Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Minds and Machines 1/2018

03-05-2017

A Theory of Resonance: Towards an Ecological Cognitive Architecture

Author: Vicente Raja

Published in: Minds and Machines | Issue 1/2018

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

This paper presents a blueprint for an ecological cognitive architecture. Ecological psychology, I contend, must be complemented with a story about the role of the CNS in perception, action, and cognition. To arrive at such a story while staying true to the tenets of ecological psychology, it will be necessary to flesh out the central metaphor according to which the animal perceives its environment by ‘resonating’ to information in energy patterns: what is needed is a theory of resonance. I offer here the two main elements of such a theory: a framework (Anderson’s neural reuse) and a methodology (multi-scale fractal DST).

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 390 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe




 

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Footnotes
1
It is important to note that I use the wording “computational approach” or “computationalism” in the broadest possible sense. This means that by these terms I refer both to classic computational approaches (Fodor 1975) and to new computational-like ones (e.g., Clark 2015). If it is more comfortable for the reader, “computational approach” may be understood as “information-processing approach”.
 
2
See Chemero (2009) for a contemporary account of ecological psychology.
 
3
The explanation in ecological terms is, of course, more technical and complicated and involves the ecological variable tau, for example. I left these details out of the text to avoid technicalities that do not add too much to its purpose. See Todd (1981) for a deeper study of those technicalities.
 
4
Gibson (1966) famously introduced the concept of perceptual system to capture the role of many parts of organisms and their very action as constitutive elements of perceptual processes. The visual system, for example, is constituted by the CNS, but also by the peripheral nervous system, and the eyes placed in a movable head, which at the same time is placed in a movable body, etc. All these elements are relevant to explain visual perception, and a theory of resonance based just on the CNS-environment interactions will be inadequate for grasping its complexity.
 
5
Both Gibson’s main books (1966, 1979) and classic texts on ecological psychology (Turvey et al. 1981; Michaels and Carello 1981; Lombardo 1987; Reed 1997) offer arguments against computation. Such arguments may be also found in the contemporary ecological psychology literature (e.g., Richardson et al. 2008; Chemero 2009; Michaels and Palatinus 2014).
 
6
Other reasons such as the necessity of a homunculus (Turvey et al. 1981, 1982) or the appeal to loans of intelligence (Dennett 1978; Kugler and Turvey 1987) to any computational theory to work have been described in the literature as well. I will not review them here due to the lack of space.
 
7
See Chomsky (1980) or Fodor (1981) for the argument. See Michaels and Carello (1981), for a critique from ecological psychology.
 
8
In his influential textbook Cognitive Psychology (1967), Neisser claims: “These patterns of light at the retina are… one-sided in their perspective, shifting radically several times each second, unique and novel every moment… bear little resemblance to either the real object that gave rise to them or to the object of experience that the perceiver will construct… Visual cognition, then, deals with the process by which a perceived, remembered, and thought-about world is brought into being from as unpromising a beginning as the retinal patterns.” (pp. 7–8).
 
9
Marr (1982), for instance, famously offered a mechanism for the construction of a 3D image from a 2D one, going through an intermediate step known as 2D1/2.
 
10
Chemero’s example can be further reinforced. In the classical interpretation of the visual event, perception happens at the end of the inner computational chain: we perceive the representation of the of outer object, and such a representation is at the last link of the chain. According to most ecological psychologists, perception is the whole chain. Perception is a state of the whole animal-environment system as a physical system. The inner end of the chain is not special in any regard. This is another way in which the idea of computation is incompatible with the main tenets of ecological psychology.
 
11
I will not deny the possibility of finding a way to interpret Anderson’s proposals in After Phrenology as computational. However, I think there are unambiguous claims explicitly pointing to anti-computationalism, such as: “[I]t is worth an initial if brief reflection on an important disanalogy between the brain and a computer: whereas a computer is typically understood as a device that carries out a specific instruction set on (and in response to) inputs, brain responses to stimuli are characterized instead by specific deviations from intrinsic dynamics.” (Anderson 2014, p. xx). Or: “My current approach to this problem… is to quantify the functional properties of neural assemblies in a multidimensional manner, in terms of their tendency to respond across a range of circumstances—that is, in terms of their dispositional tendencies—rather than trying to characterize their activities in terms of computational or information-processing operations.” (Anderson 2014, p. xxii–xxii).
 
12
Interestingly enough, it is possible that Gibson also had in mind an idea similar to neural reuse for the functional organization of the brain. In his Gibson biography, Reed (1988) refers and quotes some unpublished works in the Gibson archive at Cornell University. He writes: “Considering the capacity of the nervous system to adjust to stimulation in many different ways, Gibson hypothesized that ‘a given set of neurons is equipotential for various different functions in perception and behavior. The same neuron may be excited for different uses at different times. [Therefore] neurons, nerves, and parts of the brain have a vicarious function. A nerve cell is not the same unit in a different combination of nerve cells.’ (Ibid.)” (Reed 1988, p. 224; emphasis is mine). The similarity with neural reuse is, again, astonishing and reinforces the parallelism between both theories I am defending here.
 
13
This aspect is crucial regarding NSF’s grand challenges for the sciences of the mind. More on this in the last section.
 
14
This is the collective variable of the HKB model, one of the most famous instantiations of the explanation of a behavior by the appeal to an ecological variable (Haken et al. 1985).
 
15
Any kind of comprehensive account of such details is completely out of the scope of this work. However, the NSF’s Tutorial in Contemporary Non-Linear Methods for the Behavioral Sciences (edited by Guy van Orden and Michael Riley) is a good introduction to the field. See: https://​www.​nsf.​gov/​sbe/​bcs/​pac/​nmbs/​nmbs.​jsp.
 
16
A full account of active search is out of the scope of this paper, but the idea is that once the CNS system is described in terms of metastability and self-organized criticality, active search becomes a phenomenon easy to explain.
 
Literature
go back to reference Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organization principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 245–313. Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organization principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 245–313.
go back to reference Anderson, M. L. (2014). After phrenology: Neural reuse and the interactive brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Anderson, M. L. (2014). After phrenology: Neural reuse and the interactive brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Bak, P. (1996). How nature work: The science of self-organized criticality. New York: Copernicus.CrossRefMATH Bak, P. (1996). How nature work: The science of self-organized criticality. New York: Copernicus.CrossRefMATH
go back to reference Beer, R. D. (1995). A dynamical systems perspective on agent-environment Interaction. Artificial Intelligence, 72, 173–215.CrossRef Beer, R. D. (1995). A dynamical systems perspective on agent-environment Interaction. Artificial Intelligence, 72, 173–215.CrossRef
go back to reference Bizzarri, M., Giuliani, A., Cucina, A., D’Anselmi, F., Soto, A. M., & Sonnenschein, C. (2011). Fractal analysis in a systems biology approach to cancer. Seminars in Cancer Biology, 21, 175–182.CrossRef Bizzarri, M., Giuliani, A., Cucina, A., D’Anselmi, F., Soto, A. M., & Sonnenschein, C. (2011). Fractal analysis in a systems biology approach to cancer. Seminars in Cancer Biology, 21, 175–182.CrossRef
go back to reference Calvo, P., & Gomila, T. (2008). Handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach. San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Calvo, P., & Gomila, T. (2008). Handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach. San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
go back to reference Calvo, P., & Symmons, J. (2014). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRef Calvo, P., & Symmons, J. (2014). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell. Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.
go back to reference Clark, A. (2015). Surfing uncertainty. London, UK: Oxford University Press. Clark, A. (2015). Surfing uncertainty. London, UK: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference de Rugy, A., Taga, G., Montagne, G., Buekers, M. J., & Laurent, M. (2002). Perception-action coupling model for human locomotor pointing. Biological Cybernetics, 87, 141–150.CrossRefMATH de Rugy, A., Taga, G., Montagne, G., Buekers, M. J., & Laurent, M. (2002). Perception-action coupling model for human locomotor pointing. Biological Cybernetics, 87, 141–150.CrossRefMATH
go back to reference Dennett, D. I. (1978). Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. Montgomery, VT: Bradford Books. Dennett, D. I. (1978). Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. Montgomery, VT: Bradford Books.
go back to reference Edelman, S. (2008). Computing the mind: How the mind really works. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Edelman, S. (2008). Computing the mind: How the mind really works. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Eliasmith, C. (2013). How to build a brain. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Eliasmith, C. (2013). How to build a brain. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Favela, L. H. (2014). Radical embodied cognitive neuroscience: Addressing “Grand Challenges” of the mind sciences. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 796.CrossRef Favela, L. H. (2014). Radical embodied cognitive neuroscience: Addressing “Grand Challenges” of the mind sciences. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 796.CrossRef
go back to reference Fink, P., Foo, P., & Warren, W. (2009). Catching fly balls in virtual reality: A critical test of the outfielder problem. Journal of Vision, 9(13), 1–8. doi:10.1167/9.13.14 CrossRef Fink, P., Foo, P., & Warren, W. (2009). Catching fly balls in virtual reality: A critical test of the outfielder problem. Journal of Vision, 9(13), 1–8. doi:10.​1167/​9.​13.​14 CrossRef
go back to reference Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Fodor, J. A. (1981). Representations: Philosophical essays on the foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Fodor, J. A. (1981). Representations: Philosophical essays on the foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Gallistel, C. R., & King, A. P. (2009). Memory and the computational brain: Why cognitive science will transform neuroscience. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRef Gallistel, C. R., & King, A. P. (2009). Memory and the computational brain: Why cognitive science will transform neuroscience. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1966). The Senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Miffin. Gibson, J. J. (1966). The Senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Miffin.
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Miffin. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Miffin.
go back to reference Haken, H., Kelso, J. A. S., & Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347–356.MathSciNetCrossRefMATH Haken, H., Kelso, J. A. S., & Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347–356.MathSciNetCrossRefMATH
go back to reference Hutto, D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing enactivism: Basic minds without content. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hutto, D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing enactivism: Basic minds without content. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Ibáñez-Gijón, J., Buekers, M., Morice, A., Rao, G., Mascret, N., Laurin, J., et al. (2016). A scale-based approach to interdisciplinary research and expertise in sports. Journal of Sports Sciences. doi:10.1080/02640414.2016.1164330. Ibáñez-Gijón, J., Buekers, M., Morice, A., Rao, G., Mascret, N., Laurin, J., et al. (2016). A scale-based approach to interdisciplinary research and expertise in sports. Journal of Sports Sciences. doi:10.​1080/​02640414.​2016.​1164330.
go back to reference Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: Intentional behavior as a complex system. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: Intentional behavior as a complex system. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Kelso, J. A. S., & Tognoli, E. (2007). Toward a complementary neuroscience: Metastable coordination dynamics of the brain. In L. I. Perlovsky & R. Kozma (Eds.), Neurodynamics of cognition and consciousness (pp. 39–59). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef Kelso, J. A. S., & Tognoli, E. (2007). Toward a complementary neuroscience: Metastable coordination dynamics of the brain. In L. I. Perlovsky & R. Kozma (Eds.), Neurodynamics of cognition and consciousness (pp. 39–59). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Kiverstein, J., & Miller, M. (2015). The embodied brain: Towards a radical embodied cognitive neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 237.CrossRef Kiverstein, J., & Miller, M. (2015). The embodied brain: Towards a radical embodied cognitive neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 237.CrossRef
go back to reference Kugler, P. N., & Turvey, M. T. (1987). Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kugler, P. N., & Turvey, M. T. (1987). Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Lamarque, C.-H., Ture Savadkooh, A., Etcheverria, E., & Dimitrijevic, Z. (2012). Multi-scale dynamics of two coupled nonsmooth systems. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 22(12), 1250295.MathSciNetCrossRefMATH Lamarque, C.-H., Ture Savadkooh, A., Etcheverria, E., & Dimitrijevic, Z. (2012). Multi-scale dynamics of two coupled nonsmooth systems. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 22(12), 1250295.MathSciNetCrossRefMATH
go back to reference Large, E. W. (2008). Resonating to musical rhythm: Theory and experiment. In S. Grondin (Ed.), The psychology of time. West Yorkshire: Emerald. Large, E. W. (2008). Resonating to musical rhythm: Theory and experiment. In S. Grondin (Ed.), The psychology of time. West Yorkshire: Emerald.
go back to reference Large, E. W., Kim, J. C., Flaig, N. K., Bharucha, J. J., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2016). A neurodynamic account of musical tonality. Music Perception, 33(3), 319–331.CrossRef Large, E. W., Kim, J. C., Flaig, N. K., Bharucha, J. J., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2016). A neurodynamic account of musical tonality. Music Perception, 33(3), 319–331.CrossRef
go back to reference Lee, D. N. (2009). General tau theory: Evolution to date. Special issue: Landmarks in perception. Perception, 38, 837–858.CrossRef Lee, D. N. (2009). General tau theory: Evolution to date. Special issue: Landmarks in perception. Perception, 38, 837–858.CrossRef
go back to reference Lombardo, T. J. (1987). The reciprocity of perceiver and environment. In The evolution of James J. Gibson’s ecological psychology. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Lombardo, T. J. (1987). The reciprocity of perceiver and environment. In The evolution of James J. Gibson’s ecological psychology. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: Freeman. Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: Freeman.
go back to reference McBeath, M. K., Shaffer, D. M., & Kaiser, M. K. (1995). How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls. Science, 268(5210), 569–573.CrossRef McBeath, M. K., Shaffer, D. M., & Kaiser, M. K. (1995). How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls. Science, 268(5210), 569–573.CrossRef
go back to reference Merchant, H., Battaglia-Mayer, A., & Georgopoulos, A. P. (2004). Neural responses during interception of real and apparent circularly moving stimuli in motor cortex and area 7a. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 314–331.CrossRef Merchant, H., Battaglia-Mayer, A., & Georgopoulos, A. P. (2004). Neural responses during interception of real and apparent circularly moving stimuli in motor cortex and area 7a. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 314–331.CrossRef
go back to reference Michaels, C., & Carello, C. (1981). Direct perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Michaels, C., & Carello, C. (1981). Direct perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
go back to reference Michaels, C., & Palatinus, Z. (2014). A ten commandments for ecological psychology. In L. Shapiro (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 19–28). New York, NY: Routledge. Michaels, C., & Palatinus, Z. (2014). A ten commandments for ecological psychology. In L. Shapiro (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 19–28). New York, NY: Routledge.
go back to reference Milkowski, M. (2013). Explaining the computational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Milkowski, M. (2013). Explaining the computational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Moreau, A. L. D., Lorite, G. S., Rodrigues, C. M., Souza, A. A., & Cotta, A. (2009). Fractal Analysis of Xylella fastidiosa Biofilm Formation. Journal of Applied Physics. doi:10.1063/1.13173172. Moreau, A. L. D., Lorite, G. S., Rodrigues, C. M., Souza, A. A., & Cotta, A. (2009). Fractal Analysis of Xylella fastidiosa Biofilm Formation. Journal of Applied Physics. doi:10.​1063/​1.​13173172.
go back to reference Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
go back to reference O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 24, 939–1031.CrossRef O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 24, 939–1031.CrossRef
go back to reference Orekhova, E. V., Stroganova, T. A., & Posikera, I. N. (1999). Theta synchronization during sustained anticipatory attention in infants over the second half of the first year of life. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 32, 151–172.CrossRef Orekhova, E. V., Stroganova, T. A., & Posikera, I. N. (1999). Theta synchronization during sustained anticipatory attention in infants over the second half of the first year of life. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 32, 151–172.CrossRef
go back to reference Ouyang, F.-Y., Zheng, B., & Jiang, X.-F. (2015). Intrinsic multi-scale dynamic behaviors of complex financial systems. PLoS ONE, 10(10), e0139420.CrossRef Ouyang, F.-Y., Zheng, B., & Jiang, X.-F. (2015). Intrinsic multi-scale dynamic behaviors of complex financial systems. PLoS ONE, 10(10), e0139420.CrossRef
go back to reference Port, N. L., Kruse, W., Lee, D., & Georgopoulos, A. P. (2001). Motor cortical activity during interception of moving targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 306–318.CrossRef Port, N. L., Kruse, W., Lee, D., & Georgopoulos, A. P. (2001). Motor cortical activity during interception of moving targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 306–318.CrossRef
go back to reference Raja, V., Biener, Z., & Chemero, A. (2017). From Kepler to Gibson. Ecological Psychology, 29(2), 146–160.CrossRef Raja, V., Biener, Z., & Chemero, A. (2017). From Kepler to Gibson. Ecological Psychology, 29(2), 146–160.CrossRef
go back to reference Reed, E. S. (1988). James J. Gibson and the Psychology of Perception. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Reed, E. S. (1988). James J. Gibson and the Psychology of Perception. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Reed, E. S. (1997). Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Reed, E. S. (1997). Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Richardson, M. J., Shockley, K., Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (2008). Ecological psychology: Six principles for an embodied-embedded approach to behavior. In P. Calvo & T. Gomila (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach (pp. 161–188). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Richardson, M. J., Shockley, K., Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (2008). Ecological psychology: Six principles for an embodied-embedded approach to behavior. In P. Calvo & T. Gomila (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach (pp. 161–188). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
go back to reference Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & The PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing (Vol. 1 & 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & The PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing (Vol. 1 & 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Shaffer, D. M., Krauchunas, S. M., Eddy, M., & McBeath, M. K. (2004). How dogs navigate to catch frisbees. Psychological Science, 15(7), 437–441.CrossRef Shaffer, D. M., Krauchunas, S. M., Eddy, M., & McBeath, M. K. (2004). How dogs navigate to catch frisbees. Psychological Science, 15(7), 437–441.CrossRef
go back to reference Shapiro, L. (2014). The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition. New York, NY: Routledge. Shapiro, L. (2014). The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition. New York, NY: Routledge.
go back to reference Taga, G. (1998). A model of the neuro-musculo-skeletal system for anticipatory adjustment of human locomotion during obstacle avoidance. Biological Cybernetics, 78, 9–17. Taga, G. (1998). A model of the neuro-musculo-skeletal system for anticipatory adjustment of human locomotion during obstacle avoidance. Biological Cybernetics, 78, 9–17.
go back to reference Todd, J. T. (1981). Visual information about moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(4), 795–810. Todd, J. T. (1981). Visual information about moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(4), 795–810.
go back to reference Tognoli, E., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2014). The metastable brain. Neuron, 81, 35–48.CrossRef Tognoli, E., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2014). The metastable brain. Neuron, 81, 35–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Tuller, B., Fitch, H. L., & Turvey, M. T. (1982). The Bernstein perspective: II. The concept of muscle linkage or coordinative structure. In J. A. S. Kelso (Ed.), Understanding human motor control. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Tuller, B., Fitch, H. L., & Turvey, M. T. (1982). The Bernstein perspective: II. The concept of muscle linkage or coordinative structure. In J. A. S. Kelso (Ed.), Understanding human motor control. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
go back to reference Turvey, M. T. (1992). Ecological foundations of cognition: Invariants of perception and action. In H. L. Herbert, P. W. van der Broek, & D. C. Knill (Eds.), Cognition: conceptual and methodological issues (pp. 85–117). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.CrossRef Turvey, M. T. (1992). Ecological foundations of cognition: Invariants of perception and action. In H. L. Herbert, P. W. van der Broek, & D. C. Knill (Eds.), Cognition: conceptual and methodological issues (pp. 85–117). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.CrossRef
go back to reference Turvey, M. T., Fitch, H. L., & Tuller, B. (1982). The Bernstein perspective: I. The problems of degrees of freedom and context-conditioned variability. In J. A. S. Kelso (Ed.), Understanding human motor control. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Turvey, M. T., Fitch, H. L., & Tuller, B. (1982). The Bernstein perspective: I. The problems of degrees of freedom and context-conditioned variability. In J. A. S. Kelso (Ed.), Understanding human motor control. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
go back to reference Turvey, M. T., Shaw, R., Reed, E. S., & Mace, W. (1981). Ecological laws for perceiving and acting: A reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn. Cognition, 10, 237–304.CrossRef Turvey, M. T., Shaw, R., Reed, E. S., & Mace, W. (1981). Ecological laws for perceiving and acting: A reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn. Cognition, 10, 237–304.CrossRef
go back to reference van der Weel, F. R., & van der Meer, A. L. H. (2009). Seeing it coming: Infants’ brain responses to looming danger. Naturwissenschaften, 96, 1385–1391.CrossRef van der Weel, F. R., & van der Meer, A. L. H. (2009). Seeing it coming: Infants’ brain responses to looming danger. Naturwissenschaften, 96, 1385–1391.CrossRef
go back to reference Van Orden, G. C., Holden, J. G., & Turvey, M. T. (2003). Self-organization of cognitive performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(3), 331–350.CrossRef Van Orden, G. C., Holden, J. G., & Turvey, M. T. (2003). Self-organization of cognitive performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(3), 331–350.CrossRef
go back to reference Van Orden, G. C., Hollis, G., & Wallot, S. (2012). The blue-collar brain. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 207. Van Orden, G. C., Hollis, G., & Wallot, S. (2012). The blue-collar brain. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 207.
Metadata
Title
A Theory of Resonance: Towards an Ecological Cognitive Architecture
Author
Vicente Raja
Publication date
03-05-2017
Publisher
Springer Netherlands
Published in
Minds and Machines / Issue 1/2018
Print ISSN: 0924-6495
Electronic ISSN: 1572-8641
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-017-9431-8

Other articles of this Issue 1/2018

Minds and Machines 1/2018 Go to the issue

Premium Partner