Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 4/2007

01.12.2007 | Original Paper

The Messenger Overwhelming the Message: Ideological Cues and Perceptions of Bias in Television News

verfasst von: Joel Turner

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 4/2007

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Survey research has demonstrated that citizens perceive ideological bias in television news, specifically with regard to CNN and Fox News Channel (FNC), which allegedly represent the liberal and conservative viewpoint, respectively. In this paper I argue that attaching the CNN and FNC labels to news stories sends an ideological cue to the viewer regarding the content of the story. Utilizing an experimental design that allows manipulation of the network attribution of actual FNC and CNN content, I am able to demonstrate that the CNN and FNC labels function as ideological signals to the viewer, with this signal being most pronounced among ideologues whose views are supposedly at odds with those attributed to the network.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
These surveys were conducted by the American Society of Newspaper Editors (1999), the Gallup Organization (2003), and the Pew Research Center (2005).
 
2
Although Fox News Channel has recently been on top in the competition for ratings, it is conceivable that either network could be in first place with the other in second place, depending on when one examines the ratings. The important point, however, is that both networks outpace their cable news competitors.
 
3
This poll was conducted on behalf of Media Matters for America by the Garin-Hart-Yang Research Group, Washington, D.C., in November 2004.
 
4
Alterman, Eric (2006). Think Again: Rigging the Numbers. Center for American Progress. http://www.americanprogress.org/issues/2006/01/b1347483.html
 
5
This research was conducted in accordance with the guidelines set forth by the Human Subjects Committee of the Office of Research at Florida State University. A detailed explanation of these guidelines can be found at www.research.fsu.edu/humansubjects.
 
6
The State of the News Media 2004 study, conducted by Journalism.org, confirmed that foreign policy stories were a major part of cable news broadcasts when this study was conducted, as this type of story constituted 24% and 21% of the broadcast time on CNN and Fox News Channel, respectively. However, this is not always the case, which raises questions regarding whether these results extend to all periods and forms of news coverage. Within the confines of my data, I can determine whether respondents reacted differently to domestic and foreign news stories. They did not, as the difference of means between foreign and domestic stories originating from CNN and Fox News was only .16 and .15, respectively, and not statistically significant. Therefore, it does not appear that the mix of domestic/foreign stories influences the nature of results one way or the other. However, additional attention is warranted regarding the possibility that perceptions of ideological bias differ for domestic vs. foreign news stories.
 
7
The anchor identified his network affiliation in three different ways. First, at the beginning of the broadcast, the anchor stated: “This is Phil Streetman reporting for CNN/Fox News.” Second, during the stories the anchor would use phrases that conveyed network affiliation without changing the tone of the story, such as “CNN/Fox News has learned...” or “sources have told CNN/Fox News...”. Finally, the anchor signed off using the network taglines: “This is Phil Streetman for CNN, the most trusted name in news” and “This is Phil Streetman for Fox News, where you always get fair and balanced coverage.”
 
8
Individual professors had control over the manner and amount of extra credit that was awarded. As a result, participants were rewarded for their participation at varying degrees depending upon what class they were enrolled in. However, I have no reason to believe that the participants were aware of this fact, and I also do not believe that this had any influence on the opinions they provided in the experiment.
 
9
A reliability analysis was conducted to determine if constructing a summary variable was appropriate. The summary scale was found to reach an acceptable level of reliability (Cronbach's α = 0.81). In addition, item-total correlations range from .405 to .523, which fall within the recommendation of between .30 and .70 put forth by Kerlinger (1986).
 
10
“Anchor appearance” served as a measure of how telegenic the anchor was to the respondent. “Anchor tone of voice” served as a measure of how well the anchor vocally delivered the news. “Anchor level of knowledge” measured how intelligent the respondent thought the anchor was. “Anchor trustworthiness” measured whether the respondent trusted the anchor, and “anchor objectivity” measured whether the respondent thought the anchor was delivering the news in an ideologically neutral manner.
 
11
I ran additional models that included control variables for media use patterns (i.e., network preference, hours per week watching TV news, propensity to read the newspaper, propensity to visit online sites, newspaper and/or website used most frequently) as well as three-way interactions between ideology, content, and network attribution. The variables failed to reach significance and did not make a significant contribution to the model. As a result, they were dropped from the final analysis.
 
12
CNN content was used in the analysis presented in Figure 1. FNC content could have been used, but given the small coefficients and the high levels of insignificance associated with the content variable and the interaction between content and ideology, no significant substantive difference would have emerged by creating the figure using content presented by the other network.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Alterman, E. (2003). What liberal media? The truth about bias and the news. New York: Basic Books. Alterman, E. (2003). What liberal media? The truth about bias and the news. New York: Basic Books.
Zurück zum Zitat Bennett, W. L., & Entman, R. (2000). Mediated politics: Communication in the future of democracy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bennett, W. L., & Entman, R. (2000). Mediated politics: Communication in the future of democracy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Carmines, E. G., & Kuklinski, J. (1990). Incentives, opportunities, and the logic of public opinion in American political representation. In J. A. Ferejohn, & J. H. Kuklinski (Eds.), Information and democratic processes. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Carmines, E. G., & Kuklinski, J. (1990). Incentives, opportunities, and the logic of public opinion in American political representation. In J. A. Ferejohn, & J. H. Kuklinski (Eds.), Information and democratic processes. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in Persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752–766.CrossRef Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in Persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752–766.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of Persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Oslon, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The ontario symposium 5 (pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of Persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Oslon, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The ontario symposium 5 (pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the Persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought. New York: Guilford Press. Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the Persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought. New York: Guilford Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent, (pp. 206–261). Chicago: Free Press of Glencoe. Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent, (pp. 206–261). Chicago: Free Press of Glencoe.
Zurück zum Zitat Dalton, R. J., Beck, P, & Huckfeldt, R. (1998). Partisan cues and the media: Information flows in the 1992 election. American Political Science Review, 92, 111–26.CrossRef Dalton, R. J., Beck, P, & Huckfeldt, R. (1998). Partisan cues and the media: Information flows in the 1992 election. American Political Science Review, 92, 111–26.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Entman, R. M. (1989). How the media affect what people think: An information processing approach. Journal of Politics, 51, 347–370.CrossRef Entman, R. M. (1989). How the media affect what people think: An information processing approach. Journal of Politics, 51, 347–370.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gitlin, T. (2000). Inside prime time. Berkley: University of California Press. Gitlin, T. (2000). Inside prime time. Berkley: University of California Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldberg, B. (2001). Bias: A CBS insider exposes how the media distort the news. Washington, D.C.: Regnery Publishing. Goldberg, B. (2001). Bias: A CBS insider exposes how the media distort the news. Washington, D.C.: Regnery Publishing.
Zurück zum Zitat Graber, D. (1980). Mass media and American politics. Washington, D.C.: Congressional Quarterly Press. Graber, D. (1980). Mass media and American politics. Washington, D.C.: Congressional Quarterly Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Groseclose, T., & Milyo, J. (2005). A measure of media bias. Quarterly Journal of Economics 120, 1191–1237.CrossRef Groseclose, T., & Milyo, J. (2005). A measure of media bias. Quarterly Journal of Economics 120, 1191–1237.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hastorf, A., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A case study. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 129–134.CrossRef Hastorf, A., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A case study. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 129–134.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S., Peters, M. D., & Kinder, D. R. (1982). Experimental demonstrations of the ‘Not-So-Minimal’ consequences of television news programs. American Political Science Review 76, 848–858.CrossRef Iyengar, S., Peters, M. D., & Kinder, D. R. (1982). Experimental demonstrations of the ‘Not-So-Minimal’ consequences of television news programs. American Political Science Review 76, 848–858.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Joslyn, M. R., & Ceccoli, S. (1996). Attentiveness to TV news and opinion change in the fall 1992 election. Political Behavior, 18, 141–170.CrossRef Joslyn, M. R., & Ceccoli, S. (1996). Attentiveness to TV news and opinion change in the fall 1992 election. Political Behavior, 18, 141–170.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kerlinger, F. (1986). Foundations of behavioral research. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Kerlinger, F. (1986). Foundations of behavioral research. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuklinski, J. H., & Hurley, N. L. (1994). On hearing and interpreting political messages: A cautionary tale of citizen cue-taking. Journal of Politics, 56, 729–751.CrossRef Kuklinski, J. H., & Hurley, N. L. (1994). On hearing and interpreting political messages: A cautionary tale of citizen cue-taking. Journal of Politics, 56, 729–751.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lau, R. R, & Redlawsk, D. P. (1997). Voting correctly. American Political Science Review, 91, 585–599.CrossRef Lau, R. R, & Redlawsk, D. P. (1997). Voting correctly. American Political Science Review, 91, 585–599.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Maitre, H. J. (1994). The tilt to the news: How American journalism has swerved from the ideal of objectivity. In George McKenna, & Stanley. Feingold (Eds.), Taking sides: Clashing views on controversial political issues (9th ed., pp. 58–64). Guilford, Connecticut: Dushkin Publishing Group. Maitre, H. J. (1994). The tilt to the news: How American journalism has swerved from the ideal of objectivity. In George McKenna, & Stanley. Feingold (Eds.), Taking sides: Clashing views on controversial political issues (9th ed., pp. 58–64). Guilford, Connecticut: Dushkin Publishing Group.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations: Politically knowledgeable citizens are guided by a trusted source. American Journal of Political Science, 44, 301–315.CrossRef Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations: Politically knowledgeable citizens are guided by a trusted source. American Journal of Political Science, 44, 301–315.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, M. C. (2002). The Bush dyslexicon: Observations on a national disorder. New York: W.W. Norton and Company. Miller, M. C. (2002). The Bush dyslexicon: Observations on a national disorder. New York: W.W. Norton and Company.
Zurück zum Zitat McGuire, W. J. (1969). The nature of attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey, & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Knopf. McGuire, W. J. (1969). The nature of attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey, & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Knopf.
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. J. (1993a). Public opinion and heuristic processing of source cues. Political Behavior, 15, 167–187 .CrossRef Mondak, J. J. (1993a). Public opinion and heuristic processing of source cues. Political Behavior, 15, 167–187 .CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. J. (1993b). Source cues and policy approval: the cognitive dynamics of public support for the reagan agenda. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 186–212.CrossRef Mondak, J. J. (1993b). Source cues and policy approval: the cognitive dynamics of public support for the reagan agenda. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 186–212.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. J. (1994). Cognitive heuristics, heuristic processing, and efficiency in political decision making. In M. X. D. Carpini, L. Huddy, & R. Y. Shapiro (Eds.), Research in micropolitics (pp. 117–142). Greenwich, Conn.: JAI Press Inc. Mondak, J. J. (1994). Cognitive heuristics, heuristic processing, and efficiency in political decision making. In M. X. D. Carpini, L. Huddy, & R. Y. Shapiro (Eds.), Research in micropolitics (pp. 117–142). Greenwich, Conn.: JAI Press Inc.
Zurück zum Zitat Ottati, V. C. (1990). Determinants of political judgments: The joint influence of normative and heuristic rules of inference. Political Behavior, 12, 159–179.CrossRef Ottati, V. C. (1990). Determinants of political judgments: The joint influence of normative and heuristic rules of inference. Political Behavior, 12, 159–179.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ottati, V. C, & Wyer Jr. R. S. (1990). The cognitive mediators of political choice: toward a comprehensive model of political information processing. In J. A. Freejohn, & J. H. Kuklinski (Eds.), Information and democratic processes (pp. 186–216). Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Ottati, V. C, & Wyer Jr. R. S. (1990). The cognitive mediators of political choice: toward a comprehensive model of political information processing. In J. A. Freejohn, & J. H. Kuklinski (Eds.), Information and democratic processes (pp. 186–216). Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Ottati V., Fishbein, M., & Middlestadt, S. E. (1988). Determinants of voters’ beliefs about the candidates’ stands on issues: the role of evaluative bias heuristics and the candidates’ expressed message. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 517–529.CrossRef Ottati V., Fishbein, M., & Middlestadt, S. E. (1988). Determinants of voters’ beliefs about the candidates’ stands on issues: the role of evaluative bias heuristics and the candidates’ expressed message. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 517–529.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of Persuasion. In Leonard B. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, 19, 124–181. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of Persuasion. In Leonard B. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, 19, 124–181.
Zurück zum Zitat Riggle, E. D., Ottati, V. C., Wyer Jr. R. S., Kuklinski, J, & Schwarz, N. (1992). Bases of political judgments: The role of stereotypic and nonstereotypic information. Political Behavior, 14, 67–87.CrossRef Riggle, E. D., Ottati, V. C., Wyer Jr. R. S., Kuklinski, J, & Schwarz, N. (1992). Bases of political judgments: The role of stereotypic and nonstereotypic information. Political Behavior, 14, 67–87.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson, M., & Sheehan, M. (1983). Over the wire and on TV. Russell Sage Foundation. Robinson, M., & Sheehan, M. (1983). Over the wire and on TV. Russell Sage Foundation.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson, M. J., & Clancey, M., (1985). Teflon politics. Public Opinion, 17, 14–18. Robinson, M. J., & Clancey, M., (1985). Teflon politics. Public Opinion, 17, 14–18.
Zurück zum Zitat Sniderman, P. M., Richard, B., & Philip, T. (1991). Reasoning and choice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Sniderman, P. M., Richard, B., & Philip, T. (1991). Reasoning and choice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Tversky, A., & Kahneman D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.CrossRef Tversky, A., & Kahneman D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Vallone, R. P., Ross, L, & Lepper, M. R. (1985). The hostile media phenomenon: Biased perception and perceptions of media bias in coverage of the Beiruit massacre. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 577–585.CrossRef Vallone, R. P., Ross, L, & Lepper, M. R. (1985). The hostile media phenomenon: Biased perception and perceptions of media bias in coverage of the Beiruit massacre. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 577–585.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Walsh, K. C. (2003). Talking about politics: Informal groups and social identity in American life. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Walsh, K. C. (2003). Talking about politics: Informal groups and social identity in American life. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Weaver, P. H. (1972). Is television news biased? The Public Interest, 26, 57–74. Weaver, P. H. (1972). Is television news biased? The Public Interest, 26, 57–74.
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press. Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Metadaten
Titel
The Messenger Overwhelming the Message: Ideological Cues and Perceptions of Bias in Television News
verfasst von
Joel Turner
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2007
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 4/2007
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-007-9031-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2007

Political Behavior 4/2007 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner