Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Learning & Behavior 1/2013

01.03.2013

Associative foundation of causal learning in rats

verfasst von: Cody W. Polack, Bridget L. McConnell, Ralph R. Miller

Erschienen in: Learning & Behavior | Ausgabe 1/2013

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Are humans unique in their ability to interpret exogenous events as causes? We addressed this question by observing the behavior of rats for indications of causal learning. Within an operant motor–sensory preconditioning paradigm, associative surgical techniques revealed that rats attempted to control an outcome (i.e., a potential effect) by manipulating a potential exogenous cause (i.e., an intervention). Rats were able to generate an innocuous auditory stimulus. This stimulus was then paired with an aversive stimulus. The animals subsequently avoided potential generation of the predictive cue, but not if the aversive stimulus was subsequently devalued or the predictive cue was extinguished (Exp. 1). In Experiment 2, we demonstrated that the aversive stimulus we used was in fact aversive, that it was subject to devaluation, that the cue–aversive stimulus pairings did make the cue a conditioned stimulus, and that the cue was subject to extinction. In Experiments 3 and 4, we established that the decrease in leverpressing observed in Experiment 1 was goal-directed instrumental behavior rather than purely a product of Pavlovian conditioning. To the extent that interventions suggest causal reasoning, it appears that causal reasoning can be based on associations between contiguous exogenous events. Thus, contiguity appears capable of establishing causal relationships between exogenous events. Our results challenge the widely held view that causal learning is uniquely human, and suggest that causal learning is explicable in an associative framework.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
In principle, the use of this control treatment could make the CS a safety signal for the control groups. Causal learning in our basic experimental group could also be manifest if we were to see an increase in leverpressing to generate this putative safety signal. We consider this to be highly unlikely, as two presentations of the CS and of the US would normally be far too few to produce a reliable safety signal. Therefore, we will refrain from considering control of a safety signal in our preparation, although this interpretation would not undermine our discussion of rats manipulating a predictor of the US only if the predictor were viewed as a cause.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Adams, C. D., & Dickinson, A. (1981). Instrumental responding following reinforcer devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33B, 109–122. Adams, C. D., & Dickinson, A. (1981). Instrumental responding following reinforcer devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33B, 109–122.
Zurück zum Zitat Allan, L. G. (1993). Human contingency judgments: Rule based or associative? Psychological Bulletin, 114, 435–448.CrossRefPubMed Allan, L. G. (1993). Human contingency judgments: Rule based or associative? Psychological Bulletin, 114, 435–448.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Allan, L. G., & Tangen, J. M. (2005). Judging relationships between events: How do we do it? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 22–27.CrossRefPubMed Allan, L. G., & Tangen, J. M. (2005). Judging relationships between events: How do we do it? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 22–27.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bonardi, C., & Hall, G. (1994). Discriminative inhibition is specific to the response–reinforcer association but not to the discriminative stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 278–291. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.20.3.278 CrossRefPubMed Bonardi, C., & Hall, G. (1994). Discriminative inhibition is specific to the response–reinforcer association but not to the discriminative stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 278–291. doi:10.​1037/​0097-7403.​20.​3.​278 CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Chapman, G. B., & Robbins, S. J. (1990). Cue interaction in human contingency judgment. Memory & Cognition, 18, 537–545.CrossRef Chapman, G. B., & Robbins, S. J. (1990). Cue interaction in human contingency judgment. Memory & Cognition, 18, 537–545.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chatlosh, D. L., Neunaber, D. J., & Wasserman, E. A. (1985). Response–outcome contingency: Behavioral and judgmental effects of appetitive and aversive outcomes with college students. Learning and Motivation, 16, 1–34. doi:10.1016/0023-9690(85)90002-5 CrossRef Chatlosh, D. L., Neunaber, D. J., & Wasserman, E. A. (1985). Response–outcome contingency: Behavioral and judgmental effects of appetitive and aversive outcomes with college students. Learning and Motivation, 16, 1–34. doi:10.​1016/​0023-9690(85)90002-5 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Colwill, R. M., & Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Postconditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 120–132. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.11.1.120 CrossRef Colwill, R. M., & Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Postconditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 120–132. doi:10.​1037/​0097-7403.​11.​1.​120 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat De Houwer, J., Beckers, T., & Glautier, S. (2002). Outcome and cue properties modulate blocking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 965–985. De Houwer, J., Beckers, T., & Glautier, S. (2002). Outcome and cue properties modulate blocking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 965–985.
Zurück zum Zitat Dickinson, A. (1997). Bolles’s psychological syllogism. In M. E. Bouton & M. Fanselow (Eds.), Learning, motivation, and cognition: The functional behaviorism of Robert C. Bolles (pp. 345–367). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Dickinson, A. (1997). Bolles’s psychological syllogism. In M. E. Bouton & M. Fanselow (Eds.), Learning, motivation, and cognition: The functional behaviorism of Robert C. Bolles (pp. 345–367). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Zurück zum Zitat Dickinson, A., Campos, J., Varga, Z. I., & Balleine, B. (1996). Bidirectional instrumental conditioning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49B, 289–306. Dickinson, A., Campos, J., Varga, Z. I., & Balleine, B. (1996). Bidirectional instrumental conditioning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49B, 289–306.
Zurück zum Zitat Dickinson, A., & Dearing, M. F. (1979). Appetitive–aversive interactions and inhibitory processes. In A. Dickinson & R. Boakes (Eds.), Mechanisms of learning and memory (pp. 203–232). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Dickinson, A., & Dearing, M. F. (1979). Appetitive–aversive interactions and inhibitory processes. In A. Dickinson & R. Boakes (Eds.), Mechanisms of learning and memory (pp. 203–232). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Dwyer, D. M., Starns, J., & Honey, R. C. (2009). “Causal reasoning” in rats: A reappraisal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 578–586. doi:10.1037/a0015007 CrossRefPubMed Dwyer, D. M., Starns, J., & Honey, R. C. (2009). “Causal reasoning” in rats: A reappraisal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 578–586. doi:10.​1037/​a0015007 CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hall, G., & Honey, R. C. (1989). Contextual effects in conditioning, latent inhibition, and habituation: Associative and retrieval functions of contextual cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, 232–241.CrossRef Hall, G., & Honey, R. C. (1989). Contextual effects in conditioning, latent inhibition, and habituation: Associative and retrieval functions of contextual cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, 232–241.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Holland, P. C., & Rescorla, R. A. (1975). The effect of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after first- and second-order appetitive conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 355–363. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.1.4.355 CrossRef Holland, P. C., & Rescorla, R. A. (1975). The effect of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after first- and second-order appetitive conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 355–363. doi:10.​1037/​0097-7403.​1.​4.​355 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Killeen, P. R. (1981). Learning as causal inference. In M. L. Commons & J. A. Nevins (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Vol. 1. Discriminative properties of reinforcement schedules (pp. 89–112). Cambridge: Ballinger. Killeen, P. R. (1981). Learning as causal inference. In M. L. Commons & J. A. Nevins (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Vol. 1. Discriminative properties of reinforcement schedules (pp. 89–112). Cambridge: Ballinger.
Zurück zum Zitat Laborda, M. A., & Miller, R. R. (2011). S–R associations, their extinction, and recovery in an animal model of anxiety: A new associative account of phobias without recall of original trauma. Behavior Therapy, 42, 153–169.CrossRefPubMed Laborda, M. A., & Miller, R. R. (2011). S–R associations, their extinction, and recovery in an animal model of anxiety: A new associative account of phobias without recall of original trauma. Behavior Therapy, 42, 153–169.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, R. R., & Escobar, M. (2002). Associative interference between cues and between outcomes presented together and presented apart: An integration. Behavioural Processes, 57, 163–185.CrossRefPubMed Miller, R. R., & Escobar, M. (2002). Associative interference between cues and between outcomes presented together and presented apart: An integration. Behavioural Processes, 57, 163–185.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nieto, J. (1984). Transfer of conditioned inhibition across different aversive reinforcers in the rat. Learning and Motivation, 15, 37–57.CrossRef Nieto, J. (1984). Transfer of conditioned inhibition across different aversive reinforcers in the rat. Learning and Motivation, 15, 37–57.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Parkinson, J. A., Roberts, A. C., Everitt, B. J., & Di Ciano, P. (2005). Acquisition of instrumental conditioned reinforcement is resistant to the devaluation of the unconditioned stimulus. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58B, 19–30. doi:10.1080/02724990444000023 Parkinson, J. A., Roberts, A. C., Everitt, B. J., & Di Ciano, P. (2005). Acquisition of instrumental conditioned reinforcement is resistant to the devaluation of the unconditioned stimulus. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58B, 19–30. doi:10.​1080/​0272499044400002​3
Zurück zum Zitat Pineño, O., Denniston, J., Beckers, T., Matute, H., & Miller, R. R. (2005). Contrasting predictive and causal values of predictors and of causes. Learning & Behavior, 33, 184–196.CrossRef Pineño, O., Denniston, J., Beckers, T., Matute, H., & Miller, R. R. (2005). Contrasting predictive and causal values of predictors and of causes. Learning & Behavior, 33, 184–196.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rescorla, R. A. (1973). Effects of US habituation following conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 137–143.CrossRefPubMed Rescorla, R. A. (1973). Effects of US habituation following conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 137–143.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rescorla, R. A. (1987). A Pavlovian analysis of goal-directed behavior. American Psychologist, 42, 119–129.CrossRef Rescorla, R. A. (1987). A Pavlovian analysis of goal-directed behavior. American Psychologist, 42, 119–129.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shanks, D. R., & Dickinson, A. (1991). Instrumental judgment and performance under variations in action–outcome contingency and contiguity. Memory & Cognition, 19, 353–360.CrossRef Shanks, D. R., & Dickinson, A. (1991). Instrumental judgment and performance under variations in action–outcome contingency and contiguity. Memory & Cognition, 19, 353–360.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century.
Zurück zum Zitat Skinner, B. F. (1948). “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.CrossRefPubMed Skinner, B. F. (1948). “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat St Claire-Smith, R., & MacLaren, D. (1983). Response preconditioning effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 41–48.CrossRef St Claire-Smith, R., & MacLaren, D. (1983). Response preconditioning effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 41–48.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stout, S., Escobar, M., & Miller, R. R. (2004). Trial number and compound stimuli temporal relationship as joint determinants of second-order conditioning and conditioned inhibition. Learning & Behavior, 32, 230–239. doi:10.3758/BF03196024 CrossRef Stout, S., Escobar, M., & Miller, R. R. (2004). Trial number and compound stimuli temporal relationship as joint determinants of second-order conditioning and conditioned inhibition. Learning & Behavior, 32, 230–239. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196024 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Woodward, J. (2003). Making things happen: A theory of causal explanation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Woodward, J. (2003). Making things happen: A theory of causal explanation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Metadaten
Titel
Associative foundation of causal learning in rats
verfasst von
Cody W. Polack
Bridget L. McConnell
Ralph R. Miller
Publikationsdatum
01.03.2013
Verlag
Springer-Verlag
Erschienen in
Learning & Behavior / Ausgabe 1/2013
Print ISSN: 1543-4494
Elektronische ISSN: 1543-4508
DOI
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-012-0075-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2013

Learning & Behavior 1/2013 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner