Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Logic, Language and Information 3/2013

01.07.2013

An Analytic Tableaux Model for Deductive Mastermind Empirically Tested with a Massively Used Online Learning System

verfasst von: Nina Gierasimczuk, Han L. J. van der Maas, Maartje E. J. Raijmakers

Erschienen in: Journal of Logic, Language and Information | Ausgabe 3/2013

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

The paper is concerned with the psychological relevance of a logical model for deductive reasoning. We propose a new way to analyze logical reasoning in a deductive version of the Mastermind game implemented within a popular Dutch online educational learning system (Math Garden). Our main goal is to derive predictions about the difficulty of Deductive Mastermind tasks. By means of a logical analysis we derive the number of steps needed for solving these tasks (a proxy for working memory load). Our model is based on the analytic tableaux method, known from proof theory. We associate the difficulty of Deductive Mastermind game-items with the size of the corresponding logical trees obtained by the tableaux method. We derive empirical hypotheses from this model. A large group of students (over 37 thousand children, 5–12 years of age) played the Deductive Mastermind game, which gave empirical difficulty ratings of all 321 game-items. The results show that our logical approach predicts these item ratings well, which supports the psychological relevance of our model.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 390 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe




 

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
The exact way in which the rating works is described in Klinkenberg et al. (2011).
 
2
We do not need the rule for negation because in our formulae only propositional atoms are negated.
 
3
The recursive algorithm predicts difficulty of an item that corresponds to the minimal tableau build according to the following rules: each line is read from left to right and feedbacks are analyzed in the order \(oo, rr, gr\), and \(or\). The abstract complexity measure then corresponds to the number of nodes in such a tableau.
 
4
In general, the tableau-generating algorithm could be halted either after computing the full tree or immediately after finding a consistent solution. In the first case all conjectures are evaluated and hence \(ooFB, rrFB\), and \(grFB\) feedbacks would be the best predictors. In the second case only the non-redundant conjectures are evaluated, whose number can be less than the total number of conjectures. The second case would allow solving an item with many conjectures much faster, resulting in a lower item rating. In this case \(oo, rr, gr\), and \(or\) would be better predictors.
 
5
Note that the DMM-items are difficult if: 1) a line with \(or\) feedback needs to be analyzed in order to find the solution; or 2) a line with \(gr\) feedback needs to be analyzed and one color that is in the solution was not included in any line of the DMM-item. We will show that none of those cases is possible within 2-pin DMM-items of 2 colors. First let us consider 1: In the 2-pin DMM-items there are two possibilities, a line consists of either (a) two pins of the same color, or (b) two pins of different colors. If (a) is the case the \(or\) feedback implies that one of the pins is of the correct color but in a wrong position, which does not make sense, because the other pin is of the same color and in the only remaining position. If (b) is the case the \(or\) implies means that one pin is not in the solution, which implies that the two pins have to be of the same color (since there are only two colors). But then it means that one of the pins in the line was in fact of the correct color and position to start with, which was not reflected by an adequate \(g\) feedback. This renders such a case impossible. Let us now consider 2: Here, a line must consist of two flowers of the same color (otherwise there would be no color that is not included in any of the lines, because there are only 2 colors). But then, the \(gr\) feedback does not give a unique solution. The latter is only possible if another line introduces the missing color. In conclusion, the properties of 2-pin DMM-items of only 2 colors make it impossible for any of them to fall into the “difficult” category.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Barton, E., Berwick, R., & Ristad, E. (1987). Computational complexity and natural language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Barton, E., Berwick, R., & Ristad, E. (1987). Computational complexity and natural language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Berwick, R., & Weinberg, A. (1984). The grammatical basis of linguistic performance. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Berwick, R., & Weinberg, A. (1984). The grammatical basis of linguistic performance. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Beth, E. W. (1955). Semantic entailment and formal derivability. Mededelingen van de Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen Afdeling Letterkunde, 18(13), 309–342. Beth, E. W. (1955). Semantic entailment and formal derivability. Mededelingen van de Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen Afdeling Letterkunde, 18(13), 309–342.
Zurück zum Zitat Cherniak, C. (1986). Minimal rationality. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Cherniak, C. (1986). Minimal rationality. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Chvatal, V. (1983). Mastermind. Combinatorica, 3, 325–329. Chvatal, V. (1983). Mastermind. Combinatorica, 3, 325–329.
Zurück zum Zitat Elo, A. (1978). The rating of chessplayers, past and present. Arco. Elo, A. (1978). The rating of chessplayers, past and present. Arco.
Zurück zum Zitat Ghosh, S., & Meijering, B. (2011). On combining cognitive and formal modeling: A case study involving strategic reasoning. In J. Van Eijck, & R. Verbrugge (Eds.) Proceedings of the workshop on reasoning about other minds: Logical and cognitive perspectives (RAOM-2011), Groningen, The Netherlands, July 11th, 2011, CEUR-WS.org. CEUR Workshop Proceedings, vol 751 (pp. 79–92). Ghosh, S., & Meijering, B. (2011). On combining cognitive and formal modeling: A case study involving strategic reasoning. In J. Van Eijck, & R. Verbrugge (Eds.) Proceedings of the workshop on reasoning about other minds: Logical and cognitive perspectives (RAOM-2011), Groningen, The Netherlands, July 11th, 2011, CEUR-WS.org. CEUR Workshop Proceedings, vol 751 (pp. 79–92).
Zurück zum Zitat Ghosh, S., Meijering, B., & Verbrugge, R. (2010). Logic meets cognition: Empirical reasoning in games. In O. Boissier, A. E. Fallah-Seghrouchni, S. Hassas & N. Maudet (Eds.) Proceedings of the multi-agent logics, languages, and organisations federated workshops (MALLOW 2010), Lyon, France, August 30–September 2, 2010, CEUR-WS.org, CEUR Workshop Proceedings, vol, 627 (pp. 15–34). Ghosh, S., Meijering, B., & Verbrugge, R. (2010). Logic meets cognition: Empirical reasoning in games. In O. Boissier, A. E. Fallah-Seghrouchni, S. Hassas & N. Maudet (Eds.) Proceedings of the multi-agent logics, languages, and organisations federated workshops (MALLOW 2010), Lyon, France, August 30–September 2, 2010, CEUR-WS.org, CEUR Workshop Proceedings, vol, 627 (pp. 15–34).
Zurück zum Zitat Gierasimczuk, N., & Szymanik, J. (2009). Branching quantification v. two-way quantification. Journal of Semantics, 26(4), 367–392.CrossRef Gierasimczuk, N., & Szymanik, J. (2009). Branching quantification v. two-way quantification. Journal of Semantics, 26(4), 367–392.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Greenwell, D. L. (1999–2000). Mastermind. Journal of Recreational Mathematics, 30, 191–192. Greenwell, D. L. (1999–2000). Mastermind. Journal of Recreational Mathematics, 30, 191–192.
Zurück zum Zitat Irving, R. W. (1978–79). Towards an optimum Mastermind strategy. Journal of Recreational Mathematics, 11, 81–87. Irving, R. W. (1978–79). Towards an optimum Mastermind strategy. Journal of Recreational Mathematics, 11, 81–87.
Zurück zum Zitat Jansen, B., & Van der Maas, H. (1997). A statistical test of the rule assessment methodology by latent class analysis. Developmental Review, 17, 321–357.CrossRef Jansen, B., & Van der Maas, H. (1997). A statistical test of the rule assessment methodology by latent class analysis. Developmental Review, 17, 321–357.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Klinkenberg, S., Straatemeier, M., & Van der Maas, H. L. J. (2011). Computer adaptive practice of maths ability using a new item response model for on the fly ability and difficulty estimation. Computers in Education, 57, 1813–1824.CrossRef Klinkenberg, S., Straatemeier, M., & Van der Maas, H. L. J. (2011). Computer adaptive practice of maths ability using a new item response model for on the fly ability and difficulty estimation. Computers in Education, 57, 1813–1824.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Knuth, D. E. (1977). The computer as master mind. Journal of Recreational Mathematics, 9(1), 1–6. Knuth, D. E. (1977). The computer as master mind. Journal of Recreational Mathematics, 9(1), 1–6.
Zurück zum Zitat Kooi, B. (2005). Yet another Mastermind strategy. ICGA Journal, 28(1), 13–20. Kooi, B. (2005). Yet another Mastermind strategy. ICGA Journal, 28(1), 13–20.
Zurück zum Zitat Koyama, M., & Lai, T. (1993). An optimal Mastermind strategy. Journal of Recreational Mathematics, 25, 251–256. Koyama, M., & Lai, T. (1993). An optimal Mastermind strategy. Journal of Recreational Mathematics, 25, 251–256.
Zurück zum Zitat Maris, G., & Van der Maas, H. L. J. (2012). Speed-accuracy response models: Scoring rules based on response time and accuracy. Psychometrika, 77(4), 615–633.CrossRef Maris, G., & Van der Maas, H. L. J. (2012). Speed-accuracy response models: Scoring rules based on response time and accuracy. Psychometrika, 77(4), 615–633.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Meijering, B., van Rijn, H., Taatgen, N. A., & Verbrugge, R. (2012). What eye movements can tell about theory of mind in a strategic game. PLoS One, 7(9), e45961 Meijering, B., van Rijn, H., Taatgen, N. A., & Verbrugge, R. (2012). What eye movements can tell about theory of mind in a strategic game. PLoS One, 7(9), e45961
Zurück zum Zitat Pelánek, R. (2011). Difficulty rating of Sudoku puzzles by a computational model. In R. C. Murray & P. M. McCarthy (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-fourth international Florida artificial intelligence research society conference, May 18–20, 2011. Palm Beach, Florida, USA: AAAI Press. Pelánek, R. (2011). Difficulty rating of Sudoku puzzles by a computational model. In R. C. Murray & P. M. McCarthy (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-fourth international Florida artificial intelligence research society conference, May 18–20, 2011. Palm Beach, Florida, USA: AAAI Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Ristad, E. (1993). The language complexity game. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Ristad, E. (1993). The language complexity game. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmittmann, V. D., Van der Maas, H. L. J., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2012). Distinct discrimination learning strategies and their relation with spatial memory and attentional control in 4- to 14-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(4), 644–62.CrossRef Schmittmann, V. D., Van der Maas, H. L. J., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2012). Distinct discrimination learning strategies and their relation with spatial memory and attentional control in 4- to 14-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(4), 644–62.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stuckman, J., & Zhang, G. (2006). Mastermind is NP-complete. INFOCOMP Journal of Computer Science, 5, 25–28. Stuckman, J., & Zhang, G. (2006). Mastermind is NP-complete. INFOCOMP Journal of Computer Science, 5, 25–28.
Zurück zum Zitat Szymanik, J. (2009). Quantifiers in time and space. Computational complexity of generalized quantifiers in natural language. PhD thesis, Universiteit van Amsterdam. Szymanik, J. (2009). Quantifiers in time and space. Computational complexity of generalized quantifiers in natural language. PhD thesis, Universiteit van Amsterdam.
Zurück zum Zitat Szymanik, J. (2010). Computational complexity of polyadic lifts of generalized quantifiers in natural language. Linguistics and Philosophy, 33(3), 215–250.CrossRef Szymanik, J. (2010). Computational complexity of polyadic lifts of generalized quantifiers in natural language. Linguistics and Philosophy, 33(3), 215–250.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Szymanik, J., & Zajenkowski, M. (2010). Comprehension of simple quantifiers. Empirical evaluation of a computational model. Cognitive Science, 34(3), 521–532.CrossRef Szymanik, J., & Zajenkowski, M. (2010). Comprehension of simple quantifiers. Empirical evaluation of a computational model. Cognitive Science, 34(3), 521–532.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van Benthem, J. (1974). Semantic tableaus. Nieuw Archief voor Wiskunde, 22, 44–59. Van Benthem, J. (1974). Semantic tableaus. Nieuw Archief voor Wiskunde, 22, 44–59.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Bers, B. M. C. W., Visser, I., Van Schijndel, T. J. P., Mandell, D. J., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2011). The dynamics of development on the dimensional change card sorting task. Developmental Science, 14(5), 960–71.CrossRef Van Bers, B. M. C. W., Visser, I., Van Schijndel, T. J. P., Mandell, D. J., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2011). The dynamics of development on the dimensional change card sorting task. Developmental Science, 14(5), 960–71.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Maas, H., & Molenaar, P. (1992). Stagewise cognitive development: An application of catastrophe theory. Psychological Review, 99(3), 395–417.CrossRef Van der Maas, H., & Molenaar, P. (1992). Stagewise cognitive development: An application of catastrophe theory. Psychological Review, 99(3), 395–417.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Maas, H., Klinkenberg, S., & Straatemeier, M. (2010). Rekentuin.nl: Combinatie van oefenen en toetsen. Examens, 4, 10–14. Van der Maas, H., Klinkenberg, S., & Straatemeier, M. (2010). Rekentuin.nl: Combinatie van oefenen en toetsen. Examens, 4, 10–14.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Rooij, I. (2008). The tractable cognition thesis. Cognitive Science, 32, 939–984.CrossRef Van Rooij, I. (2008). The tractable cognition thesis. Cognitive Science, 32, 939–984.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van Rooij, I., Kwisthout, J., Blokpoel, M., Szymanik, J., Wareham, T., & Toni, I. (2011). Intentional communication: Computationally easy or difficult? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 1–18. Van Rooij, I., Kwisthout, J., Blokpoel, M., Szymanik, J., Wareham, T., & Toni, I. (2011). Intentional communication: Computationally easy or difficult? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 1–18.
Zurück zum Zitat Verbrugge, R., & Mol, L. (2008). Learning to apply theory of mind. Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 17(4), 489–511.CrossRef Verbrugge, R., & Mol, L. (2008). Learning to apply theory of mind. Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 17(4), 489–511.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zajenkowski, M., Styła, R., & Szymanik, J. (2011). A computational approach to quantifiers as an explanation for some language impairments in schizophrenia. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(6), 595–600.CrossRef Zajenkowski, M., Styła, R., & Szymanik, J. (2011). A computational approach to quantifiers as an explanation for some language impairments in schizophrenia. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(6), 595–600.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
An Analytic Tableaux Model for Deductive Mastermind Empirically Tested with a Massively Used Online Learning System
verfasst von
Nina Gierasimczuk
Han L. J. van der Maas
Maartje E. J. Raijmakers
Publikationsdatum
01.07.2013
Verlag
Springer Netherlands
Erschienen in
Journal of Logic, Language and Information / Ausgabe 3/2013
Print ISSN: 0925-8531
Elektronische ISSN: 1572-9583
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10849-013-9177-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2013

Journal of Logic, Language and Information 3/2013 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner