Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Learning & Behavior 4/2018

30.04.2018

Behavioral synchronization and affiliation: Dogs exhibit human-like skills

verfasst von: Charlotte Duranton, Florence Gaunet

Erschienen in: Learning & Behavior | Ausgabe 4/2018

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Behavioral synchronization is evolutionary adaptive, fostering social cohesion. In humans, affiliation between partners is associated with a high level of behavioral synchronization; people show increased affiliation towards people who synchronize with them. Surprisingly, until recently, little was known about these phenomena at an interspecific level, which is, however, essential to better understand the respective roles of evolution and ontogeny. After presenting why dog–human dyads are a relevant biological model to study this field of social cognition, we review the recent findings about dog–human behavioral synchronization. We summarize recently published findings on behavioral synchronization and affiliation between dogs and humans. We also review results showing that genetic selection modulates behavioral synchronization propensity in dogs, emphasizing the role of genetic selection on dog’s social behaviors towards humans. Finally, we discuss the possible evolutionary influences and proximate mechanisms of this phenomenon. We conclude that, as in humans, behavioral synchronization acts as a social glue between dogs and humans. After dogs’ ability to use human-directional cues or to produce referential cues towards humans, we evidenced a new human-like social process in the dog, at the interspecfic level with humans.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
There are two different categories of behavioral synchronization: conscious synchronization, in which each partner is aware of adjusting his or her behavior to others so that it can lead to phenomena such as cooperation (see, e.g., Dávid-Barrett & Dunbar, 2012; Valdesolo et al., 2010), and nonconscious synchronization, when the interacting partners are not aware of being synchronized or synchronizing their behaviors with others (see Duranton et al., 2017a).
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Ákos, Z., Beck, R., Nagy, M., Vicsek, T., & Kubinyi, E. (2014). Leadership and path characteristics during walks are linked to dominance order and individual traits in dogs. PLOS Comp Biol 10(1), e1003446.CrossRef Ákos, Z., Beck, R., Nagy, M., Vicsek, T., & Kubinyi, E. (2014). Leadership and path characteristics during walks are linked to dominance order and individual traits in dogs. PLOS Comp Biol 10(1), e1003446.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Asendorpf, J. B., Warkentin, V., & Baudonnière, P.-M. (1996). Self-awareness and other-awareness II: Mirror self-recognition, social contingency, awareness, and synchronic imitation. Developmental Psychology, 32(2), 313–321.CrossRef Asendorpf, J. B., Warkentin, V., & Baudonnière, P.-M. (1996). Self-awareness and other-awareness II: Mirror self-recognition, social contingency, awareness, and synchronic imitation. Developmental Psychology, 32(2), 313–321.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bunford, N., Andics, A., Kis, A., Miklósi, Á., & Gácsi, M. (2017). Canis familiaris as a model for non-invasive comparative neuroscience. Trends in Neuroscience, 40(7), 438–452.CrossRef Bunford, N., Andics, A., Kis, A., Miklósi, Á., & Gácsi, M. (2017). Canis familiaris as a model for non-invasive comparative neuroscience. Trends in Neuroscience, 40(7), 438–452.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy 1(2), 149–219.CrossRef Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy 1(2), 149–219.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A., (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.CrossRefPubMed Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A., (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Chartrand, T. L., & Lakin, J. L., (2013). The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 285–308.CrossRefPubMed Chartrand, T. L., & Lakin, J. L., (2013). The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 285–308.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Clearfield, M. W., Osborne, C. N., & Mullen, M. (2008). Learning by looking: infants’ social looking behavior across the transition from crawling to walking. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100, 297–307.CrossRefPubMed Clearfield, M. W., Osborne, C. N., & Mullen, M. (2008). Learning by looking: infants’ social looking behavior across the transition from crawling to walking. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100, 297–307.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Dávid-Barrett, T., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012). Cooperation, behavioural synchrony and status in social network. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 308, 88–95.CrossRefPubMed Dávid-Barrett, T., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012). Cooperation, behavioural synchrony and status in social network. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 308, 88–95.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat De Rosnay, M., Cooper, P. J., Tsigaras, N., & Murray, L. (2006). Transmission of social anxiety from mother to infant: An experimental study using a social referencing paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1165–1175.CrossRefPubMed De Rosnay, M., Cooper, P. J., Tsigaras, N., & Murray, L. (2006). Transmission of social anxiety from mother to infant: An experimental study using a social referencing paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1165–1175.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Duffy, D. L., Hsu, Y., & Serpell, J. A. (2008). Breed differences in canine aggression. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 114, 441–460.CrossRef Duffy, D. L., Hsu, Y., & Serpell, J. A. (2008). Breed differences in canine aggression. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 114, 441–460.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Duranton, C. (2017). Dog–human behavioral synchronization and affiliation (Doctoral dissertation). Marseille: Aix-Marseille University. Duranton, C. (2017). Dog–human behavioral synchronization and affiliation (Doctoral dissertation). Marseille: Aix-Marseille University.
Zurück zum Zitat Duranton, C., Bedossa, T., & Gaunet, F. (2016). When facing an unfamiliar person, pet dogs present social referencing based on their owner’s direction of movement alone. Animal Behavior, 113, 147–156.CrossRef Duranton, C., Bedossa, T., & Gaunet, F. (2016). When facing an unfamiliar person, pet dogs present social referencing based on their owner’s direction of movement alone. Animal Behavior, 113, 147–156.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Duranton, C., Bedossa, T., & Gaunet, F. (2017b). Do shelter dogs engage in social referencing with their caregiver in an approach paradigm? An exploratory study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 189, 57–65.CrossRef Duranton, C., Bedossa, T., & Gaunet, F. (2017b). Do shelter dogs engage in social referencing with their caregiver in an approach paradigm? An exploratory study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 189, 57–65.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Duranton, C., Bedossa, T., Gaunet, F. (2018b). Shelter dogs present activity synchrony but only mere location synchrony with their caregivers when walking in an outside area. Manuscript submitted for publication. Duranton, C., Bedossa, T., Gaunet, F. (2018b). Shelter dogs present activity synchrony but only mere location synchrony with their caregivers when walking in an outside area. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Zurück zum Zitat Duranton, C., Bedossa, T. & Gaunet, F (2018c). Pet dogs exhibit social preference for people who synchronize their behaviours with them. Manuscript submitted for publication. Duranton, C., Bedossa, T. & Gaunet, F (2018c). Pet dogs exhibit social preference for people who synchronize their behaviours with them. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Zurück zum Zitat Duranton, C., & Gaunet, F. (2015). Canis sensitivus: Affiliation and dogs’ sensitivity to others’ behavior as the basis for synchronization with humans? Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 10, 513–524.CrossRef Duranton, C., & Gaunet, F. (2015). Canis sensitivus: Affiliation and dogs’ sensitivity to others’ behavior as the basis for synchronization with humans? Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 10, 513–524.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Duranton, C., & Gaunet, F. (2016a). Behavioural synchronization from an ethological perspective: Short overview of its adaptive values. Adaptive Behavior, 24(3), 181–191.CrossRef Duranton, C., & Gaunet, F. (2016a). Behavioural synchronization from an ethological perspective: Short overview of its adaptive values. Adaptive Behavior, 24(3), 181–191.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Duranton, C., & Gaunet, F. (2016b). Effects of shelter housing on dogs’ sensitivity to human social cues. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 14, 20–27.CrossRef Duranton, C., & Gaunet, F. (2016b). Effects of shelter housing on dogs’ sensitivity to human social cues. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 14, 20–27.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Eken Asp, H., Fikse, W. F., Nilsson, K., & Strandberg, E. (2015). Breed differences in everyday behavior of dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 169, 69–77.CrossRef Eken Asp, H., Fikse, W. F., Nilsson, K., & Strandberg, E. (2015). Breed differences in everyday behavior of dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 169, 69–77.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fallani, G., Prato Previde, E., & Valsecchi, P. (2007). Behavioral and physiological responses of guide dogs to a situation of emotional distress. Physiology & Behavior, 90, 648–655.CrossRef Fallani, G., Prato Previde, E., & Valsecchi, P. (2007). Behavioral and physiological responses of guide dogs to a situation of emotional distress. Physiology & Behavior, 90, 648–655.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A., & Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLOS Biology, 4, e302.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A., & Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLOS Biology, 4, e302.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Gácsi, M., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Dóka A, & Csányi, V. (2001). Attachment behavior of adult dogs (Canis familiaris) living at rescue centers: Forming new bonds. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(4), 423–431.CrossRefPubMed Gácsi, M., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Dóka A, & Csányi, V. (2001). Attachment behavior of adult dogs (Canis familiaris) living at rescue centers: Forming new bonds. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(4), 423–431.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gaunet, F., Deldalle, S. (2014). Effects of 2 training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog–owner relationship. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 9, 58–65.CrossRef Gaunet, F., Deldalle, S. (2014). Effects of 2 training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog–owner relationship. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 9, 58–65.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gaunet, F., & El Massioui, F. (2014). Marked referential communicative behaviours, but no differentiation of the “knowledge state” of humans in untrained pet dogs versus 1-year-old infants. Animal Cognition, 17(5), 1137–1147.CrossRefPubMed Gaunet, F., & El Massioui, F. (2014). Marked referential communicative behaviours, but no differentiation of the “knowledge state” of humans in untrained pet dogs versus 1-year-old infants. Animal Cognition, 17(5), 1137–1147.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gottlieb, G. (2002). Individual development & evolution. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Gottlieb, G. (2002). Individual development & evolution. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Gunnarsson, T. G., Gill, J. A., Sigurbjörnsson, T., & Sutherland, W. J. (2004). Arrival synchrony in migratory birds. Nature, 431, 646.CrossRefPubMed Gunnarsson, T. G., Gill, J. A., Sigurbjörnsson, T., & Sutherland, W. J. (2004). Arrival synchrony in migratory birds. Nature, 431, 646.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 155–186.CrossRefPubMed Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 155–186.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439–444.CrossRef Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439–444.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 298(5598), 1634–1636.CrossRef Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 298(5598), 1634–1636.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hare, B., Plyusnina, I., Ignacio, N., Schepina, O., Stepika, A., Wrangham, R., & Trut, L. (2005). Social cognitive evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by-product of experimental domestication. Current Biology, 15(3), 226–230.CrossRefPubMed Hare, B., Plyusnina, I., Ignacio, N., Schepina, O., Stepika, A., Wrangham, R., & Trut, L. (2005). Social cognitive evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by-product of experimental domestication. Current Biology, 15(3), 226–230.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kaminski, J., Schulz, L., & Tomasello, M. (2012). How dogs know when communication is intended for them? Developmental Science, 15(2), 222–232.CrossRefPubMed Kaminski, J., Schulz, L., & Tomasello, M. (2012). How dogs know when communication is intended for them? Developmental Science, 15(2), 222–232.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat King, A. J., & Cowlishaw, G. (2009). All together now: Behavioural synchrony in baboons. Animal Behavior, 78, 1381–1387.CrossRef King, A. J., & Cowlishaw, G. (2009). All together now: Behavioural synchrony in baboons. Animal Behavior, 78, 1381–1387.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat King, T., Marston, L. C., & Bennett, P. C. (2012). Breeding dogs for beauty and behaviour: why scientists need to do more to develop valid and reliable behaviour assessments for dogs kept as companions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 137, 1–12.CrossRef King, T., Marston, L. C., & Bennett, P. C. (2012). Breeding dogs for beauty and behaviour: why scientists need to do more to develop valid and reliable behaviour assessments for dogs kept as companions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 137, 1–12.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 354–364.CrossRef Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 354–364.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kis, Á., Bence, M., Lakatos, G., Pergel, E., Turcsán, B., Pluijmakers, J., . . . Kubinyi, E. (2014). Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms are associated with human directed social behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris). PLOS ONE, 9, e83993.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kis, Á., Bence, M., Lakatos, G., Pergel, E., Turcsán, B., Pluijmakers, J., . . . Kubinyi, E. (2014). Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms are associated with human directed social behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris). PLOS ONE, 9, e83993.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Social referencing: The infant’s use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22, 427–432.CrossRef Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Social referencing: The infant’s use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22, 427–432.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kubinyi, E., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2003). Social mimetic behaviour and social anticipation in dogs: Preliminary results. Animal Cognition, 6, 57–63.CrossRefPubMed Kubinyi, E., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2003). Social mimetic behaviour and social anticipation in dogs: Preliminary results. Animal Cognition, 6, 57–63.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., &Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 145–162.CrossRef Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., &Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 145–162.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R., Bard, E. G., & Jeuniaux, P. (2012). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cognitive Science, 36, 1–23.CrossRef Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R., Bard, E. G., & Jeuniaux, P. (2012). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cognitive Science, 36, 1–23.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat MacLean, E. L., Herrmann, E., Suchindra, S., & Hare, B. (2017). Individual differences in cooperative communicative skills are more similar between dogs and humans than chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 126, 41–51.CrossRef MacLean, E. L., Herrmann, E., Suchindra, S., & Hare, B. (2017). Individual differences in cooperative communicative skills are more similar between dogs and humans than chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 126, 41–51.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Marshall-Pescini, S., Dale, R., Quervel-Chaumette, Q., & Range, F. (2016). Critical issues in experimental studies of prosociality in non-human species. Animal Cognition, 19(4), 679–705.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Marshall-Pescini, S., Dale, R., Quervel-Chaumette, Q., & Range, F. (2016). Critical issues in experimental studies of prosociality in non-human species. Animal Cognition, 19(4), 679–705.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Mehrkam, L. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2014). Behavioral differences among breeds of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris): Current status of the science. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 155, 12–27.CrossRef Mehrkam, L. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2014). Behavioral differences among breeds of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris): Current status of the science. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 155, 12–27.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Merola, I., Marshall-Pescini, S., D’Aniello, B., & Prato-Previde, E. (2013). Social referencing: Water rescue trained dogs are less affected than pet dogs by the stranger’s message. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 147, 132–138.CrossRef Merola, I., Marshall-Pescini, S., D’Aniello, B., & Prato-Previde, E. (2013). Social referencing: Water rescue trained dogs are less affected than pet dogs by the stranger’s message. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 147, 132–138.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., Lazzaroni, M., & Marshall-Peschini, S. (2014). Dogs’ comprehension of referential emotional expressions: Familiar people and familiar emotions are easier. Animal Cognition, 17, 373–385.CrossRefPubMed Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., Lazzaroni, M., & Marshall-Peschini, S. (2014). Dogs’ comprehension of referential emotional expressions: Familiar people and familiar emotions are easier. Animal Cognition, 17, 373–385.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2012a). Social referencing in dog owner dyads? Animal Cognition, 15, 175–185.CrossRefPubMed Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2012a). Social referencing in dog owner dyads? Animal Cognition, 15, 175–185.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Miklósi, Á, Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (1998). Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Animal Cognition, 1, 113–121.CrossRef Miklósi, Á, Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (1998). Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Animal Cognition, 1, 113–121.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13–20.CrossRef Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13–20.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mumme, D. L., Fenald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infants’ responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Development, 67, 3219–3237.CrossRefPubMed Mumme, D. L., Fenald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infants’ responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Development, 67, 3219–3237.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nagasawa, M., Mitsui, S., En, S., Ohtani, N., Ohta, M., Sakuma, Y., . . . Kikusui, T. (2015). Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science, 348(6232), 333–336.CrossRefPubMed Nagasawa, M., Mitsui, S., En, S., Ohtani, N., Ohta, M., Sakuma, Y., . . . Kikusui, T. (2015). Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science, 348(6232), 333–336.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Oullier, O., & Scott Kelso, J. A. (2009). Coordination from the perspective of social coordination dynamics. In R. A. Meyer (Ed.), The encyclopedia of complexity and systems science (pp. 8198–8213). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.CrossRef Oullier, O., & Scott Kelso, J. A. (2009). Coordination from the perspective of social coordination dynamics. In R. A. Meyer (Ed.), The encyclopedia of complexity and systems science (pp. 8198–8213). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Paukner, A., Suomi, S. J., Visalberghi, E., & Ferrari, P. F. (2009). Capuchin monkeys display affiliation towards humans who imitate them. Science, 325(5942), 880–883.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Paukner, A., Suomi, S. J., Visalberghi, E., & Ferrari, P. F. (2009). Capuchin monkeys display affiliation towards humans who imitate them. Science, 325(5942), 880–883.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Protopopova, S., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2014). Adopter-dog interactions at the shelter: Behavioral and contextual predictors of adoption. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 157, 109–116.CrossRef Protopopova, S., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2014). Adopter-dog interactions at the shelter: Behavioral and contextual predictors of adoption. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 157, 109–116.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Quervel-Chaumette, M., Dale, R., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Range, F. (2016a). Familiarity affects other-regarding preferences in pet dogs. Scientific Reports, 5, 18102.CrossRef Quervel-Chaumette, M., Dale, R., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Range, F. (2016a). Familiarity affects other-regarding preferences in pet dogs. Scientific Reports, 5, 18102.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Quervel-Chaumette, M., Mainix, G., Range, F., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2016b). Dogs do not show pro-social preferences towards humans. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1416.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Quervel-Chaumette, M., Mainix, G., Range, F., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2016b). Dogs do not show pro-social preferences towards humans. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1416.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., Shockley, K. (2008). Synchrony and swing in conversation: Coordination, temporal dynamics, and communication. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., Shockley, K. (2008). Synchrony and swing in conversation: Coordination, temporal dynamics, and communication. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science, 26, 867–891.CrossRefPubMed Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science, 26, 867–891.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Ross, M. D., & Menzler, S. (2008). Rapid facial mimicry in orangutan play. Biology Letters, 4, 27–30.CrossRef Ross, M. D., & Menzler, S. (2008). Rapid facial mimicry in orangutan play. Biology Letters, 4, 27–30.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sakai, M., Morisaka, T., Kogi, K., Hishii, T., & Kohshima, S. (2010). Fine-scale analysis of synchronous breathing in wild Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus). Behavioural Processes, 83, 48–53.CrossRefPubMed Sakai, M., Morisaka, T., Kogi, K., Hishii, T., & Kohshima, S. (2010). Fine-scale analysis of synchronous breathing in wild Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus). Behavioural Processes, 83, 48–53.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Savalli, C., Ades, C., & Gaunet, F. (2014). Are dogs able to communicate with their owners about a desirable food in a referential and intentional way? PLOS ONE, 9(9), e108003.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Savalli, C., Ades, C., & Gaunet, F. (2014). Are dogs able to communicate with their owners about a desirable food in a referential and intentional way? PLOS ONE, 9(9), e108003.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Starling, M. J., Branson, N., Thomson, P. C., & McGreevy, P. D. (2013). Age, sex and reproductive status affect boldness in dogs. The Veterinary Journal, 197, 868–872.CrossRefPubMed Starling, M. J., Branson, N., Thomson, P. C., & McGreevy, P. D. (2013). Age, sex and reproductive status affect boldness in dogs. The Veterinary Journal, 197, 868–872.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Stupacher, J., Maes, P.-J., Witte, M., & Wood, G. (2017). Music strengthens prosocial effects of interpersonal synchronization: If you move in time with the beat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 39–44.CrossRef Stupacher, J., Maes, P.-J., Witte, M., & Wood, G. (2017). Music strengthens prosocial effects of interpersonal synchronization: If you move in time with the beat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 39–44.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Svartberg, K. (2006). Breed-typical behaviour in dogs—Historical remnants or recent constructs? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 96, 293–313.CrossRef Svartberg, K. (2006). Breed-typical behaviour in dogs—Historical remnants or recent constructs? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 96, 293–313.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Svartberg, K., & Forkman, B. (2002). Personality traits in the domestic dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 79, 133–155.CrossRef Svartberg, K., & Forkman, B. (2002). Personality traits in the domestic dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 79, 133–155.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Csányi, V., & Dóka, A. (1998). Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): A new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) Strange Situation Test. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112(3), 219–229.CrossRefPubMed Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Csányi, V., & Dóka, A. (1998). Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): A new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) Strange Situation Test. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112(3), 219–229.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Townsend, S. W., Koski, S. E., Byrne, R. W., Slocombe, K. E., Bickel, B., Boeckle, M., . . . Glock, H. J. (2017). Exorcising Grice’s ghost: An empirical approach to studying intentional communication in animals. Biological Reviews, 92(3), 1427–1433.CrossRefPubMed Townsend, S. W., Koski, S. E., Byrne, R. W., Slocombe, K. E., Bickel, B., Boeckle, M., . . . Glock, H. J. (2017). Exorcising Grice’s ghost: An empirical approach to studying intentional communication in animals. Biological Reviews, 92(3), 1427–1433.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Turcsán, B., Kubinyi, E., & Miklósi, Á. (2011). Trainability and boldness traits differ between dog breed clusters based on conventional breed categories and genetic relatedness. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 132, 61–70.CrossRef Turcsán, B., Kubinyi, E., & Miklósi, Á. (2011). Trainability and boldness traits differ between dog breed clusters based on conventional breed categories and genetic relatedness. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 132, 61–70.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Udell, M. A., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. (2010). What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions. Biological Reviews, 85, 327–345.CrossRefPubMed Udell, M. A., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. (2010). What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions. Biological Reviews, 85, 327–345.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., Wynne, C. D. L. (2012). Interspecies social learning in dogs: The inextricable roles of phylogeny and ontogeny. In T. R. Zentall & E. A. Wasserman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of comparative cognition (2nd ed., pp. 819–831). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., Wynne, C. D. L. (2012). Interspecies social learning in dogs: The inextricable roles of phylogeny and ontogeny. In T. R. Zentall & E. A. Wasserman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of comparative cognition (2nd ed., pp. 819–831). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Udell, M. A. R., Lord, K., Feuerbacher, E. N., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2014). A dog’s eye view of canine cognition. In A. Horowitz (Ed.), Domestic dog cognition and behavior: The scientific study of Canis familiaris (pp. 221–240). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Academic Press.CrossRef Udell, M. A. R., Lord, K., Feuerbacher, E. N., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2014). A dog’s eye view of canine cognition. In A. Horowitz (Ed.), Domestic dog cognition and behavior: The scientific study of Canis familiaris (pp. 221–240). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Academic Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Udell, M. A. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2010). Ontogeny and phylogeny: Both are essential to human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis. Animal Behavior, 79, e9–e14.CrossRef Udell, M. A. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2010). Ontogeny and phylogeny: Both are essential to human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis. Animal Behavior, 79, e9–e14.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 693–695.CrossRef Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 693–695.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat van Ulzen, N. R., Lamoth, C. J. C., Daffertshofer, A., Semin, R., & Beek, P. J. (2008). Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neuroscience Letters, 432, 88–93.CrossRefPubMed van Ulzen, N. R., Lamoth, C. J. C., Daffertshofer, A., Semin, R., & Beek, P. J. (2008). Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neuroscience Letters, 432, 88–93.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wynne, C. D. L. (2016). What is special about dog cognition?. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(5), 345–350.CrossRef Wynne, C. D. L. (2016). What is special about dog cognition?. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(5), 345–350.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wynne, C. D. L., Udell, M. A. R., & Lord, K. A. (2008). Ontogeny’s impacts on human-dog communication. Animal Behavior, 76, e1–e4.CrossRef Wynne, C. D. L., Udell, M. A. R., & Lord, K. A. (2008). Ontogeny’s impacts on human-dog communication. Animal Behavior, 76, e1–e4.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Xavier, J., Tilmont, E., & Bonnot, O. (2013). Children’s synchrony and rhythmicity in imitation of peers: Toward a developmental model of empathy. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 107(4), 291–297.CrossRefPubMed Xavier, J., Tilmont, E., & Bonnot, O. (2013). Children’s synchrony and rhythmicity in imitation of peers: Toward a developmental model of empathy. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 107(4), 291–297.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Zarbatany, L., & Lamb, M. E. (1985). Social referencing as a function of information source: Mothers versus strangers. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 25–33.CrossRef Zarbatany, L., & Lamb, M. E. (1985). Social referencing as a function of information source: Mothers versus strangers. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 25–33.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Behavioral synchronization and affiliation: Dogs exhibit human-like skills
verfasst von
Charlotte Duranton
Florence Gaunet
Publikationsdatum
30.04.2018
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Learning & Behavior / Ausgabe 4/2018
Print ISSN: 1543-4494
Elektronische ISSN: 1543-4508
DOI
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-018-0323-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2018

Learning & Behavior 4/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner