Skip to main content
Erschienen in: EPJ Data Science 1/2017

Open Access 01.12.2017 | Regular article

Absence makes the heart grow fonder: social compensation when failure to interact risks weakening a relationship

verfasst von: Kunal Bhattacharya, Asim Ghosh, Daniel Monsivais, Robin Dunbar, Kimmo Kaski

Erschienen in: EPJ Data Science | Ausgabe 1/2017

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
download
DOWNLOAD
print
DRUCKEN
insite
SUCHEN
loading …

Abstract

Social networks require active relationship maintenance if they are to be kept at a constant level of emotional closeness. For primates, including humans, failure to interact leads inexorably to a decline in relationship quality, and a consequent loss of the benefits that derive from individual relationships. As a result, many social species compensate for weakened relationships by investing more heavily in them. Here we study how humans behave in similar situations, using data from mobile call detail records from a European country. For the less frequent contacts between pairs of communicating individuals we observe a logarithmic dependence of the duration of the succeeding call on the time gap with the previous call. We find that such behaviour is likely when the individuals in these dyadic pairs have the same gender and are in the same age bracket as well as being geographically distant. Our results indicate that these pairs deliberately invest more time in communication so as to reinforce their social bonding and prevent their relationships decaying when these are threatened by lack of interaction.
Hinweise

Electronic Supplementary Material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1140/​epjds/​s13688-016-0097-x) contains supplementary material.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

KB, AG and DM carried out the analysis of the data. All the authors were involved in designing the project and the preparation of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

1 Introduction

For intensely social species like primates, close friendships buffer the individual against the stresses of living in enforced proximity to others, and so make living in social groups possible [1, 2]. In both primates [35] and humans [69], the number and quality of a female’s social relationships has a direct effect on the stress she suffers, the illness she and her offspring experience, the number of offspring she has and even her survival (see also [10]). However, in both anthropoid primates and humans, such relationships depend on frequent interaction, often at quite specific rates, to maintain relationship quality [11, 12]. In humans at least, interacting at less than the specified frequency results in the rapid decline in the emotional quality of a relationship [13], and hence the relationship’s effectiveness as a buffer. Primates, and humans, thus invest considerable quantities of time in maintaining relationship quality. Perhaps because relationships are so important, animals seem to be sensitive to fact that relationship quality may be adversely affected by circumstances. Baboon mothers, for example, are obliged by the foraging demands of lactation to reduce time invested in grooming their main social partners; they later exhibit a rebound effect in which they groom these individuals more than usual once the infant has started to wean and requires less of her time to be devoted to feeding [14, 15]. Similarly, greetings are often more intense when animals have been separated for some considerable time. Elephant greetings, for example, are more elaborate following a prolonged absence [16], while among bonobos joiners who had been separate from residents longer receive the most socio-sexual solicitations [17]. Similar findings have been noted following conflicts: in both primates [18, 19] and hyenas [20], greeting behaviour is more intense when the past interactions between individuals have been aggressive. In sum, it seems that it takes more effort to repair damaged ties than to maintain existing stable ties. Humans are no less susceptible to the risks imposed by the fragility of close relationships, and may be expected to adopt similar strategies to manage their relationships. However, the inelasticity of time invariably restricts the effort an individual can invest such that only a certain number of relationships would be stable at different levels of emotional intensity [2123]. Hence, while humans obviously maintain large and complex social networks [21, 24], constraints of cognition and time play a vital role in shaping how well their social interactions function [2528]. If investment effort is insufficient, social ties will inexorably decay [29, 30]. Even though the advent of new technologies has introduced a number of new communication channels, the maintenance of relationships still requires investment of considerable time and effort [3133]. Nonetheless, these new technologies have one key advantage, namely allowing individuals to interact and service a relationship when they are geographically far apart.
In this paper, we use a large dataset from a mobile phone service provider in a European country, spanning over seven consecutive months in the year 2007, to investigate whether humans adjust their investment effort to compensate for reduced contact. To do this, we first examine whether the duration of calls is related to the time since last contact, and then ask whether this is especially true of individuals who are geographically further apart (and hence cannot meet up in person so easily). For a given ego (individual) we study the data on mobile phone communication with the alters (individuals participating in calls with the ego) with whom the communication events are sufficiently spaced in time yet showing a degree of regularity. In the offline world, individuals who are contacted at least once a month fall into the category of close, as opposed to intimate, friends, and constitute the second most important group of network alters [22, 26]. As such they represent a stable group of alters that are emotionally important to ego, but ones that are not contacted so frequently as to obliterate any trace of the effect we are interested in. We expect that the investment by the ego in actively maintaining such ties would be reflected as a relationship between the inter-event times and the call durations.

