Skip to main content

2017 | OriginalPaper | Buchkapitel

22. Handlung und Wahrnehmung

verfasst von : Prof. Dr. Wilfried Kunde

Erschienen in: Allgemeine Psychologie

Verlag: Springer Berlin Heidelberg

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Zusammenfassung

Die Allgemeine Psychologie hat Wahrnehmung und Handlung lange Zeit getrennt voneinander betrachtet. Das ist zum einen das Ergebnis einer fast unvermeidlichen Arbeitsteilung unter Wissenschaftlern. Andererseits liegt es aber auch an bestimmten Modellvorstellungen, die davon ausgehen, dass Wahrnehmung und Handlungssteuerung unabhängig voneinander operieren. Wahrnehmung macht aber nur Sinn, wenn sie der Handlungskontrolle dient. Umgekehrt dienen Handlungen dazu, bestimmte Wahrnehmungen zu erzeugen. Diese Überlegungen führen zu theoretischen Konzepten, die eine strikte Trennung von Wahrnehmung und Handlung aufgeben.
Schlüsselwörter: Handlung; Stadienmodell; Reiz-Reaktions-Kompatibilität; Zwei-Pfade-Modell; Unbewusste Bahnung; Ideomotorik

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 390 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe




 

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Aglioti, S., Desouza, J. F. X., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–685.PubMedCrossRef Aglioti, S., Desouza, J. F. X., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–685.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 365–377.PubMed Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 365–377.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Ansorge, U., Kunde, W., & Kiefer, M. (2014). Unconscious vision and executive control: How unconscious processing and conscious action control interact. Consciousness and Cognition, 27(27), 268–287.PubMedCrossRef Ansorge, U., Kunde, W., & Kiefer, M. (2014). Unconscious vision and executive control: How unconscious processing and conscious action control interact. Consciousness and Cognition, 27(27), 268–287.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baars, B. J. (1993). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Baars, B. J. (1993). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Baess, P., Horváth, J., Jacobsen, T., & Schröger, E. (2011). Selective suppression of self-initiated sounds in an auditory stream: an ERP study. Psychophysiology, 48, 1276–1283.PubMedCrossRef Baess, P., Horváth, J., Jacobsen, T., & Schröger, E. (2011). Selective suppression of self-initiated sounds in an auditory stream: an ERP study. Psychophysiology, 48, 1276–1283.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(4), 1076–1096.PubMed Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(4), 1076–1096.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D., & Frith, C. (2000). Why can’t you tickle yourself. Neuroreport, 11, 11–16.CrossRef Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D., & Frith, C. (2000). Why can’t you tickle yourself. Neuroreport, 11, 11–16.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta psychologica, 106(1), 3–22.PubMedCrossRef Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta psychologica, 106(1), 3–22.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bruno, N., & Franz, V. H. (2009). When is grasping affected by the Müller-Lyer illusion? A quantitative review. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1421–1433.PubMedCrossRef Bruno, N., & Franz, V. H. (2009). When is grasping affected by the Müller-Lyer illusion? A quantitative review. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1421–1433.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cardoso-Leite, P., Mamassian, P., Schutz-Bosbach, S., & Waszak, F. (2010). A New Look at Sensory Attenuation: Action-Effect Anticipation Affects Sensitivity, Not Response Bias. Psychological Science, 21(12), 1740–1745.PubMedCrossRef Cardoso-Leite, P., Mamassian, P., Schutz-Bosbach, S., & Waszak, F. (2010). A New Look at Sensory Attenuation: Action-Effect Anticipation Affects Sensitivity, Not Response Bias. Psychological Science, 21(12), 1740–1745.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chen, J., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Response-effect compatibility defines the natural scrolling direction. Human Factors, 55, 1112–1129.PubMedCrossRef Chen, J., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Response-effect compatibility defines the natural scrolling direction. Human Factors, 55, 1112–1129.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Daniel, D. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston, MA: Little Brown. Daniel, D. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston, MA: Little Brown.
Zurück zum Zitat Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec, H. G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597–600.PubMedCrossRef Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec, H. G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597–600.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little, Brown. Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little, Brown.
Zurück zum Zitat Desantis, A., Roussel, C., & Waszak, F. (2014). The temporal dynamics of the perceptual consequences of action-effect prediction. Cognition, 132(3), 243–250.PubMedCrossRef Desantis, A., Roussel, C., & Waszak, F. (2014). The temporal dynamics of the perceptual consequences of action-effect prediction. Cognition, 132(3), 243–250.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Descartes, R. (1664). Über den Menschen.Lateinische Erstausgabe. Lugdunum Batavorum. Descartes, R. (1664). Über den Menschen.Lateinische Erstausgabe. Lugdunum Batavorum.
Zurück zum Zitat Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Eder, A. B., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Representing the hyphen in action-effect associations: Automatic acquisition and bidirectional retrieval of action-effect intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 40, 1701. Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Eder, A. B., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Representing the hyphen in action-effect associations: Automatic acquisition and bidirectional retrieval of action-effect intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 40, 1701.
Zurück zum Zitat Donders, F. C. (1868). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431.CrossRef Donders, F. C. (1868). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Eder, A. B., Rothermund, K., De Houwer, J., & Hommel, B. (2014). Directive and incentive functions of affective action consequences: An ideomotor approach. Psychological Research, 79, 630.PubMedCrossRef Eder, A. B., Rothermund, K., De Houwer, J., & Hommel, B. (2014). Directive and incentive functions of affective action consequences: An ideomotor approach. Psychological Research, 79, 630.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Effenberg, A. O., & Mechling, H. (1998). Bewegung hörbar machen – Warum? Zur Perspektive einer systematischen Umsetzung von Bewegung in Klänge. Psychologie und Sport, 5(1), 29–38. Effenberg, A. O., & Mechling, H. (1998). Bewegung hörbar machen – Warum? Zur Perspektive einer systematischen Umsetzung von Bewegung in Klänge. Psychologie und Sport, 5(1), 29–38.
Zurück zum Zitat Elsner, & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(1), 229–240.PubMed Elsner, & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(1), 229–240.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Firestone, C. (2013). How „paternalistic“ is spatial perception? Why wearing a heavy backpack doesn’t – and couldn’t – make hills look steeper. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 455–473.PubMedCrossRef Firestone, C. (2013). How „paternalistic“ is spatial perception? Why wearing a heavy backpack doesn’t – and couldn’t – make hills look steeper. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 455–473.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.PubMedCrossRef Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ganel, T., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape. Nature, 426, 664–667.PubMedCrossRef Ganel, T., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape. Nature, 426, 664–667.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goldstein, B. (1997). Wahrnehmungspsychologie. Heidelberg: Spektrum. Goldstein, B. (1997). Wahrnehmungspsychologie. Heidelberg: Spektrum.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20–25.CrossRef Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20–25.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S., & Carey, D. P. (1991). A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature, 349(6305), 154–156.PubMedCrossRef Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S., & Carey, D. P. (1991). A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature, 349(6305), 154–156.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control – With special reference to ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMedCrossRef Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control – With special reference to ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38(1861), 50–73. Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38(1861), 50–73.
Zurück zum Zitat Hickok, G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(7), 1229–1243.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hickok, G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(7), 1229–1243.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis: Die Funktion von Antizipationen in der menschlichen Verhaltenssteuerung und Wahrnehmung. Göttingen: Hogrefe. Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis: Die Funktion von Antizipationen in der menschlichen Verhaltenssteuerung und Wahrnehmung. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Zurück zum Zitat Holst, E. von, & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464–476.CrossRef Holst, E. von, & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464–476.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2010). Does dorsal processing require central capacity? More evidence from the PRP paradigm. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 89–100.PubMedCrossRef Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2010). Does dorsal processing require central capacity? More evidence from the PRP paradigm. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 89–100.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2012). Visual processing for action resists similarity of relevant and irrelevant object features. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19, 412–417.PubMedCrossRef Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2012). Visual processing for action resists similarity of relevant and irrelevant object features. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19, 412–417.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Janczyk, M., Skirde, S., Weigelt, M., & Kunde, W. (2009). Visual and tactile action effects determine bimanual coordination performance. Human Movement Science, 28, 437–449.PubMedCrossRef Janczyk, M., Skirde, S., Weigelt, M., & Kunde, W. (2009). Visual and tactile action effects determine bimanual coordination performance. Human Movement Science, 28, 437–449.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Wallmeier, G., & Kunde, W. (2014). Exceptions to the PRP effect? A comparison of prepared and unconditioned reflexes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 776–786.PubMed Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Wallmeier, G., & Kunde, W. (2014). Exceptions to the PRP effect? A comparison of prepared and unconditioned reflexes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 776–786.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kirsch, & Kunde, W. (2013). Visual near space is scaled to parameters of current action plans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1313–1325.PubMed Kirsch, & Kunde, W. (2013). Visual near space is scaled to parameters of current action plans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1313–1325.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kirsch, W., Königstein, E., & Kunde, W. (2014). Action feedback affects the perception of action-related objects beyond actual action success. Frontiers in Movement Science and Sport Psychology, 5, 17. Kirsch, W., Königstein, E., & Kunde, W. (2014). Action feedback affects the perception of action-related objects beyond actual action success. Frontiers in Movement Science and Sport Psychology, 5, 17.
Zurück zum Zitat Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297(5582), 846–848.PubMedCrossRef Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297(5582), 846–848.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus – response compatibility – A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMedCrossRef Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus – response compatibility – A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Külpe, O. (1893). Grundriss der Psychologie, auf experimenteller Grundlage dargestellt. Leipzig: W. Engelmann. Külpe, O. (1893). Grundriss der Psychologie, auf experimenteller Grundlage dargestellt. Leipzig: W. Engelmann.
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W. (2004). Response priming by supraliminal and subliminal action effects. Psychological Research, 68(2–3), 91–96.PubMedCrossRef Kunde, W. (2004). Response priming by supraliminal and subliminal action effects. Psychological Research, 68(2–3), 91–96.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W. (2006). Antezedente Effektrepräsentationen in der Verhaltenssteuerung. Psychologische Rundschau, 57(1), 34–42.CrossRef Kunde, W. (2006). Antezedente Effektrepräsentationen in der Verhaltenssteuerung. Psychologische Rundschau, 57(1), 34–42.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W., & Stöcker, C. (2002). A Simon effect for stimulus – response duration. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 581–592.CrossRef Kunde, W., & Stöcker, C. (2002). A Simon effect for stimulus – response duration. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 581–592.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W., & Wühr, P. (2004). Actions blind to conceptually overlapping stimuli. Psychological Research, 68(4), 199–207.PubMedCrossRef Kunde, W., & Wühr, P. (2004). Actions blind to conceptually overlapping stimuli. Psychological Research, 68(4), 199–207.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W., Hoffmann, J., & Zellmann, P. (2002). The impact of anticipated action effects on action planning. Acta Psychologica, 109(2), 137–155.PubMedCrossRef Kunde, W., Hoffmann, J., & Zellmann, P. (2002). The impact of anticipated action effects on action planning. Acta Psychologica, 109(2), 137–155.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W., Elsner, K., & Kiesel, A. (2007). No anticipation – no action: the role of anticipation in action and perception. Cognitive Processing, 8, 71–78.PubMedCrossRef Kunde, W., Elsner, K., & Kiesel, A. (2007). No anticipation – no action: the role of anticipation in action and perception. Cognitive Processing, 8, 71–78.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation, and execution of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 57(1), 87–106.CrossRef Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation, and execution of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 57(1), 87–106.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W., Skirde, S., & Weigelt, M. (2011). Trust my face: Cognitive factors of head fakes in sports. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 110–127.PubMed Kunde, W., Skirde, S., & Weigelt, M. (2011). Trust my face: Cognitive factors of head fakes in sports. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 110–127.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W., Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). The locus of tool-transformation costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 703–714.PubMedCrossRef Kunde, W., Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). The locus of tool-transformation costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 703–714.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Liberman, A. M., & Mattingly, I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21(1), 1–36.PubMedCrossRef Liberman, A. M., & Mattingly, I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21(1), 1–36.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Linser, K., & Goschke, T. (2007). Unconscious modulation of the conscious experience of voluntary control. Cognition, 104(3), 459–475.PubMedCrossRef Linser, K., & Goschke, T. (2007). Unconscious modulation of the conscious experience of voluntary control. Cognition, 104(3), 459–475.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Martikainen, M. H., Kaneko, K. I., & Hari, R. (2005). Suppressed responses to self-triggered sounds in the human auditory cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 15(3), 299–302.PubMedCrossRef Martikainen, M. H., Kaneko, K. I., & Hari, R. (2005). Suppressed responses to self-triggered sounds in the human auditory cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 15(3), 299–302.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mattes, S., Leuthold, H., & Ulrich, R. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility in intensity-force relations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1175–1191.CrossRef Mattes, S., Leuthold, H., & Ulrich, R. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility in intensity-force relations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1175–1191.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.PubMedCrossRef Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, J. (2006). Simon congruency effects based on stimulus and response numerosity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(2), 387–396.PubMedCrossRef Miller, J. (2006). Simon congruency effects based on stimulus and response numerosity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(2), 387–396.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. New York: Oxford University Press. Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psycholology. Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.PubMedCrossRef Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psycholology. Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J., Kunde, W., Gausepohl, D., & Heuer, H. (2008). Does a tool eliminate spatial compatibility effects? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 211–231.CrossRef Müsseler, J., Kunde, W., Gausepohl, D., & Heuer, H. (2008). Does a tool eliminate spatial compatibility effects? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 211–231.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton.
Zurück zum Zitat Neumann, O. (1990). Direct parameter specification and the concept of perception. Psychological Research, 52, 207–215.PubMedCrossRef Neumann, O. (1990). Direct parameter specification and the concept of perception. Psychological Research, 52, 207–215.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Noton, D., & Stark, L. (1971). Scanpaths in eye movements during pattern perception. Science, 171(3968), 308–311.PubMedCrossRef Noton, D., & Stark, L. (1971). Scanpaths in eye movements during pattern perception. Science, 171(3968), 308–311.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Paelecke, M., & Kunde, W. (2007). Action-effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 627–644.PubMedCrossRef Paelecke, M., & Kunde, W. (2007). Action-effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 627–644.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pellegrino di, Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176–180.PubMedCrossRef Pellegrino di, Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176–180.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). Harleß’apparatus of will: 150 years later. Psychological Research, 76(5), 561–565.PubMedCrossRef Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). Harleß’apparatus of will: 150 years later. Psychological Research, 76(5), 561–565.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2013). Dissecting the response in response-effect compatibility. Experimental Brain Research, 224(4), 647–655.PubMedCrossRef Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2013). Dissecting the response in response-effect compatibility. Experimental Brain Research, 224(4), 647–655.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pfister, R., Dignath, D., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2013). It Takes Two to Imitate Anticipation and Imitation in Social Interaction. Psychological Science, 24(10), 2117–2121.PubMedCrossRef Pfister, R., Dignath, D., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2013). It Takes Two to Imitate Anticipation and Imitation in Social Interaction. Psychological Science, 24(10), 2117–2121.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pfister, R., Janczyk, M., Gressmann, M., Fournier, L. R., & Kunde, W. (2014a). Good vibrations? Vibrotactile self-stimulation reveals anticipation of body-related action effects in motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 232(3), 847–854.PubMedCrossRef Pfister, R., Janczyk, M., Gressmann, M., Fournier, L. R., & Kunde, W. (2014a). Good vibrations? Vibrotactile self-stimulation reveals anticipation of body-related action effects in motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 232(3), 847–854.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pfister, R., Pfeuffer, C. U., & Kunde, W. (2014b). Perceiving by proxy: Effect-based action control with unperceivable effects. Cognition, 132(3), 251–261.PubMedCrossRef Pfister, R., Pfeuffer, C. U., & Kunde, W. (2014b). Perceiving by proxy: Effect-based action control with unperceivable effects. Cognition, 132(3), 251–261.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Poulet, J. F., & Hedwig, B. (2002). A corollary discharge maintains auditory sensitivity during sound production. Nature, 418, 872–876.PubMedCrossRef Poulet, J. F., & Hedwig, B. (2002). A corollary discharge maintains auditory sensitivity during sound production. Nature, 418, 872–876.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Prinz, W. (1990). A common-coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann, & W. Prinz (Hrsg.), Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches (S. 167–201). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1990). A common-coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann, & W. Prinz (Hrsg.), Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches (S. 167–201). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Prinz, W. (1998). Die Reaktion als Willenshandlung. Psychologische Rundschau, 49, 10–20. Prinz, W. (1998). Die Reaktion als Willenshandlung. Psychologische Rundschau, 49, 10–20.
Zurück zum Zitat Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661–670.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661–670.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sanders, A. F. (1980). Stage analysis of reaction processes. Tutorials in motor behavior, 331–354. Sanders, A. F. (1980). Stage analysis of reaction processes. Tutorials in motor behavior, 331–354.
Zurück zum Zitat Sato, A. (2008). Action observation modulates auditory perception of the consequence of others’ actions. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(4), 1219–1227.PubMedCrossRef Sato, A. (2008). Action observation modulates auditory perception of the consequence of others’ actions. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(4), 1219–1227.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sato, A., & Itakura, S. (2013). Intersubjective action-effect binding: Eye contact modulates acquisition of bidirectional association between our and others’ actions. Cognition, 127(3), 383–390.PubMedCrossRef Sato, A., & Itakura, S. (2013). Intersubjective action-effect binding: Eye contact modulates acquisition of bidirectional association between our and others’ actions. Cognition, 127(3), 383–390.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 943–974.PubMedCrossRef Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 943–974.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Spijkers, W., Heuer, H., Kleinsorge, T., & Loo, H. van der (1997). Preparation of bimanual movements with same and different amplitudes: Specification interference as revealed by reaction time. Acta Psychologica, 96(3), 207–227.CrossRef Spijkers, W., Heuer, H., Kleinsorge, T., & Loo, H. van der (1997). Preparation of bimanual movements with same and different amplitudes: Specification interference as revealed by reaction time. Acta Psychologica, 96(3), 207–227.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stock, A., & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research, 68(2–3), 176–188.PubMedCrossRef Stock, A., & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research, 68(2–3), 176–188.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor, J. E. T., Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2011). When walls are no longer barriers: Perception of wall height in parkour. Perception-London, 40(6), 757–780.PubMedCrossRef Taylor, J. E. T., Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2011). When walls are no longer barriers: Perception of wall height in parkour. Perception-London, 40(6), 757–780.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Thomaschke, R., Hopkins, B., & Miall, R. C. (2012). The planning and control model (PCM) of motorvisual priming: reconciling motorvisual impairment and facilitation effects. Psychological Review, 119(2), 388–407.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Thomaschke, R., Hopkins, B., & Miall, R. C. (2012). The planning and control model (PCM) of motorvisual priming: reconciling motorvisual impairment and facilitation effects. Psychological Review, 119(2), 388–407.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Topolinski, S. (2010). Moving the Eye of the Beholder Motor Components in Vision Determine Aesthetic Preference. Psychological Science, 21, 1220–1224.PubMedCrossRef Topolinski, S. (2010). Moving the Eye of the Beholder Motor Components in Vision Determine Aesthetic Preference. Psychological Science, 21, 1220–1224.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Valle-Inclán, F., & Redondo, M. (1998). On the automaticity of ipsilateral response activation in the Simon effect. Psychophysiology, 35(4), 366–371.PubMedCrossRef Valle-Inclán, F., & Redondo, M. (1998). On the automaticity of ipsilateral response activation in the Simon effect. Psychophysiology, 35(4), 366–371.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Weiskrantz, L. (1986). Blindsight: A Case Study and Implications. NewYork: Oxford University Press. Weiskrantz, L. (1986). Blindsight: A Case Study and Implications. NewYork: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Wiggett, A. J., Hudson, M., Tipper, S. P., & Downing, P. E. (2011). Learning associations between action and perception: effects of incompatible training on body part and spatial priming. Brain and cognition, 76(1), 87–96.PubMedCrossRef Wiggett, A. J., Hudson, M., Tipper, S. P., & Downing, P. E. (2011). Learning associations between action and perception: effects of incompatible training on body part and spatial priming. Brain and cognition, 76(1), 87–96.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Witt, J. K. (2011). Action’s effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 201–206.CrossRef Witt, J. K. (2011). Action’s effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 201–206.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wohlschläger, A. (2000). Visual motion priming by invisible actions. Vision Research, 40, 925–930.PubMedCrossRef Wohlschläger, A. (2000). Visual motion priming by invisible actions. Vision Research, 40, 925–930.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 358, 593–602.CrossRef Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 358, 593–602.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Handlung und Wahrnehmung
verfasst von
Prof. Dr. Wilfried Kunde
Copyright-Jahr
2017
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-53898-8_22

Premium Partner