., Green, D. P., & Shaw, D. R. (2007). The influence of television and radio advertising on candidate evaluations: Results from a large scale randomized experiment. Unpublished paper, Yale University. Gerber, A., Gimpel, J. G., Green, D. P., & Shaw, D. R. (2007). The influence of television and radio advertising on candidate evaluations: Results from a large scale randomized experiment. Unpublished paper, Yale University.
go back to reference Gilovich, T., & Griffin, D. (2002). Introduction—Heuristics and biases: Then and now. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. New York: Cambridge University Press. Gilovich, T., & Griffin, D. (2002). Introduction—Heuristics and biases: Then and now. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. New York: Guilford Press. Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Higgins, E. T., & King, G. (1981). Social constructs: Information-processing consequences of individual and contextual variability”. In N. Cantor & J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Personality, cognition, and social interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Higgins, E. T., & King, G. (1981). Social constructs: Information-processing consequences of individual and contextual variability”. In N. Cantor & J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Personality, cognition, and social interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Huckfeldt, R., Johnson, P. E., & Sprague, J. (2004). Political disagreement: The survival of diverse opinions within communication networks. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Huckfeldt, R., Johnson, P. E., & Sprague, J. (2004). Political disagreement: The survival of diverse opinions within communication networks. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters: Television and American opinion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters: Television and American opinion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Jackman, S., & Sniderman, P. M. (2002). The institutional organization of choice spaces: A political conception of political psychology. In K. Monroe (Ed.), Political psychology. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum. Jackman, S., & Sniderman, P. M. (2002). The institutional organization of choice spaces: A political conception of political psychology. In K. Monroe (Ed.), Political psychology. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Kinder, D. R. (1998). Opinion and action in the realm of politics. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Kinder, D. R. (1998). Opinion and action in the realm of politics. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
go back to reference Kinder, D. R., & Sears, D. O. (1985). Public opinion and political action. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Random House. Kinder, D. R., & Sears, D. O. (1985). Public opinion and political action. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Random House.
go back to reference Krosnick, J. A. (2002). Is political psychology sufficiently psyhological: Distinghuishing political psychology from psychological political science. In J. H. Kuklinksi (Ed.), Thinking about political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press. Krosnick, J. A. (2002). Is political psychology sufficiently psyhological: Distinghuishing political psychology from psychological political science. In J. H. Kuklinksi (Ed.), Thinking about political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Kuklinski, J. H. (Ed.). (2001). Citizens and politics: Perspectives from political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kuklinski, J. H. (Ed.). (2001). Citizens and politics: Perspectives from political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Kuklinski, J. H. (Ed.). (2002). Thinking about political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kuklinski, J. H. (Ed.). (2002). Thinking about political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Kuklinski, J. H., & Jerit, J. (2001). Commentary. In J. H. Kuklinski (Ed.), Citizens and politics: Perspectives from political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kuklinski, J. H., & Jerit, J. (2001). Commentary. In J. H. Kuklinski (Ed.), Citizens and politics: Perspectives from political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2000). Reconsidering the rational public: Cognition, heuristics, and mass opinion”. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Understanding and expanding the limits of political rationality. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2000). Reconsidering the rational public: Cognition, heuristics, and mass opinion”. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Understanding and expanding the limits of political rationality. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Kuklinski, J. H., Quirk, P. J., Jerit, J., & Rich, R. F. (2001). The political environment and citizen competence. American Journal of Political Science, 45, 410–424. doi:10.2307/2669349.CrossRef Kuklinski, J. H., Quirk, P. J., Jerit, J., & Rich, R. F. (2001). The political environment and citizen competence. American Journal of Political Science, 45, 410–424. doi:10.​2307/​2669349.CrossRef
go back to reference Lasswell, H. D. (1946). Describing the contents of communication. In B. L. Smith, H. D. Lasswell, & R. D. Casey (Eds.), Propaganda, communication, and public opinion. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Lasswell, H. D. (1946). Describing the contents of communication. In B. L. Smith, H. D. Lasswell, & R. D. Casey (Eds.), Propaganda, communication, and public opinion. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
go back to reference Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2006). How voters decide: Information processing in election campaigns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2006). How voters decide: Information processing in election campaigns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Lenz, G. S. (2009). Learning and opinion change, not priming: Reconsidering the evidence for the priming hypothesis. American Journal of Political Science, 53 (forthcoming). Lenz, G. S. (2009). Learning and opinion change, not priming: Reconsidering the evidence for the priming hypothesis. American Journal of Political Science, 53 (forthcoming).
go back to reference Lippmann, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: MacMillan. Lippmann, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: MacMillan.
go back to reference Lodge, M. (1995). Toward a procedural model of candidate evaluation. In M. Lodge & K. M. McGraw (Eds.), Political judgment: Structure and process. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Lodge, M. (1995). Toward a procedural model of candidate evaluation. In M. Lodge & K. M. McGraw (Eds.), Political judgment: Structure and process. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
go back to reference Lodge, M., McGraw, K. M., & Stroh, P. (1989). An impression-driven model of candidate evaluation. The American Political Science Review, 83, 399–420. doi:10.2307/1962397.CrossRef Lodge, M., McGraw, K. M., & Stroh, P. (1989). An impression-driven model of candidate evaluation. The American Political Science Review, 83, 399–420. doi:10.​2307/​1962397.CrossRef
go back to reference Lodge, M., Steenbergen, M. R., & Brau, S. (1995). The responsive voter: Campaign information and the dynamics of candidate evaluation. The American Political Science Review, 89, 309–326. doi:10.2307/2082427.CrossRef Lodge, M., Steenbergen, M. R., & Brau, S. (1995). The responsive voter: Campaign information and the dynamics of candidate evaluation. The American Political Science Review, 89, 309–326. doi:10.​2307/​2082427.CrossRef
go back to reference Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2000). Three steps toward a theory of motivated political reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Cognition, choice, and the bounds of rationality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2000). Three steps toward a theory of motivated political reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Cognition, choice, and the bounds of rationality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2008). The rationalizing voter: Unconscious thought in political information processing. Unpublished paper, Stony Brook University. Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2008). The rationalizing voter: Unconscious thought in political information processing. Unpublished paper, Stony Brook University.
go back to reference Lodge, M., Taber, C. S., & Burdein, I. (2008). The impact of self-identification on political attitudes: An experimental test employing subliminal priming. Unpublished Manuscript, Stony Brook University. Lodge, M., Taber, C. S., & Burdein, I. (2008). The impact of self-identification on political attitudes: An experimental test employing subliminal priming. Unpublished Manuscript, Stony Brook University.
go back to reference Lupia, A., & McCubbins, M. D. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know?. New York: Cambridge University Press. Lupia, A., & McCubbins, M. D. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know?. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Lupia, A., McCubbins, M. D., & Popkin, S. L. (2000). Beyond rationality: Reason and the study of politics. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Understanding and expanding the limits of political rationality. New York: Cambridge University Press. Lupia, A., McCubbins, M. D., & Popkin, S. L. (2000). Beyond rationality: Reason and the study of politics. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Understanding and expanding the limits of political rationality. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Luskin, R. C. (2001). The heavenly public: What would be the ideal democratic citizenry be like. In G. Rabinowitz & M. B. MacKuen (Eds.), Electoral democracy. Ann Arbor, Mi: University of Michigan Press. Luskin, R. C. (2001). The heavenly public: What would be the ideal democratic citizenry be like. In G. Rabinowitz & M. B. MacKuen (Eds.), Electoral democracy. Ann Arbor, Mi: University of Michigan Press.
go back to reference MacDonald, P. K. (2003). Useful fiction or miracle maker: The competing epistemological foundations of rational choice theory. The American Political Science Review, 97, 551–565. doi:10.1017/S000305540300087X. MacDonald, P. K. (2003). Useful fiction or miracle maker: The competing epistemological foundations of rational choice theory. The American Political Science Review, 97, 551–565. doi:10.​1017/​S000305540300087​X.
go back to reference Martin, L. L., & Achee, J. W. (1992). Beyond accessibility: The role of processing objectives in judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Martin, L. L., & Achee, J. W. (1992). Beyond accessibility: The role of processing objectives in judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference McGraw, K. M. (2003). Political impressions: Formation and management. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. McGraw, K. M. (2003). Political impressions: Formation and management. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference McGraw, K. M. (2006). The infrastructure of political psychology. In L. Valenty (Ed.), Political psychology. Oplanden & Farmington Hills: Barbara Budich Publishers. McGraw, K. M. (2006). The infrastructure of political psychology. In L. Valenty (Ed.), Political psychology. Oplanden & Farmington Hills: Barbara Budich Publishers.
