Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Political Behavior 4/2009

01-12-2009 | Original Paper

Who Said What? The Effects of Source Cues in Issue Frames

Authors: Todd K. Hartman, Christopher R. Weber

Published in: Political Behavior | Issue 4/2009

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Drawing on previous research concerning the role that source cues play in political information processing, we examine whether an ideological identity match between the source of a framed message and the respondent moderates framing effects. We test our hypotheses in two experiments concerning attitudes toward a proposed rally by the Ku Klux Klan. In Experiment 1 (N = 274), we test our hypothesis in a simple issue framing experiment. We find that framing effects occur for strong identifiers only when there is a match between the ideology of the speaker and respondent. In Experiment 2 (N = 259), we examine whether matched frames resonate equally well when individuals are simultaneously exposed to competing frames. The results from this experiment provide mixed support for our hypotheses. The results from our studies suggest that identity matching is an important factor to consider in future framing research.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
Druckman (2001b) distinguishes between two types of framing: Equivalency frames and expectancy (i.e., issue) frames. The former refers to logically equivalent frames, used, for example, in the progeny of experiments measuring risk-averse and risk-seeking behavior, whereas the latter refers to messages that invoke a subset of considerations, often commonly held values, which affect how citizens consider political issues.
 
2
Two subjects refused to identify their gender, and five subjects did not answer the race or party identification questions.
 
3
We also conducted a series of analyses with the standard 7-point NES ideology item in place of our differenced ideological identification measure and found the same statistically significant 3-way interaction presented in Table 1 (B = −0.78, SE = 0.36, p < 0.05). For subjects in the liberal matching condition, we found a statistically significant slope for frame, B = −0.22, SE = 0.11, p < 0.05; for conservatives this matching effect was even more pronounced, B = −0.43, SE = 0.17, p < 0.01. For strong identifiers in the mismatched conditions, there were no framing effects: A conservative source paired with liberal identifiers, B = 0.04, SE = 0.11, n.s.; a liberal source paired with conservative identifiers, B = 0.08, SE = 0.16, n.s.
 
4
Not surprisingly, we also find a main effect for all non-white subjects, such that they were less supportive (and tolerant) of the KKK rally.
 
5
This 3-way interaction holds when we use the 7-point NES ideology item as a measure of ideological identification: frame × source × identification, B = −5.04, SE = 1.99, p < 0.01).
 
6
According to Baron and Kenny (1986), mediated-moderation is tested as follows: (a) The dependent variable must be significantly predicted by the independent variable in the absence of the mediator variable, (b) the mediator must be significantly predicted by the independent variable, and (c) in an equation with both the mediator and independent variable, the independent variable should have no effect and/or be significantly reduced, while the mediator should significantly predict the dependent variable.
 
7
This 3-way interaction satisfies Baron and Kenny’s (1986) first criterion of mediated moderation.
 
8
Respondents indicated the importance of each value on a 7-point scale, which was recoded from 0 to 1, where high scores equal greater importance.
 
9
These models tests Baron and Kenny’s (1986) second criterion of mediated-moderation.
 
10
The fully saturated models test Baron and Kenny’s (1986) third criterion of mediated-moderation.
 
11
We find no differences when we substitute the 7-point NES ideology item as a measure of ideological identification for the frame-source × identification interaction, B = 0.16, SE = 0.18, n.s. Likewise we report null results for the ordered logit using the substituted measure of ideological identification, B = 0.47, SE = 0.97, n.s.
 
12
L = Liberal; C = Conservative; FS = Free Speech; PO = Public Order.
 
13
Our findings are somewhat at odds with rational updating of partisan attachments (Gerber and Green 1998). Our experiments suggest that participants do not adjust their identities from the position taken by liberals and conservatives; rather, participants adjust their positions depending on the position taken by liberals and conservatives. In short, our findings suggest that tolerance attitudes follow from identification, not the other way around.
 
Literature
go back to reference Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc. Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.
go back to reference Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173.CrossRef Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. doi:10.​1037/​0022-3514.​51.​6.​1173.CrossRef
go back to reference Brambor, T., Clark, W. R., & Golder, M. (2006). Understanding interaction models: Improving empirical analyses. Political Analysis, 14, 63–82. Brambor, T., Clark, W. R., & Golder, M. (2006). Understanding interaction models: Improving empirical analyses. Political Analysis, 14, 63–82.
go back to reference Cialdini, R. (1984). Influence: Science and practice. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Cialdini, R. (1984). Influence: Science and practice. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.
go back to reference Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1981). The origins and meaning of liberal/conservative self-identifications. American Journal of Political Science, 25, 617–645. doi:10.2307/2110756.CrossRef Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1981). The origins and meaning of liberal/conservative self-identifications. American Journal of Political Science, 25, 617–645. doi:10.​2307/​2110756.CrossRef
go back to reference Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1984). Group identification, values, and the nature of political beliefs. American Politics Research, 12, 151–175. Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1984). Group identification, values, and the nature of political beliefs. American Politics Research, 12, 151–175.
go back to reference Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press. Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press.
go back to reference Druckman, J. N. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (ir)relevance of framing effects. The American Political Science Review, 98, 671–686. doi:10.1017/S0003055404041413. Druckman, J. N. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (ir)relevance of framing effects. The American Political Science Review, 98, 671–686. doi:10.​1017/​S000305540404141​3.
go back to reference Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.
go back to reference Feldman, S., & Weber, C. R. (2008, July). Understanding social conservatism: Values, threats, and framing issue preferences. Paper Presented at the annual meeting of the International Society of Political Psychology. Feldman, S., & Weber, C. R. (2008, July). Understanding social conservatism: Values, threats, and framing issue preferences. Paper Presented at the annual meeting of the International Society of Political Psychology.
go back to reference French, J. R. R., Jr., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 150–167). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan. French, J. R. R., Jr., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 150–167). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.
go back to reference Gamson, W. A. (1992). Talking politics. New York: Cambridge University Press. Gamson, W. A. (1992). Talking politics. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Gamson, W. A., & Modigliani, A. (1987). The changing culture of affirmative action. Research in Political Sociology, 3, 137–177. Gamson, W. A., & Modigliani, A. (1987). The changing culture of affirmative action. Research in Political Sociology, 3, 137–177.
go back to reference Gerber, A., & Green, D. P. (1998). Rational learning and partisan attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 42, 794–818.CrossRef Gerber, A., & Green, D. P. (1998). Rational learning and partisan attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 42, 794–818.CrossRef
go back to reference Hooghe, L., & Marks, G. (2004). Does identity or economic rationality drive public opinion on European integration. PS: Political Science and Politics, 37, 415–420. Hooghe, L., & Marks, G. (2004). Does identity or economic rationality drive public opinion on European integration. PS: Political Science and Politics, 37, 415–420.
go back to reference Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible?: How television frames political issues. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible?: How television frames political issues. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). New that matters. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). New that matters. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Jaccard, J. (2001). Interaction effects in logistic regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. Jaccard, J. (2001). Interaction effects in logistic regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
go back to reference Jaccard, J., & Turrisi, R. (2003). Interaction effects in multiple regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. Jaccard, J., & Turrisi, R. (2003). Interaction effects in multiple regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
go back to reference Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Lazarsfeld, P. F., Berelson, B., & Gaudet, H. (1944). The people’s choice; how the voter makes up his mind in a presidential campaign. New York: Duell. Lazarsfeld, P. F., Berelson, B., & Gaudet, H. (1944). The people’s choice; how the voter makes up his mind in a presidential campaign. New York: Duell.
go back to reference McClosky, H., & Zaller, J. (1984). The American ethos: Public attitudes toward capitalism and democracy. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. McClosky, H., & Zaller, J. (1984). The American ethos: Public attitudes toward capitalism and democracy. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference McGuire, W. J. (1969). The nature of attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 136–314). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. McGuire, W. J. (1969). The nature of attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 136–314). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
go back to reference Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. New York: Harper & Row. Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. New York: Harper & Row.
go back to reference Mondak, J. J. (1993). Source cues and policy approval: The cognitive dynamics of public support for the Reagan agenda. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 186–212. Mondak, J. J. (1993). Source cues and policy approval: The cognitive dynamics of public support for the Reagan agenda. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 186–212.
go back to reference Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing effects of a civil liberties conflict and its effect on tolerance. The American Political Science Review, 91, 567–583. doi:10.2307/2952075.CrossRef Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing effects of a civil liberties conflict and its effect on tolerance. The American Political Science Review, 91, 567–583. doi:10.​2307/​2952075.CrossRef
go back to reference Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys: Experiments on question form, wording, and context. New York: Academic Press. Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys: Experiments on question form, wording, and context. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Shamir, M., & Arian, A. (1999). Collective identity and electoral competition in Israel. The American Political Science Review, 93, 265–277. doi:10.2307/2585395.CrossRef Shamir, M., & Arian, A. (1999). Collective identity and electoral competition in Israel. The American Political Science Review, 93, 265–277. doi:10.​2307/​2585395.CrossRef
go back to reference Slothuus, R. (2008). More than weighting cognitive importance: A dual process model of issue framing effects. Political Psychology, 29, 1–28. Slothuus, R. (2008). More than weighting cognitive importance: A dual process model of issue framing effects. Political Psychology, 29, 1–28.
go back to reference Sniderman, P. M., & Theriault, S. M. (2004). The structure of political argument and the logic of issue framing. In W. Saris & P. Sniderman (Eds.), Studies in public opinion: Attitudes, nonattitudes, measurement error, and change. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Sniderman, P. M., & Theriault, S. M. (2004). The structure of political argument and the logic of issue framing. In W. Saris & P. Sniderman (Eds.), Studies in public opinion: Attitudes, nonattitudes, measurement error, and change. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
go back to reference Tate, K. (2003). Black faces in the mirror: African Americans and their representatives in the US Congress. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Tate, K. (2003). Black faces in the mirror: African Americans and their representatives in the US Congress. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
go back to reference Tomz, M., & Sniderman, P. M. (n.d.). Brand names and the organization of mass belief systems. Unpublished manuscript. Tomz, M., & Sniderman, P. M. (n.d.). Brand names and the organization of mass belief systems. Unpublished manuscript.
go back to reference Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press. Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Zaller, J. R., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response: Answering questions versus revealing preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 579–616. doi:10.2307/2111583.CrossRef Zaller, J. R., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response: Answering questions versus revealing preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 579–616. doi:10.​2307/​2111583.CrossRef
go back to reference Zimbardo, P. (2007). The Lucifer effect: Understanding how good people turn evil. New York: Random House. Zimbardo, P. (2007). The Lucifer effect: Understanding how good people turn evil. New York: Random House.
Metadata
Title
Who Said What? The Effects of Source Cues in Issue Frames
Authors
Todd K. Hartman
Christopher R. Weber
Publication date
01-12-2009
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Political Behavior / Issue 4/2009
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-009-9088-y

Other articles of this Issue 4/2009

Political Behavior 4/2009 Go to the issue