Skip to main content

2019 | OriginalPaper | Buchkapitel

Is Generative Theory Misleading for Music Theory?

verfasst von : Rafael Barbosa

Erschienen in: Language, Music and Computing

Verlag: Springer International Publishing

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

During the decade of the 1960s linguistics entered what can be seen as a paradigm shift following Thomas Kuhn’s theory of the Structure of Scientific Revolutions (1962). As a result, the discipline steps out of the Cartesian dualism between body and mind. During the 80th’s analytical musicology was related to the methodological approach of transformational grammars, the best known example being the Generative Theory of Tonal Music (Lerdahl and Jackendoff 1983). For musicologist, the motivation to adopt this position is naturally nurtured by the work of Heinrich Schenker (Der Freier Satz 1935) in which, as in transformational grammar, a hierarchy of layers going from the actual piece of music to its Ursatz (Kernel) is proposed. The hypothesis developed in this article is that the analytical musicology, despite the efforts to link it with modern linguistics, has not yet stepped into the new scientific paradigm led by cognitive sciences. The reason for this is that musicology has not yet adopted a redefinition of its object of study from a non-dualistic and transdisciplinary perspective. With the development of experimental aesthetics, the ontological gap between the object of musicology and that of the scientific approach to music has been growing larger. As a result, if the study of aesthetic meaning in music has become possible today, it seems to be inconsistent with the traditional reductionist methods of analytical musicology, from which the analogy with transformational grammar rely upon.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 390 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe




 

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
This is also one of the ideas that Kuhn develops extensively on his book. “Men whose research is based on shared paradigms are committed to the same rules and standards for scientific practice. That commitment and the apparent consensus it produces are prerequisites for normal science, i.e., for the genesis and continuation of a particular research tradition.” [4].
 
2
The development of complexity as a methodological approach; a kind of Discours de la méthode for the 20th century, has been the work of thinkers like Morin [9] who elaborates a theory of complex thinking in order to defend a new epistemological definition of anthropology: what he named fundamental anthropology.
 
3
Not only the authors of the gestalt theory like Ehrenfelds and Kurt Kofka used melody to explain the effect of grouping laws in time, but Husserl used it as well on his Lessons on the Phenomenology of Inner Time Consciousness. Studying time perception, psychologist like Paul Fraisse and John Michon have also reflected both on the nature of melody and musical rhythm.
 
4
The Ionien mode is one of the musical scales used in the ancient Greece. This mode is known to be used on festivities related to the tribute of Dionysos. Surprisingly, it corresponds very closely to a major scale, which is the basis of tonal music, as developed in Europe from the baroque era.
 
5
The sophisticated notation of sound parameters like pitch and duration, played an important role on the evolution of musical style. Theoretical concepts of melodic phrase, harmony, counterpoint and basso continuo, are also tightly related to the evolution of notation.
 
6
From antiquity the term harmony use to be applied to the study of astronomy. It is still the case during the 17th when Kepler publishes his Harmonices Mundi (1619). We can see that it is the same sense used by Rameau on this passage.
 
7
Allen Forte and Steven Gilbert are the authors of Introduction to schenkerian analysis, New York, Norton & Co, 1983.
 
8
It is the case of the well known Divje Babe flute found in 1995 in a Slovenian cave by Ivan Turk.
 
9
A rapid evolution of polyphonic music takes place following the adoption of the staff invented by Guido de Arezzo during the first half on the 11th century. The music of Léonin (1150–1201) and Pérotin le Grand (1160–1230), using long melismas on each vowel, shows how the understanding of the sacred text became secondary in comparison with Gregorian plainchant.
 
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Deliège, C.: Fondements de la musique tonale. Lattès, Paris (1984) Deliège, C.: Fondements de la musique tonale. Lattès, Paris (1984)
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Schenker, H.: Free Composition, 2nd edn. (trans: Oster, E.) Longman, New York (1979). (original from 1935) Schenker, H.: Free Composition, 2nd edn. (trans: Oster, E.) Longman, New York (1979). (original from 1935)
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Virués-Ortega, J.: The Case Against B. F. Skinner 45 years Later: An Encounter with N. Chomsky. In: The Behavior Analyst, vol. 29, pp. 243–251 (2006). p. 245CrossRef Virués-Ortega, J.: The Case Against B. F. Skinner 45 years Later: An Encounter with N. Chomsky. In: The Behavior Analyst, vol. 29, pp. 243–251 (2006). p. 245CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuhn, T.: The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, p. 23. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1962) Kuhn, T.: The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, p. 23. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1962)
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Delalande, F.: Analyser la musique, pourquoi, comment?. INA, Paris (2013) Delalande, F.: Analyser la musique, pourquoi, comment?. INA, Paris (2013)
6.
Zurück zum Zitat von Helmholtz, H.: On the Sensation of Tone as Physiological Basis for the Theory of Music. éd. and trad. A. J. Ellis, New York, Dover, New York, p. 6 (1954) von Helmholtz, H.: On the Sensation of Tone as Physiological Basis for the Theory of Music. éd. and trad. A. J. Ellis, New York, Dover, New York, p. 6 (1954)
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Francès, R.: La perception de la musique. Vrin, Paris (1958) Francès, R.: La perception de la musique. Vrin, Paris (1958)
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Berlyne, D. (ed.): Studies in Experimental Aesthetics: A Step Toward an Objective Psychology of Aesthetic Appreciation. Hemisphere Publishing Corporation, Washington (1974) Berlyne, D. (ed.): Studies in Experimental Aesthetics: A Step Toward an Objective Psychology of Aesthetic Appreciation. Hemisphere Publishing Corporation, Washington (1974)
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Morin, E.: Introduction à la pensée complexe. Seuil, Paris (2005) Morin, E.: Introduction à la pensée complexe. Seuil, Paris (2005)
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Rohrmeier, M., Rebuschat, P.: Implicit learning and acquisition of Music. TopiCS 4, 525–553 (2012) Rohrmeier, M., Rebuschat, P.: Implicit learning and acquisition of Music. TopiCS 4, 525–553 (2012)
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Granot, R., Jacoby, N.: Musically Puzzling I: Sensitivity to Overall Structure in the Sonata Form? Musicae Scientiae 15(3), 365–386 (2011)CrossRef Granot, R., Jacoby, N.: Musically Puzzling I: Sensitivity to Overall Structure in the Sonata Form? Musicae Scientiae 15(3), 365–386 (2011)CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Tillmann, B., Bigand, E.: Global context effect in normal and scrambled musical sequences. J. Exp. Psychol. 27(5), 1185–1196 (2001) Tillmann, B., Bigand, E.: Global context effect in normal and scrambled musical sequences. J. Exp. Psychol. 27(5), 1185–1196 (2001)
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Imberty, M., Maya, G. (eds.): Temps, geste et musicalité. L’harmattan, Paris (2007) Imberty, M., Maya, G. (eds.): Temps, geste et musicalité. L’harmattan, Paris (2007)
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Bent, I.: Analysis. Macmillan, London (1987) Bent, I.: Analysis. Macmillan, London (1987)
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Rameau, J.-P.: Démonstration du principe de l’harmonie, p. viii. Durant-Pissot, Paris (1750) Rameau, J.-P.: Démonstration du principe de l’harmonie, p. viii. Durant-Pissot, Paris (1750)
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Boulez, P.: Jalons, Pour une décennie, p. 34. Christian Bourgois, Paris (1989) Boulez, P.: Jalons, Pour une décennie, p. 34. Christian Bourgois, Paris (1989)
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Cook, N.: Music Imagination and Culture, p. 3. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1992) Cook, N.: Music Imagination and Culture, p. 3. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1992)
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Lorenz, K.: Behind the Mirror: A Search for a Natural History of Human Knowledge, p. 49. Methuen, London (1977) Lorenz, K.: Behind the Mirror: A Search for a Natural History of Human Knowledge, p. 49. Methuen, London (1977)
19.
Zurück zum Zitat This assumption is made by Jean-Claude Risset, in the forword to: Auriol, Berdard: La clef des sons. Erès, Toulouse (1991) This assumption is made by Jean-Claude Risset, in the forword to: Auriol, Berdard: La clef des sons. Erès, Toulouse (1991)
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Jackendoff, R., Lerdahl, F.: The capacity for music: what is it, and what’s special about it? In: Cognition, no. 100, pp. 33–72 (2006). p. 55 Jackendoff, R., Lerdahl, F.: The capacity for music: what is it, and what’s special about it? In: Cognition, no. 100, pp. 33–72 (2006). p. 55
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Chomsky, N.: Language and Mind, 3rd edn, p. 25. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2006)CrossRef Chomsky, N.: Language and Mind, 3rd edn, p. 25. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2006)CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Darmesteter, A.: La vie des mots. Delagrave, Paris (1887) Darmesteter, A.: La vie des mots. Delagrave, Paris (1887)
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Reber, A.: Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1993) Reber, A.: Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1993)
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Imberty, M.: L’aquisition de structures tonales chez l’enfant. Klincksieck, Paris (1969) Imberty, M.: L’aquisition de structures tonales chez l’enfant. Klincksieck, Paris (1969)
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Bigand, E., Charles, D.: L’apprentissage implicite de la musique occidentale. In: Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., Peretz, I. (eds.) Musique, Langage, Emotion: une approche neuro-cognitive. Presse Universitaire de Renne, Renne (2010) Bigand, E., Charles, D.: L’apprentissage implicite de la musique occidentale. In: Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., Peretz, I. (eds.) Musique, Langage, Emotion: une approche neuro-cognitive. Presse Universitaire de Renne, Renne (2010)
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Rohrmeier, M., Cross, I.: Tacit tonality: implicit learning of context-free harmonic structures. In: Proceedings of the 7th Trennial Conference of the ESCOM, pp. 443–452 (2009) Rohrmeier, M., Cross, I.: Tacit tonality: implicit learning of context-free harmonic structures. In: Proceedings of the 7th Trennial Conference of the ESCOM, pp. 443–452 (2009)
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Peretz, I.: The nature of music from a biological perspective. In: Elsevier Cognition, no. 100, pp. 1–32 (2006)CrossRef Peretz, I.: The nature of music from a biological perspective. In: Elsevier Cognition, no. 100, pp. 1–32 (2006)CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Bosseur, J.-Y.: Du son au signe. Alternatives, Paris (2005) Bosseur, J.-Y.: Du son au signe. Alternatives, Paris (2005)
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Ball, P.: The Music Instinct, How Music Works and Why We Can’t Do Without It. Vintage, London (2011) Ball, P.: The Music Instinct, How Music Works and Why We Can’t Do Without It. Vintage, London (2011)
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Sloboda, J.: The Musical Mind. The Cognitive Psychology of Music. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1986)CrossRef Sloboda, J.: The Musical Mind. The Cognitive Psychology of Music. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1986)CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Is Generative Theory Misleading for Music Theory?
verfasst von
Rafael Barbosa
Copyright-Jahr
2019
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-05594-3_2

Premium Partner