go back to reference McGraw, K. M., Lodge, M., & Stroh, P. (1990). On-line processing in candidate evaluation: The effects of issue order, issue salience, and sophistication. Political Behavior, 12, 41–58. doi:10.1007/BF00992331.CrossRef McGraw, K. M., Lodge, M., & Stroh, P. (1990). On-line processing in candidate evaluation: The effects of issue order, issue salience, and sophistication. Political Behavior, 12, 41–58. doi:10.​1007/​BF00992331.CrossRef
go back to reference Mendelsohn, M. (1996). The media and interpersonal communication: The priming of issues, leaders, and party identification. The Journal of Politics, 58, 112–125. doi:10.2307/2960351.CrossRef Mendelsohn, M. (1996). The media and interpersonal communication: The priming of issues, leaders, and party identification. The Journal of Politics, 58, 112–125. doi:10.​2307/​2960351.CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations: Politically knowledgeable citizens are guided by a trusted source. American Journal of Political Science, 44, 295–309. Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations: Politically knowledgeable citizens are guided by a trusted source. American Journal of Political Science, 44, 295–309.
go back to reference Monroe, K. (Ed.). (2002). Political psychology. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum. Monroe, K. (Ed.). (2002). Political psychology. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Mutz, D. C. (2006). Hearing the other side: Deliberative versus participatory democracy. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Mutz, D. C. (2006). Hearing the other side: Deliberative versus participatory democracy. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Mutz, D. C., & Reeves, B. (2005). The new videomalaise: Effects of televised incivility on political trust. The American Political Science Review, 99, 1–15. doi:10.1017/S0003055405051452. Mutz, D. C., & Reeves, B. (2005). The new videomalaise: Effects of televised incivility on political trust. The American Political Science Review, 99, 1–15. doi:10.​1017/​S000305540505145​2.
go back to reference Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing of a civil liberties conflict and its effect on tolerance. The American Political Science Review, 91, 567–583. doi:10.2307/2952075.CrossRef Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing of a civil liberties conflict and its effect on tolerance. The American Political Science Review, 91, 567–583. doi:10.​2307/​2952075.CrossRef
go back to reference Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. D. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. D. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
go back to reference O’Keefe, D. J. (2002). Persuasion (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. O’Keefe, D. J. (2002). Persuasion (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
go back to reference Oppenheim, P., & Putnam, H. (1958). Unity of science as a working hypothesis. In H. Fiegel, M. Scriven, & G. Maxwell (Eds.), Studies in the philosophy of science (Vol. II). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Oppenheim, P., & Putnam, H. (1958). Unity of science as a working hypothesis. In H. Fiegel, M. Scriven, & G. Maxwell (Eds.), Studies in the philosophy of science (Vol. II). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
go back to reference Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1992). The rational public: Fifty years of trends in Americans’ policy preferences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1992). The rational public: Fifty years of trends in Americans’ policy preferences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York: Cambridge University Press. Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer.
go back to reference Popkin, S. L. (1991). The reasoning voter: Communication and persuasion in presidential campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Popkin, S. L. (1991). The reasoning voter: Communication and persuasion in presidential campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Proceedings of the American Political Science Association. (1904). The organization of the American Political Science Association. Proceedings of the American Political Science Association, 1, 5–15. Proceedings of the American Political Science Association. (1904). The organization of the American Political Science Association. Proceedings of the American Political Science Association, 1, 5–15.
go back to reference Rahn, W. M., Aldrich, J. H., & Borgida, E. (1994). Individual and contextual variations in political candidate appraisal. The American Political Science Review, 88, 193–199. doi:10.2307/2944891.CrossRef Rahn, W. M., Aldrich, J. H., & Borgida, E. (1994). Individual and contextual variations in political candidate appraisal. The American Political Science Review, 88, 193–199. doi:10.​2307/​2944891.CrossRef
go back to reference Redlawsk, D. (2001). Hot cognition or cool consideration: Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. The Journal of Politics, 64, 1021–1044. Redlawsk, D. (2001). Hot cognition or cool consideration: Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. The Journal of Politics, 64, 1021–1044.
go back to reference Riker, W. H. (1996). The strategy of rhetoric: Campaigning for the American constitution. New Haven: Yale University Press. Riker, W. H. (1996). The strategy of rhetoric: Campaigning for the American constitution. New Haven: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and content of human values? The Journal of Social Issues, 50, 19–46.CrossRef Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and content of human values? The Journal of Social Issues, 50, 19–46.CrossRef
go back to reference Sears, D. O., Huddy, L., & Jervis, R. (Eds.). (2003). Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Sears, D. O., Huddy, L., & Jervis, R. (Eds.). (2003). Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Sears, D. O., & Whitney, R. W. (1973). Political persuasion. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Sears, D. O., & Whitney, R. W. (1973). Political persuasion. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
go back to reference Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. (Eds.). (1969). Interdisciplinary relationships in the social sciences. Chicago: Aldine. Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. (Eds.). (1969). Interdisciplinary relationships in the social sciences. Chicago: Aldine.
go back to reference Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man. New York: Wiley. Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Simon, H. A. (1963). Problems of methodology. American Economic Review Proceedings, 53, 229–231. Discussion. Simon, H. A. (1963). Problems of methodology. American Economic Review Proceedings, 53, 229–231. Discussion.
go back to reference Sniderman, P. M. (2000). Taking sides: A fixed choice theory of political reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Cognition, choice, and the bounds of rationality. New York: Cambridge University Press. Sniderman, P. M. (2000). Taking sides: A fixed choice theory of political reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Cognition, choice, and the bounds of rationality. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1991). Reasoning and choice: Explorations in political psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1991). Reasoning and choice: Explorations in political psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Somin, I. (2006). Knowledge about ignorance: New directions in the study of political information. Critical Review, 18, 255–278. Somin, I. (2006). Knowledge about ignorance: New directions in the study of political information. Critical Review, 18, 255–278.
go back to reference Stapel, D., Koomen, W., & Zeelenberg, M. (1998). The impact of accuracy motivation on interpretation, comparison, and correction processes: Accuracy × knowledge accessibility effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 878–893. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.4.878.CrossRef Stapel, D., Koomen, W., & Zeelenberg, M. (1998). The impact of accuracy motivation on interpretation, comparison, and correction processes: Accuracy × knowledge accessibility effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 878–893. doi:10.​1037/​0022-3514.​74.​4.​878.CrossRef
go back to reference Taber, C. S. (2003). Information processing in public opinion. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Taber, C. S. (2003). Information processing in public opinion. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Taber, C. S., Cann, A., & Kucsova, S. (2009). The motivated processing of political arguments. Unpublished paper, Stony Brook University. Taber, C. S., Cann, A., & Kucsova, S. (2009). The motivated processing of political arguments. Unpublished paper, Stony Brook University.
go back to reference Taber, C. S., Lodge, M., & Glather, J. (2001). The motivated construction of political judgment. In J. H. Kuklinski (Ed.), Citizens and politics: Perspectives from political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press. Taber, C. S., Lodge, M., & Glather, J. (2001). The motivated construction of political judgment. In J. H. Kuklinski (Ed.), Citizens and politics: Perspectives from political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1978). Salience, attention, and attribution: Top-of-the-head phenomena. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 11). New York: Academic Press. Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1978). Salience, attention, and attribution: Top-of-the-head phenomena. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 11). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Thompson, E. P., Roman, R. J., Moskowitz, G. B., Chaiken, S., & Bargh, J. (1994). Accuracy motivation attenuates covert priming: The systematic reprocessing of social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 474–489. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.3.474.CrossRef Thompson, E. P., Roman, R. J., Moskowitz, G. B., Chaiken, S., & Bargh, J. (1994). Accuracy motivation attenuates covert priming: The systematic reprocessing of social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 474–489. doi:10.​1037/​0022-3514.​66.​3.​474.CrossRef
go back to reference Wilson, E. O. (1998). Conscilience: The unity of knowledge. New York: Vintage. Wilson, E. O. (1998). Conscilience: The unity of knowledge. New York: Vintage.
go back to reference Wittman, D. (1995). The myth of democratic failure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Wittman, D. (1995). The myth of democratic failure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Wyer, R. S., Jr, & Srull, T. K. (1980). The processing of social stimulus information: A conceptual integration. In R. Hastie, T. Ostrom, E. Ebbesen, R. Wyer, D. Hamilton, & D. Carlston (Eds.), Person memory: The cognitive basis of social perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wyer, R. S., Jr, & Srull, T. K. (1980). The processing of social stimulus information: A conceptual integration. In R. Hastie, T. Ostrom, E. Ebbesen, R. Wyer, D. Hamilton, & D. Carlston (Eds.), Person memory: The cognitive basis of social perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Wyer, R. S., Jr, & Srull, T. K. (1981). Category accessibility: Some theoretical issues concerning the processing of social stimulus information”. In E. T. Higgins, C. P. Herman, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Social cognition: The Ontario symposium on personality and social psychology (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wyer, R. S., Jr, & Srull, T. K. (1981). Category accessibility: Some theoretical issues concerning the processing of social stimulus information”. In E. T. Higgins, C. P. Herman, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Social cognition: The Ontario symposium on personality and social psychology (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Wyer, R. S., Jr, & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its social context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wyer, R. S., Jr, & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its social context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press. Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Metadata
Title
The Unmet Potential of Interdisciplinary Research: Political Psychological Approaches to Voting and Public Opinion
Authors
James N. Druckman
James H. Kuklinski
Lee Sigelman
Publication date
01-12-2009
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Political Behavior / Issue 4/2009
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-009-9092-2

Other articles of this Issue 4/2009

Political Behavior 4/2009 Go to the issue