2 Results

For a given ego-alter pair we measure the inter-event time (‘gap’) between two successive calls in number of days irrespective of the directionality of the calls. Then we examine the variation in the duration of the succeeding call (in seconds) as a function of the length of the inter-event time. For analytical convenience, instead of considering individual calls we aggregate all calls in a given day and ignore the days in which the total calling time is less than 10 seconds. Thus by mentioning ‘call’ or an ‘event’ with respect to a given pair on a particular day we refer to the aggregated voice communication on that day. For a given ego i and its alter j, we construct the set of ordered pairs \(\{(\tau_{ij},T_{ij})\}\), such that, \(\tau_{ij}\) is a gap between two calls and \(T_{ij}\) is the duration of the succeeding call. For a given pair ij, we define \(\langle\tau \rangle_{ij}\) and \(\langle T\rangle_{ij}\) as the average gap and the average duration of calls, respectively.
We concentrate on the ego-alter pairs for which the communication is sufficiently spaced over time by considering the set of pairs, \(\mathcal {S}\), for which the maximum number of calls in any calendar month does not exceed 4 and there is at least one call in each of the 7 months. The bound on the maximum number of calls results in a characteristic gap of just over a week. Higher values of this bound would allow for more calls per week and would result in the inclusion of the more frequently contacted alters for a given ego [34] and for such alters the probability of finding large gaps in communication would be comparatively less (illustrated in Figure 1). At least one call per month allows us to focus on relationships which may be considered otherwise stable. Additionally, we consider only those pairs for which the distance between their most common location (\(d_{ij}\)) is greater than zero to reduce the likelihood of face-to-face interaction. The detailed criteria for selecting pairs is provided in the Materials and methods and the robustness of the results is discussed in Additional file 1.
First we show the probability distribution of the gaps and the call durations corresponding to pairs belonging to the set \(\mathcal{S}\). For comparison, we construct another set \(\mathcal{U}\) by relaxing the restriction on the maximum number of calls per month (other parameters being the same as for \(\mathcal{S}\)) to include the more frequently contacted alters, such that \(\mathcal{U}\supseteq\mathcal{S}\). We term \(\mathcal{S}\) as the set of ‘regular contacts’ and \(\mathcal{U}\) as the set of ‘all frequent contacts’. In Figure 1 we plot the probability distribution functions (PDFs) of \(\tau_{ij}\) and \(T_{ij}\) for the pairs in \(\mathcal{S}\) and \(\mathcal{U}\), irrespective of the age and gender of the individuals. In general, the PDFs for \(\tau_{ij}\) and \(T_{ij}\) are fat tailed. The PDF for \(\tau_{ij}\) shows peaks at multiples of seven days, which indicates a high propensity to make calls during weekends. The PDFs for the averages for individual pairs (\(\langle\tau\rangle_{ij}\) and \(\langle T\rangle_{ij}\)) show well defined peaks. Figure 1(a) and (b) shows that a typical average separation and a typical average call duration for alters in \(\mathcal{S}\) is around 12 days and 170 seconds, respectively. The PDF for the average gap in \(\mathcal{U}\) falls off exponentially and the typical average duration in \(\mathcal{U}\) is around 130 seconds.
In Figure 2 we plot the binned curves for the duration of the succeeding calls as a function of the gaps corresponding to communication between egos and the alters in the set \(\mathcal{S}\). Each curve corresponds to an age and sex cohort for the ego and the sex cohort for the alter. The curves indicate a logarithmic increase in the duration of calls with the increase in the gap. Although, the behaviour is found across all the cohorts considered, within the ego-age range of 25-60 year olds the trend appears to be particularly well defined. The cohorts within this range are shown in Figure 2. We use linear regression to fit the following: \(T_{ij}=\beta\log\tau _{ij}+\alpha\) to the data. The larger the β, the stronger is the dependence. The coefficient α provides a basal value for the duration of the calls. In Figure 3(a) we provide the values of β for the different ages and genders (filled symbols). Overall, the effect is strongest in the age range of 25-40 year olds and for same-sex pairs.
The dependence of \(T_{ij}\) on \(\tau_{ij}\) as reflected in Figure 2 results from accumulating the data from multiple sequences belonging to different ego-alter pairs. However, different pairs are expected to have their own idiosyncrasies, and as evident from Figure 1, the averages (gap and call duration) corresponding to different pairs follow unimodal distributions. Therefore, we first analyze the extent to which the properties of different pairs influence this dependence. For a set \(\{(\tau_{ij},T_{ij})\}\) belonging to a given pair ij, we construct an ensemble of artificial sets \(\{(\tau _{ij},T'_{ij})\}\), where, the \(T'_{ij}\)’s are obtained by randomly shuffling the original sequence of \(T_{ij}\)’s. In Figure 4(a) we illustrate the behaviour of the artificial data (black circles) for a particular case. The manufactured durations show a much weaker increase when compared to the original (red squares). We show the β’s resulting from the regression on the randomized data in Figure 3(a) (unfilled symbols). In general, the slopes for the randomized data are much lower, although different from zero. This comparison suggests that the correlations are truly present in the real data.
To extract the behaviour in a form that is independent of the characteristics of the ego-alter pairs, we scale the variables for a given pair with their corresponding averages. The dependence of the scaled variable \(T_{ij}/\langle T\rangle_{ij}\) on \(\tau_{ij}/\langle \tau\rangle_{ij}\) is shown in Figure 4(b) (red squares). The fact that the scaling extracts the correct nature of the correlations is evidenced when we scale the randomized data. The resulting curve (black circles) is flat and shows the absence of any correlation when the data are randomized. We employ a regression of the form: \(T_{ij}/\langle T\rangle_{ij}=\beta'\log (\tau_{ij}/\langle\tau \rangle_{ij} )+\alpha'\) for the scaled data. In Figure 3(b) we plot the slopes \(\beta'\). The figure clearly illustrates that the relationship between the scaled variables is qualitatively the same as that of the unscaled variables. Also, the scaled variables exhibit clear correlations, whereas the slopes for the randomized data are not different from zero. The relationship shows that for a given pair when the length of the gap is larger than the average gap, the duration of the successive call is larger than the duration of the average call. Conversely, if the gap is less than the average, then the duration also falls below the average.
To closely examine the nature of the ties we construct the distribution of alter age-ego age for the pairs in \(\mathcal{S}\). The distributions (Additional file 1) show that \(\mathcal{S}\) is predominantly constituted by alters having the same sex as that of the ego and falling in the same age cohort. In general, the above pattern holds up to the age of 50 year olds. For egos aged above 50, peaks appears at an age separated by one generation. The preferred alters (those called most often) are mainly age peers. Because these dyads were in different geographical locations, they are unlikely to be spouses (indeed, most are same sex peers) and are more likely to be either friends, siblings or distant similar age kin (e.g. cousins). Above the age of 50 years, the double peak suggests that, in addition to peers, egos invest heavily in alters that are about a generation younger, most likely either children or nephews/nieces.
We further categorized the pairs in the set \(\mathcal{S}\) based on the distance and the frequency of communication. First we divide the set into two groups, one consisting of pairs with distances (\(d_{ij}\)) smaller than \(d_{c}\) (‘close’) and the other with distances larger than \(d_{c}\) (‘distant’). We consider the average gap as a proxy for the frequency of calling. Therefore, we again split each of these groups into two subsets based on whether the average gap (\(\langle\tau\rangle _{ij}\)) is less than \(\tau_{c}\) (‘frequent’) or greater than \(\tau_{c}\) (‘infrequent’). We choose \(d_{c}=50~\mbox{km}\) which is larger than the spatial extension of the largest cities in the concerned country. Note that, in general, distance to alters is distributed according to an inverse power law [35]. Also, age and gender preferences of egos for their alters have been found to correlate with their geographic proximity [36]. We choose \(\tau_{c}=12\) days, which is the most probable value of \(\langle\tau\rangle_{ij}\) as can be seen in Figure 1. (See Additional file 1 for the joint PDF of average gap and distance).
In Figure 5 we provide the values of the coefficients α, β and \(\beta'\) for the fits to the data with the categorization as described in the previous paragraph. We obtain the coefficients with the data being further classified according to the gender of the individuals forming the pair. (See Additional file 1 for the coefficients when pairs are analyzed irrespective of the gender.) The plot clearly indicates an increase in α with the increase in distance. However, variation with average gap is not significant. The fact that females are involved in longer calls than the males is also evident. For the β’s we observe a variation with distance very similar to α. However, there is a marginal dependence on the average gap. These facts suggest that the reinforcement effect is stronger when the calling frequency is low and the distance of separation is large. The values of \(\beta'\) reflect the fact that the observation regarding β’s persist when the data is scaled. It also shows a strong gender homophily as the \(\beta'\)’s for same gender pairs appear to be larger compared to mixed gender pairs. This observation is consistent with the results shown in Figure 3(b).

3 Discussion

Our main focus in the study has been with whether individuals adjust the time investment they make in relationships that matter to them when these are at risk. Our index of being ‘at risk’ was a greater than the average gap between contacts (in this case phone calls). Friendships require constant time investment for their maintenance, and failure to match quite specific investment schedules leads inexorably to a rapid reduction in relationship quality [12, 22, 26]. Our findings demonstrate a logarithmic increase in call duration with an increase in inter-call gap between a pair of individuals. In addition, we found that there is marked gender homophily in who calls who (and especially so in respect of same-age peers) and showed that the dependence of call duration on gap is stronger for (1) younger individuals (and especially so in 30-year-olds) than for older people, (2) individuals separated by larger distances, (3) same-sex pairs, and (4) individual for whom the average inter-call gap is larger (marginal effect). Given that the frequency of contact determines the quality of a relationship [22, 26], these results suggest that when individuals fail to contact each other frequently enough they compensate by devoting more time to the next call. This trade-off suggests that time acts as a form of social capital, and needs to be allocated to one’s alters carefully so as to maintain relationships that are considered to be important.
If we assume the complete or partial lack of face-to-face contact in such relationships (because dyads were in different geographical locations), then the lack of opportunity for face-to-face communication would naturally make it difficult to maintain relationship quality over time. The increase in the duration of the succeeding call after a long gap (time gap with the previous call) could thus serve as an act of relationship repair. The nature of the calls can also be inferred from the peaks (in multiples of 7 days) in the distribution of gaps (Figure 1(a)). This suggests that the alters being called are among the set of emotionally close members of ego’s network (somewhere within the inner core of five, normally contacted at least once a week, and the next layer out summing to 15 that are called at least once a month) [22].
These results parallel those noted in a number of animal species [1420], suggesting that where relationships provide valuable benefits that influence fitness, individuals may be anxious to ensure their stability and future persistence. The fact that animals respond in this way might be taken as prima facie evidence for the ability to foresee the consequences of behaviour or events. That humans can do this is, of course, no surprise. However, it does underscore the importance attached to social relationships, suggesting that when these are perceived as likely to become degraded through, for example, lack of opportunity to interact, we make special efforts to reinforce them. We imagine that this is true only for those core relationships within the inner layers of egocentric networks [22] and that such an effect is unlikely to be true of the weaker relationships that populate the outer layers of egocentric networks where interaction rates are already very low [22]. The innermost network layers, typically amounting to just 15 people, are the ones that provide us with our principal sources of support and protection [37].

4 Materials and methods

For this study we use an anonymized dataset containing details of mobile phone communication of subscribers of a particular operator in a European country over a period of 7 consecutive months during the year 2007. The details include full calling histories for the subscribers. Out of all the subscribers, the egos considered for our study are those whose age, gender and the most common location (location of the most accessed cell tower) are known and who are active over the entire period of seven months. We use the following criteria to select ego-alter pairs for whom the communication is sufficiently spaced over time. We consider the pairs for whom the maximum number of calls in any calendar month does not exceed \(c_{\mathrm{max}}=4\). Also, we take into account the number of different months (\(m_{ij}\)) over which a pair participates in calls. To focus on pairs where regularity of communication is observed we use \(m_{ij}=7\). Also, we consider only those pairs for which the distance between their most common locations, \(d_{ij}\) is greater than zero. We also use a minimum threshold for the total calling time in the entire period, \(t_{\mathrm{min}}=30\) minutes. The role of \(t_{\mathrm{min}}\) is to ensure that for a given pair, the relationship would be significantly valued by both as indexed by the invested time. We denote the set of ego-alter pairs chosen by the above criteria (\(c_{\mathrm{max}}=4\), \(m_{ij}=7\), \(d_{ij}>0\) and \(t_{\mathrm{min}}=30\)) as \(\mathcal {S}\). The total number of egos considered is around 400,000. We group the egos (males and females appearing in almost equal ratios) into different age cohorts denoted by 21 (18-24), 27 (25-30), 35 (31-40), 45 (41-50), 55 (50-60) and 65 (60-70) years.
We find that the qualitative nature of our findings is sufficiently robust and is not delicately dependent on the choice of the different thresholds to define \(\mathcal{S}\). On lowering the value of \(t_{\mathrm{min}}\), we observe a decrease in the values of α, β and \(\beta'\) (Figures S1 and S2 in Additional file 1). With the lowering of \(t_{\mathrm{min}}\), the pairs having lower aggregated calling times are also included in the analysis. As a result, there is a decrease in α. However, both β and \(\beta'\) are still found to be significantly different from zero. Also, reducing the number of months when communication is present (\(m_{ij}\)), or allowing more than 4 calls in a month (by increasing \(c_{\mathrm{max}}\)) does not appear to have marked influence on \(\beta'\). But if we replace the condition \(d_{ij}>0\) with \(d_{ij}=0\) (that is, consider only those pairs where the ego and the alter have the same most common location), then the dependence of \(T_{ij}/\langle T\rangle_{ij}\) on \(\tau _{ij}/\langle\tau\rangle_{ij}\) is largely absent (Figure S3 in Additional file 1). The above analysis shows that the dependence, \(T_{ij}\propto\log\tau_{ij}\) should be reflected in the communication between distant alters even when the frequency of communication is less uniform, for example, when \(m_{ij}<7\), or, \(c_{\mathrm{max}}>4\). However, in such cases the average inter-call gap \(\langle\tau\rangle_{ij}\) may not remain a well-defined quantity [38].

Acknowledgements

KB, DM and KK acknowledge support from H2020 EU project IBSEN. AG and KK acknowledge support from project COSDYN, Academy of Finland (Project no. 276439). DM acknowledges CONACYT, Mexico for supporting grant 383907. RD’s research is supported by an ERC Advanced Investigator award.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

KB, AG and DM carried out the analysis of the data. All the authors were involved in designing the project and the preparation of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Schaik CP (1983) Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour 87(1):120-144 CrossRef Van Schaik CP (1983) Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour 87(1):120-144 CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunbar RIM (2012) Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B, Biol Sci 367(1597):1837-1846 CrossRef Dunbar RIM (2012) Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B, Biol Sci 367(1597):1837-1846 CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2003) Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302(5648):1231-1234 CrossRef Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2003) Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302(5648):1231-1234 CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Cameron EZ, Setsaas TH, Linklater WL (2009) Social bonds between unrelated females increase reproductive success in feral horses. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(33):13850-13853 CrossRef Cameron EZ, Setsaas TH, Linklater WL (2009) Social bonds between unrelated females increase reproductive success in feral horses. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(33):13850-13853 CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2010) Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Curr Biol 20(15):1359-1361 CrossRef Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2010) Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Curr Biol 20(15):1359-1361 CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Uchino BN (2006) Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med 29(4):377-387 CrossRef Uchino BN (2006) Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med 29(4):377-387 CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Spence J (1954) One thousand families in Newcastle Spence J (1954) One thousand families in Newcastle
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith KP, Christakis NA (2008) Social networks and health. Annu Rev Sociol 34:405-429 CrossRef Smith KP, Christakis NA (2008) Social networks and health. Annu Rev Sociol 34:405-429 CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Oesch N, Dunbar R (2015) Influence of kin network on maternal and infant health and illness. J Pregnancy Child Health 2(2):146 Oesch N, Dunbar R (2015) Influence of kin network on maternal and infant health and illness. J Pregnancy Child Health 2(2):146
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB (2010) Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS Med 7(7):e1000316 CrossRef Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB (2010) Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS Med 7(7):e1000316 CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Lehmann J, Korstjens A, Dunbar R (2007) Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates. Anim Behav 74(6):1617-1629 CrossRef Lehmann J, Korstjens A, Dunbar R (2007) Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates. Anim Behav 74(6):1617-1629 CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts SB, Dunbar R (2015) Managing relationship decay. Hum Nat 26(4):426-450 CrossRef Roberts SB, Dunbar R (2015) Managing relationship decay. Hum Nat 26(4):426-450 CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Saramäki J, Leicht EA, López E, Roberts SGB, Reed-Tsochas F, Dunbar RIM (2014) Persistence of social signatures in human communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111(3):942-947 CrossRef Saramäki J, Leicht EA, López E, Roberts SGB, Reed-Tsochas F, Dunbar RIM (2014) Persistence of social signatures in human communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111(3):942-947 CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Altmann J (2001) Baboon mothers and infants. University of Chicago Press, Chicago Altmann J (2001) Baboon mothers and infants. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunbar R, Dunbar P (1988) Maternal time budgets of gelada baboons. Anim Behav 36(4):970-980 CrossRef Dunbar R, Dunbar P (1988) Maternal time budgets of gelada baboons. Anim Behav 36(4):970-980 CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Moss C, Poole J (1983) Relationships and social structure of African elephants. In: Primate social relationships: an integrated approach, pp 315-325 Moss C, Poole J (1983) Relationships and social structure of African elephants. In: Primate social relationships: an integrated approach, pp 315-325
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Wittig RM, Boesch C (2005) How to repair relationships-reconciliation in wild chimpanzees (pan troglodytes). Ethology 111(8):736-763 CrossRef Wittig RM, Boesch C (2005) How to repair relationships-reconciliation in wild chimpanzees (pan troglodytes). Ethology 111(8):736-763 CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Moscovice LR, Deschner T, Hohmann G (2015) Welcome back: responses of female bonobos (pan paniscus) to fusions. PLoS ONE 10(5):e0127305 CrossRef Moscovice LR, Deschner T, Hohmann G (2015) Welcome back: responses of female bonobos (pan paniscus) to fusions. PLoS ONE 10(5):e0127305 CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Aureli F, Cords M, Van Schaik CP (2002) Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a predictive framework. Anim Behav 64(3):325-343 CrossRef Aureli F, Cords M, Van Schaik CP (2002) Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a predictive framework. Anim Behav 64(3):325-343 CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith JE, Powning KS, Dawes SE, Estrada JR, Hopper AL, Piotrowski SL, Holekamp KE (2011) Greetings promote cooperation and reinforce social bonds among spotted hyaenas. Anim Behav 81(2):401-415 CrossRef Smith JE, Powning KS, Dawes SE, Estrada JR, Hopper AL, Piotrowski SL, Holekamp KE (2011) Greetings promote cooperation and reinforce social bonds among spotted hyaenas. Anim Behav 81(2):401-415 CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Hill RA, Dunbar RI (2003) Social network size in humans. Hum Nat 14(1):53-72 CrossRef Hill RA, Dunbar RI (2003) Social network size in humans. Hum Nat 14(1):53-72 CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Sutcliffe A, Dunbar R, Binder J, Arrow H (2012) Relationships and the social brain: integrating psychological and evolutionary perspectives. Br J Psychol 103(2):149-168 CrossRef Sutcliffe A, Dunbar R, Binder J, Arrow H (2012) Relationships and the social brain: integrating psychological and evolutionary perspectives. Br J Psychol 103(2):149-168 CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhou W-X, Sornette D, Hill RA, Dunbar RI (2005) Discrete hierarchical organization of social group sizes. Proc R Soc Lond B, Biol Sci 272(1561):439-444 CrossRef Zhou W-X, Sornette D, Hill RA, Dunbar RI (2005) Discrete hierarchical organization of social group sizes. Proc R Soc Lond B, Biol Sci 272(1561):439-444 CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunbar R (2016) Do online social media cut through the constraints that limit the size of offline social networks? R Soc Open Sci 3(1):150292 CrossRefMathSciNet Dunbar R (2016) Do online social media cut through the constraints that limit the size of offline social networks? R Soc Open Sci 3(1):150292 CrossRefMathSciNet
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Nie NH (2001) Sociability, interpersonal relations, and the Internet reconciling conflicting findings. Am Behav Sci 45(3):420-435 CrossRef Nie NH (2001) Sociability, interpersonal relations, and the Internet reconciling conflicting findings. Am Behav Sci 45(3):420-435 CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts SGB (2010) Constraints on social networks. In: Dunbar RIM, Gamble C, Gowlett JAJ (eds) Social brain, distributed mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, chapter 6, pp 117-138 Roberts SGB (2010) Constraints on social networks. In: Dunbar RIM, Gamble C, Gowlett JAJ (eds) Social brain, distributed mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, chapter 6, pp 117-138
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Gonçalves B, Perra N, Vespignani A (2011) Modeling users’ activity on Twitter networks: validation of Dunbar’s number. PLoS ONE 6(8):e22656 CrossRef Gonçalves B, Perra N, Vespignani A (2011) Modeling users’ activity on Twitter networks: validation of Dunbar’s number. PLoS ONE 6(8):e22656 CrossRef
28.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Oswald DL, Clark EM (2003) Best friends forever?: high school best friendships and the transition to college. Pers Relatsh 10(2):187-196 CrossRef Oswald DL, Clark EM (2003) Best friends forever?: high school best friendships and the transition to college. Pers Relatsh 10(2):187-196 CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Cummings J, Lee J, Kraut R (2006) Communication technology and friendship during the transition from high school to college. In: Computers, phones, and the Internet: domesticating information technology, pp 265-278 Cummings J, Lee J, Kraut R (2006) Communication technology and friendship during the transition from high school to college. In: Computers, phones, and the Internet: domesticating information technology, pp 265-278
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Miritello G, Moro E, Lara R, Martínez-López R, Belchamber J, Roberts SGB, Dunbar RIM (2013) Time as a limited resource: communication strategy in mobile phone networks. Soc Netw 35(1):89-95 CrossRef Miritello G, Moro E, Lara R, Martínez-López R, Belchamber J, Roberts SGB, Dunbar RIM (2013) Time as a limited resource: communication strategy in mobile phone networks. Soc Netw 35(1):89-95 CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Palchykov V, Kaski K, Kertész J, Barabási A-L, Dunbar RI (2012) Sex differences in intimate relationships. Sci Rep 2:370 CrossRef Palchykov V, Kaski K, Kertész J, Barabási A-L, Dunbar RI (2012) Sex differences in intimate relationships. Sci Rep 2:370 CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Onnela J-P, Arbesman S, González MC, Barabási A-L, Christakis NA (2011) Geographic constraints on social network groups. PLoS ONE 6(4):e16939. CrossRef Onnela J-P, Arbesman S, González MC, Barabási A-L, Christakis NA (2011) Geographic constraints on social network groups. PLoS ONE 6(4):e16939. CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Jo H-H, Saramäki J, Dunbar RI, Kaski K (2014) Spatial patterns of close relationships across the lifespan. Sci Rep 4:6988 CrossRef Jo H-H, Saramäki J, Dunbar RI, Kaski K (2014) Spatial patterns of close relationships across the lifespan. Sci Rep 4:6988 CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Curry O, Roberts SG, Dunbar RI (2013) Altruism in social networks: evidence for a ‘kinship premium’. Br J Psychol 104(2):283-295. CrossRef Curry O, Roberts SG, Dunbar RI (2013) Altruism in social networks: evidence for a ‘kinship premium’. Br J Psychol 104(2):283-295. CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Jo H-H, Karsai M, Kertész J, Kimmo K (2012) Circadian pattern and burstiness in mobile phone communication. New J Phys 14(1):013055 CrossRef Jo H-H, Karsai M, Kertész J, Kimmo K (2012) Circadian pattern and burstiness in mobile phone communication. New J Phys 14(1):013055 CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Absence makes the heart grow fonder: social compensation when failure to interact risks weakening a relationship
verfasst von
Kunal Bhattacharya
Asim Ghosh
Daniel Monsivais
Robin Dunbar
Kimmo Kaski
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2017
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
EPJ Data Science / Ausgabe 1/2017
Elektronische ISSN: 2193-1127
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1140/epjds/s13688-016-0097-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2017

EPJ Data Science 1/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner