Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Happiness Studies 7/2022

Open Access 02.09.2022 | Research Paper

Does Happiness Increase in Old Age? Longitudinal Evidence from 20 European Countries

verfasst von: Christoph K. Becker, Stefan T. Trautmann

Erschienen in: Journal of Happiness Studies | Ausgabe 7/2022

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Several studies indicate that happiness follows a U-shape over the life cycle: Happiness decreases after the teenage years until reaching its nadir in middle age. A similar number of studies views the U-shape critically, stating that it is the result of the wrong controls or the wrong model. In this paper, we study the upward-pointing branch of the U-shape, tracing the happiness of European citizens 50 and older over multiple waves. Consistent with a U-shape around middle age, we find that happiness initially increases after the age of 50, but commonly stagnates afterwards and eventually reverts at high age. This pattern is generally observed irrespective of the utilized happiness measure, control variables, estimation methods, and the consideration of selection effects due to mortality. However, the strength of this pattern depends on the utilized happiness measure, control variables, and on mortality effects. The general pattern does not emerge for all countries, and is not always observed for women.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10902-022-00569-4.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

1 Introduction

Individual happiness can be gauged using various methods, for example self-reports of life satisfaction, measures of positive and negative affect or indirect measures, such as the number of antidepressants consumed. A substantial amount of work has been devoted to study how happiness measured in such ways develops over the course of the lifetime. This allows insight into how happiness evolves alongside important life events, such as changes in employment status, getting married, having children, but also ageing in general. Studies in economics often find that happiness decreases from the teenage years to middle age, only to increase afterwards (and then to fall again in very high age). This dip in middle age is referred to as the U-shape of happiness and has been reported for a variety of countries (Bell & Blanchflower, 2020; Blanchflower, 2021; Blanchflower & Graham, 2021; Blanchflower & Oswald, 2008; Blanchflower & Piper, 2021; Gerdtham & Johannesson, 2001; Gwozdz & Sousa-Poza, 2010; Stone et al., 2010). This would indicate that people experience a low point of happiness around the age of 45–50. This dip is usually found to be comparable in magnitude to events such as getting divorced or losing employment (Blanchflower, 2021; Blanchflower & Graham, 2020). Taken together, this literature gives a persuasive reason to focus on this happiness dip as a researcher or policy maker. This is reflected in the attention this literature has received outside of academic research, reflected for example in articles in the Economist (2010) or the leading German weekly newspaper Die Zeit (Novotny, 2021), and many others.
At the same time, the U-shape around middle age has been contested by numerous other studies. Critique includes using the wrong controls (Glenn, 2009; Morgan & O’Connor, 2020), the wrong statistical model (Frijters & Beatton, 2012; Kratz & Brüderl, 2021; Ulloa et al., 2013), looking only at selected countries (Deaton, 2008), neglecting sample attrition in panels caused by higher mortality among the unhappiest respondents (Hudomiet et al., 2021), and not accounting for cohort effects (Ulloa et al., 2013). This critique in turn has produced several replies, indicating that the U-shape exists, even when accounting for these critiques (Blanchflower & Graham, 2020; Blanchflower & Oswald, 2009; Clark, 2019). A further criticism is that a lot of evidence on the U-shape stems from cross-sectional data (Galambos et al., 2020; Ulloa et al., 2013), although some studies confirm the U-shape based on longitudinal data (Cheng et al., 2017; Clark & Oswald, 2006; Van Landeghem, 2012). Looking at cross-sections might produce a U-shape because events can affect disparate age groups differently. Crucially, there seems to be no clear consensus in the literature on which statistical tools should be used to estimate the relationship between age and happiness.
In this paper, we aim to add to this debate by providing an account based on a large European database. We use SHARE (Survey of Health, Age and Retirement in Europe) data, which includes people 50 and upwards. Accordingly, we study if happiness increases after middle age, the right branch of the U-shape. SHARE is a multi-wave panel; hence we add to the literature by providing further evidence for longitudinal data. We use different specifications and control sets based on previous literature to provide a detailed account of the age-happiness relation in old age for 20 European countries. Our results indicate support for a U-shape around middle age in the sense that happiness increases with age after midlife. Congruent with other studies we also find that happiness starts to deteriorate at high age (Blanchflower, 2021; Blanchflower & Graham, 2020; Gwozdz & Sousa-Poza, 2010). These results are generally robust to the specification used, as well as to using different subsets of the sample to account for country, gender, or selection effects due to mortality. Some countries do not or not clearly exhibit a positive relation between age and happiness. However, these results might in part be driven by lack of sufficient observations for the individual countries.

2 Methodology

2.1 Data

We use waves 1 to 7 of the SHARE Release 7.0.0 (Survey of Health, Age and Retirement in Europe) database (Börsch-Supan et al., 2013; Börsch-Supan, 2018a, 2018b, 2018c, 2018d, 2018e, 2018f), except for wave 3. Wave 3 of SHARE (SHARELIFE) focused solely on past life events and does not include our target variables. SHARE is a database intended to be used to study the effects of ageing over the life-course of European citizens aged 50 and older, managed by the Munich Center for the Economics of Aging, Max Planck Institute for Social Law and Social Policy. The cross-national panel database provides extensive data on health and socio-economic status. We merge data over the above-mentioned six waves in order to track respondents over the course of the different interviews. Respondents over the age of 80 were dropped due to small sample size. In total, the merged data set has 139,116 individual observations. Theses waves interviewed the respondents from 2004 to 2017, spanning 13 years and 20 countries. During this time some participants left the study (due to death or other reasons), while others joined (especially because later waves include additional countries).

2.2 Measuring Happiness

Measuring happiness, well-being or life satisfaction is crucial to our research question. How happy, well or satisfied people are with their life can depend on multiple domains, such as employment, relationships, physical and mental health, financial situation or the fulfillment of goals and desires (Easterlin, 1974; Frey & Stutzer, 2002). Accordingly, one can elicit broad measures of happiness (the simplest would be to ask respondents directly “How happy are you with your life?”) or measures that zoom into specific domains. While there have been attempts to provide a unified, targetable index of happiness (such as Bhutan’s Gross National Happiness or the Happy Planet Index), there is no consensus how to best measure happiness. In our study, we utilize three measures to map respondents’ well-being: a simple single-item question regarding life satisfaction, the CASP-12 multi-item quality of life scale; and the EURO-D depressive symptoms scale. In the following, we discuss the three measures in more detail.
Our first measure, life satisfaction, measures a general, subjective feeling about the quality of life. It is extracted by a single-item question in which respondents indicate on a scale from 0 (low satisfaction) to 10 (high satisfaction) how satisfied they are with their life. This scale has acceptable reliability and validity (Beckie & Hayduk, 1997; Pavot & Diener, 1993) and relates meaningfully to various health and psychosocial measures (Kim et al., 2021).
Second, the CASP-12, a quality of life scale, which is designed to capture quality of life in old age (Hyde et al., 2003). Participants indicate for twelve statements whether they apply on a scale from 1 (often) to 4 (never). The twelve questions concern four dimensions of quality of life, control, autonomy, pleasure, and self-realization, resulting in an aggregate index ranging from 12 (low quality of life) to 48 (high quality of life). Hence, the CASP-12 relates more closely to affective measures or to the concept of eudemonia, where happiness follows from activity and control over one’s life (see Aristoteles’ Nicomachean Ethics, e.g. in Ameriks & Clarke, 2000). We normalize it such that it ranges from 0 (low quality of life) to 10 (high quality of life).
Our third measure is the EURO-D depression score (Prince et al. 1999b), which was designed to capture depressive symptoms among older people. It has been demonstrated to provide a valid comparison of depressive symptoms across European countries (Castro-Costa et al., 2008; Prince et al., 1999a). The EURO-D depression score is generated from questions on 12 dimensions: Depression, pessimism, suicidality, guilt, sleep, interest, irritability, appetite, fatigue, concentration, enjoyment, and tearfulness. The answers to these questions result in an aggregate index ranging from 0 (not depressed) to 12 (very depressed). We normalize it such that it ranges from 0 (very depressed) to 10 (not depressed) and call it lack of depressive symptoms, such that higher values of this index are comparable to higher values in the other two measures.
Table 8 in the appendix provides an overview of the specific questions asked for these three measures. In the following sections, we address these three measures collectively as measures of happiness, unless specified otherwise.

2.3 Controls

Different events and choices in a person’s life can influence the experienced level of happiness and life satisfaction (such as marrying, finding a better job, becoming a parent). If one wants to isolate the pure effect of ageing on happiness, one might want to control for such factors. On the other hand, these events are an inherent part of ageing. For example, many people become parents neither early nor very late in life. Controlling for such life events might thus lead to underestimating how happiness changes over the life course. If most of the important life events of a respondent are controlled for in their own variables, the effect of age is bound to become insignificant. As of yet, there appears to be no general agreement which set of controls should be included when analyzing happiness and life satisfaction in the literature.
Easterlin and Schaeffer (1999), Hellevik (2017) and Clark (2019) stressed the importance of controlling for cohort effects. Laaksonen (2018) showed that different controls sets can influence whether one obtains a U-shape (or any other specific form) in the first place. On the other hand, Frijters and Beatton (2012) favor fixed effects models that would exclude time-invariant controls, such as the birth cohort, in order to account for unobserved heterogeneity. Finally, a number of studies (Blanchflower, 2021; Blanchflower & Oswald, 2009) have shown that the U-shape can be obtained even without using any controls at all. More importantly, if and which controls are used should depend on the underlying research question: Specifications with controls can capture the pure effect of ageing, while abstracting from life events. Specifications without controls allow to estimate the overall trajectory of happiness over the life course (Blanchflower & Graham, 2020). A middle ground between those two approaches is to just include exogenous controls, that is only factors that remain constant over time, such as birth cohort or country of origin.
In order to accommodate the above outlined approaches, we conduct our analyses in the following ways: First, without any controls. Second, using a set of completely exogenous controls including gender, birth cohort, country of origin, and whether a respondent participated in all waves of the panel. Third, using a set of controls including the exogenous controls, as well as income, health and marriage/registered partnerships. Fourth, we use a fixed effect specification. One further concern can be the presence of selection effects: If less happy respondents are more likely to die early, they might disproportionally drop out of the panel, leading to a spurious positive correlation between age and happiness. Previous studies have indicated that different measures of happiness correlate positively with life expectation (Guven & Saloumidis, 2014; Kim et al., 2021; Lee & Singh, 2020). That is, older people could be happier, simply because their unhappier contemporaries are likely to die earlier and thus drop out of the pool of respondents. We control for this in three ways: First, we test whether we find evidence for such selection effects. Second, as we find such effects to be present (see Sect. 3.2.2), we control for respondents that participated in all waves as mentioned above. This gives us a primary indication if selection effects might be present. Third, we conduct our main analysis for both, the full sample of all respondents and a subsample of respondents participating in all waves, thus excluding selection effects.
For the analyses including controls, we use the following variables from the SHARE data set as controls: Relationship status (1 if the respondent is married or in a registered partnership, 0 otherwise), gender (1 if female, 0 if male), age (of the respondent at the time of the interview), age squared, self-assessed physical health (measured on a 5-point scale from “poor” to “excellent”), and a dummy variable indicating the country of residence of the respondent to control for cultural differences. Further, we include the level of education according to the international classification of education ISCED-97 and brackets for the average monthly household income, which represent country-specific 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles of the reported household incomes from previous waves. This allows us to compare the effects of higher incomes across countries more easily. Additionally, we include a dummy variable for the birth cohort (which always covers a decade: 1930–1939, 1940–1949, etc.) and, as mentioned, a dummy variable for respondents that were present in all waves (subsequently called in all waves), to account for selection effects.

2.4 Models and Hypotheses

According to the previous sections, we estimate the following three models to test our research question. The observations of one participant in the different waves form a panel, standard errors are clustered on the level of the individual respondent.
$$M_{i,t} = \beta_{0} + \beta_{1} D^{\prime} + \gamma X^{\prime}_{i,t} + u_{i,t}$$
(1)
$$M_{i,t} = \beta_{0} + \beta_{1} Age_{i,t} + \beta_{2} Age_{i,t}^{2} + \gamma X_{i,t}^{\prime } + u_{i,t}$$
(2)
$$M_{i,t} = \beta_{1} Age_{i,t} + \beta_{2} Age_{i,t}^{2} + \gamma X_{i,t}^{\prime } + \alpha_{i} + u_{i,t}$$
(3)
Equation (1) is a pooled OLS regression using dummies for different age categories, (2) is a pooled OLS regression including terms for age and ages squared. These two models are intended as a very basic test of a possible age-happiness relation, similar to Blanchflower and Oswald (2009). Equation (3) specifies a fixed effects GLS model, which allows to eliminate unobserved heterogeneity between respondents (Frijters & Beatton, 2012).1\({M}_{i,t}\) refers to our three happiness measures, life satisfaction, the CASP-12 index, and the EURO-D lack of depressive symptoms index, respectively (for individual \(i=1,\dots ,N\) and wave \(t=1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 7\)). \(D\) is a vector of dummies quantifying age tuples starting from 52 (based on the literature of Blanchflower & Oswald, 2009, pp. 52–53, 54–55, and so on), respondents of younger age than that form the reference category (a total of 9308 observations fall in this category). \(Ag{e}_{i,t}\) and \(Age_{i,t}^{2}\) refer to the age and the squared age of respondent i at time t. \({X}_{i,t}\) is a vector of time-varying (income brackets, education and subjective health) and time-invariant (gender, birth cohort and country of origin)2 personal controls (see Sect. 2.3), \({\alpha }_{i}\) is the time-invariant personal effect of respondent i, and \({u}_{i,t}\) is an individual error term. As discussed in Sect. 2.3, models are run without controls, with only the exogenous controls, or with all controls, the latter two control sets being represented by \({X}_{i,t}\).
All three model specifications test the same underlying research question: Does happiness increase after middle age (in line with the right side of the U-shape), after which it stagnates and eventually drops at high age? As our sample includes only respondents of age 50 and upwards, these two factors would imply a hill shaped path for the three happiness measures after middle age. Or put differently, a positive coefficient for age and a negative one for age squared (as happiness tends to fall for high age). In other words, we test:
Hypothesis 1
The coefficients of the dummy variables \(\beta_{1}\) in model (1) are positive for lower ages, then close to zero and finally negative.
Hypothesis 2
The age coefficients \({\beta }_{1}\) are positive and the age-squared coefficients \({\beta }_{2}\) are negative in models (2) and (3) (implying a concave shape, which would indicate that happiness increases after middle age and drops towards the end of life).
Furthermore, we try to strengthen these hypotheses by running a series of robustness checks. First, as mentioned in Sect. 2.3, one important concern studying happiness and old age is the presence of selection effects. In order to see if this concern is well-founded in our data set, we run the following fixed effects logit models:
$$\Pr \left( {Y_{i,t} = 1|x_{i,t} } \right) = F\left( {\beta_{0} + \beta_{1} M_{i,t} + \gamma X_{i,t}^{\prime } + \alpha_{i} + u_{i,t} } \right)$$
(4)
where \(\mathrm{Pr}\left({Y}_{i,t}=1|{x}_{i,t}\right)\) is the probability that respondent i dies between wave t and wave t + 1 (Y = 1), \({M}_{i,t}\) refers again to our three happiness measures, \({X}_{i,t}\) refers to the vector of control variables, \({\alpha }_{i}\) is the time-invariant personal effect of respondent i, and \({u}_{i,t}\) is an individual error term. If more happy people (according to our measures) are indeed less likely to die, we expect \(\beta_{1}\) to be negative. As discussed in Sect. 2.3, we then take this into account for subsequent analyses. Additionally, our set of controls also contains the in all waves dummy variable. This allows us to capture any level effects caused by selection.
Second, we check whether our results differ if we perform some additional robustness checks. We run the regressions interacting the aforementioned in all waves dummy with the age and age squared variables. This provides further insight into the role of selection effects for the shape of the age-happiness relation. We also check if the age-happiness relation differs between male and female respondents, as well as between countries. Research has shown that the happiness of women and men differs (Laaksonen, 2018), and that the U-shape might be specific to some countries (Deaton, 2008). However, these control variables can only capture a level difference, not an overall different happiness-age pattern. Hence, we run our analyses again for men and women, as well as the different countries, separately.

3 Results

3.1 Summary Statistics

Table 1 provides an overview of key variables in our data set: the number of respondents per wave, percentages of female and married respondents, the average age, and our three variables on happiness and life satisfaction.3 The number of respondents increases over the waves, as further countries and more respondents were added. At the same time, other respondents dropped out of the survey due to attrition, noticeable in the drop in wave 7. Figure 1 provides an overview of the number of respondents per country. As visible, the number of respondents can vary considerably relative to the population size of the country. We account for this fact in our inferential analyses by using the sampling weights provided by SHARE.4 Figure 2 shows the share of the various birth cohorts over the different waves, indicating that e.g. most respondents in the 1930–1939 birth cohort dropped out of the survey at one point. Figure 3 shows the number of living respondents relative to those that died before the wave was conducted, giving an overview of how the sample evolved over time. Respondents that do not drop out of the survey are interviewed again in subsequent waves, which overall leads to the average age of respondents increasing slightly over the waves.
Table 1
Summary statistics of key variables
 
Wave 1
Wave 2
Wave 4
Wave 5
Wave 6
Wave 7
N
23,505
27,478
45,576
50,488
50,827
44,961
Female
53%
54%
55%
54%
55%
55%
Married
76%
76%
72%
73%
72%
71%
Age
61.08
(7.50)
61.87
(7.32)
62.82
(7.39)
63.74
(7.45)
64.77
(7.52)
66.47
(7.34)
Life satisfaction (0–10)
 
7.56
(1.77)
7.55
(1.84)
7.61
(1.80)
7.67
(1.76)
7.67
(1.77)
CASP-12 (0–10)
7.06
(1.68)
7.02
(1.71)
7.05
(1.76)
7.32
(1.69)
7.07
(1.72)
7.11
(1.73)
EURO-D (0–10)
8.12
(1.85)
8.15
(1.87)
7.92
(1.88)
8.11
(1.82)
8.05
(1.85)
8.10
(1.85)
The values in rows four to seven report means, standard deviation in brackets
Table 1 shows how the different measures for happiness and life satisfaction remained mostly stable on average over the waves. Before estimating the relationship between age and happiness, we can look at the raw answers to the different questions by age. Figure 4 shows the mean reported happiness over age pooled across all waves (see Figs. S1–S6 in the online supplement for graphs for the individual waves). As the figure indicates, happiness seems indeed to increase with age starting from a low point in middle age in the raw data, before dropping strongly at high age.

3.2 Main Analysis

3.2.1 Age and Happiness

Next, we estimate the relationship between the happiness measures and age. First, we are considering model (1), the pooled OLS model using age dummies. Figure 5 depicts the coefficients of the age dummies plotted against age for all respondents, with the left panel showing results without sampling weights and the right panel with weights. The implied happiness increases for all three measures starting with middle age, but tend to flatten or decrease in old age (the latter is a common finding in other studies, see e.g. Blanchflower & Oswald, 2008 or Deaton, 2008). Including controls makes the increase after middle age even more pronounced, with the strong dip at old age becoming much less noticeable. A majority of the coefficients for the age dummies is highly significant (at p < 0.001, see Tables S1–S6 in the online supplement for the full regressions) and follow the predicted path: Earlier age dummies are positive, while later ones are either negative or positive but smaller and ultimately not significant. An exception to this seems to be life satisfaction, once the full control set is included. As factors such as deteriorating health and changes in marriage status are accounted for, life satisfaction appears to increase over the course of life.
Importantly, the effect sizes of the dummies (ranging from close to 0 to close to 1 at maximum) are similar to the results of other studies (Blanchflower, 2021; Blanchflower & Graham, 2020; Gwozdz & Sousa-Poza, 2010). To better illustrate the effect of ageing, we can also compare the effect sizes to that of important life events, such as getting divorced, losing a job, or losing a loved one (Blanchflower, 2021; Blanchflower & Graham, 2020). In our study for example, the effect of being married or in a registered partnership contributes between 0.171 and 0.411 to the happiness measures when using the full control set. Overall, these findings indicate a positive correlation between happiness and age with a tendency to flatten or decrease at high age, providing partial evidence in support of hypothesis 1.
Result 1
The coefficients of the dummy variables \(\upbeta_{1}\) in model (1) are positive after middle age. Towards higher age they tend to become closer to zero or negative, depending on the model and control set used. Happiness increases with age but flattens of falls towards high age.
These results are corroborated by the results of both the pooled OLS (2) and the fixed effects model (3) using age and age squared variables instead of dummies. Both models indicate an increase of all three measures over age that slows down, the older the respondents are. Table 2 displays the age and age squared coefficients of the pooled OLS model, again with and without sampling weights (the full regression tables are provided in Tables S7–S12 in the online supplement5), Table 3 the ones of the fixed effects model (see Tables S19–S21 in the online supplement for the full regressions).6 As we test multiple hypotheses here on the same data set, a concern might be that the obtained significant results are suffering from multiple hypothesis testing. Tables 2 and 3 thus also display the t-statistics for the two models. As these statistics show, our results are highly significant. Furthermore, the results obtained from the fixed effects model are overall remarkably close to the ones from the pooled OLS. This would suggest, at least for our data, that using either model leads to valid results. In addition, Table 4 depicts the turning points implied by our models, i.e., the age where happiness starts to decrease. Generally, the more controls are included, the higher the turning points become. This is as expected, as controlling for changes e.g., in health or family status isolates negative shocks are more likely to occur in higher age. For CASP-12 and EURO-D this results in the turning point moving upwards, with this change never exceeding 10 years. For life satisfaction this effect is more pronounced. In fact, in the OLS models the turning point moves beyond the age range of our sample once all controls are included. Taken together, these results corroborate that the increase in happiness after middle age slows down and might ultimately turn into a decrease later in life. Overall, we thus find evidence for hypothesis 2.
Table 2
Coefficients of the pooled OLS model (2) (using cohort dummies)
 
Unweighted
Using sampling weights
No controls
Ex. controls
All controls
No controls
Ex. controls
All controls
Life satisfaction
Age
0.173***
0.118***
0.0915***
0.146***
0.122***
0.101***
(17.28)
(10.51)
(6.92)
(6.57)
(4.86)
(3.74)
Age2
 − 0.00136***
 − 0.000829***
 − 0.000526***
 − 0.00123***
 − 0.000865***
 − 0.000582**
(− 17.39)
(− 9.52)
(− 5.10)
(− 7.10)
(− 4.39)
(− 2.73)
CASP-12
Age
0.242***
0.191***
0.148***
0.250***
0.189***
0.154***
(25.45)
(19.50)
(12.17)
(12.41)
(8.31)
(6.44)
Age2
 − 0.00204***
 − 0.00152***
 − 0.00108***
 − 0.00218***
 − 0.00159***
 − 0.00116***
(− 27.55)
(− 19.85)
(− 11.45)
(− 13.99)
(− 8.91)
(− 6.22)
EURO-D
Age
0.274***
0.204***
0.138***
0.272***
0.171***
0.116***
(26.59)
(17.74)
(8.88)
(11.58)
(6.24)
(3.73)
Age2
 − 0.00225***
 − 0.00169***
 − 0.00101***
 − 0.00227***
 − 0.00141***
 − 0.000835***
(− 27.77)
(− 18.70)
(− 8.34)
(− 12.35)
(− 6.55)
(− 3.38)
t-statistics in parentheses
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Table 3
Coefficients of the fixed effects model (3)
  
No controls
All controls
Life satisfaction
Age
0.154***
0.189***
(12.79)
(10.70)
Age2
 − 0.00111***
 − 0.00129***
(− 11.80)
(− 9.50)
CASP-12
Age
0.220***
0.216***
(21.92)
(14.42)
Age2
 − 0.00172***
 − 0.00163***
(− 22.07)
(− 14.17)
EURO-D
Age
0.255***
0.272***
(19.40)
(11.42)
Age2
 − 0.00207***
 − 0.00196***
(− 20.13)
(− 10.76)
t-statistics in parentheses
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Table 4
Age turning points of the OLS model (2) (using cohort dummies) and the FE model (3)
 
OLS—Unweighted
OLS—Using sampling weights
FE
No controls
Ex. controls
All controls
No controls
Ex. controls
All controls
No controls
All controls
Life satisfaction
63.60
71.17
86.98
59.35
70.52
86.77
69.37
73.26
CASP-12
59.31
62.83
68.52
57.34
59.43
66.38
63.95
66.26
EURO-D
60.89
60.36
68.32
59.91
60.64
69.46
61.59
69.39
Result 2
The age coefficients \({\upbeta }_{1}\) are positive and significant and the age-squared coefficients \({\upbeta }_{2}\) are negative and significant in models (2) and (3). We find a concave shape for the age-happiness relation. Our results for CASP-12 and EURO-D indicate that happiness increases after middle age and drops towards the end of life. For life satisfaction, the drop becomes less pronounced as more controls are included.

3.2.2 Selection Effects

One major concern in the interpretation of the age effects shown in the previous sections is the presence of selection effects due to respondents dying depending on their happiness. Running fixed-effects logit regressions of the likelihood to die before a given wave on the different happiness measures (model 4 in Sect. 2.4), we find indeed evidence of a selection effect. The regression coefficients for the three happiness measures are all negative and significant for the CASP-12 and EURO-D lack of depressive symptoms (p < 0.05, p < 0.01 for lack depressive symptoms, see Table 10 in the appendix). The likelihood of dying before a given wave decreases by 0.000126, 0.000189 and 0.000347 percentage points for each point on the scales of life satisfaction, CASP-12, and EURO-D lack of depressive symptoms, respectively. We additionally find that respondents with better physical health status are less likely to die. In the preceding section, the full control set also included the in all waves dummy for respondents that were present in all waves. The coefficients for this dummy are positive (Life satisfaction: 0.0412 [2.38], CASP: 0.104 [5.77], Lack of depressive symptoms: 0.0724 [3.68], t-statistics in square brackets, pooled OLS regression).
However, these coefficients can only account for a level effect between respondents that took part in all waves and those that dropped out of the sample at one point. To test if selection affects the shape of the happiness-age relation, we run our analyses for the subset of respondents that participated in all waves. Note that in the latter subset we also drop respondents that did not die between the waves, but either dropped out due to other reasons, or only joined the panel during the later waves. Past studies highlighted the fact that cross-sectional studies do not follow respondents over the life cycle and might thus have limited explanatory power (Galambos et al., 2020; Hudomiet et al., 2021; Ulloa et al., 2013): Accordingly, this subset represents the most stringent subset of respondents, specifically those for which we can track happiness over all waves.
Figure 6 shows the dummy coefficients of model (1) for the no attrition subset.7 Looking at this subset, the obtained relationship between happiness and age emerges again, but loses part of its significance depending on the control set (in terms of the number of significant age dummies (see Tables S22–S27 in the online supplement for the full regression). However, these results might in part be driven by the sharp decrease in observations once controls are used in the already strict no attrition subsample.
Tables 5 and 6 depict the age and age squared coefficients of the pooled OLS and fixed effects models (see Tables S28–S36 in the online supplement for the full regressions). The estimated coefficients are all highly significant and fit our predictions. Comparing Tables 2 and 3, 4, 5 and 6 shows that the coefficients are comparable in sign and size across the full sample and the no attrition subsample. We take this as further indication that selection effects are in place, but do not account for the observed correlation between happiness and age. Taking the findings of this section together, there is clear evidence that, while selection effects play a role, they seem to matter in the form of a level effect, rather than influencing the shape of the age-happiness relation. Notably, these results differ from the recent study of Hudomiet et al. (2021), which reports a decline in subjective well-being in U.S. data, as soon as attrition due to mortality is accounted for. Overall, our results are comparable, irrespective of whether we use sampling weights, account for attrition, using fixed effects, or using no or only exogenous controls. For the following subsample analysis, we hence use pooled OLS, sampling weights and the full set of controls for simplicity.
Table 5
Coefficients of the pooled OLS model (2) (using cohort dummies), no attrition subsample
 
Unweighted
Using sampling weights
No controls
Ex. controls
All controls
No controls
Ex. controls
All controls
Life satisfaction
Age
0.177***
0.154***
0.153**
0.200***
0.192***
0.191**
(5.01)
(4.90)
(2.74)
(4.62)
(5.31)
(2.82)
Age2
 − 0.00134***
 − 0.00114***
 − 0.00104*
 − 0.00147***
 − 0.00142***
 − 0.00129**
(− 5.00)
(− 4.74)
(− 2.54)
(− 4.49)
(− 5.11)
(− 2.59)
CASP-12
Age
0.253***
0.233***
0.327***
0.305***
0.257***
0.343***
(8.10)
(9.03)
(5.83)
(8.13)
(8.40)
(4.88)
Age2
 − 0.00203***
 − 0.00173***
 − 0.00232***
 − 0.00235***
 − 0.00190***
 − 0.00240***
(− 8.36)
(− 8.68)
(− 5.61)
(− 8.12)
(− 7.97)
(− 4.66)
EURO-D
Age
0.236***
0.201***
0.149*
0.231***
0.201***
0.166*
(7.58)
(7.34)
(2.35)
(5.96)
(6.12)
(2.18)
Age2
 − 0.00193***
 − 0.00165***
 − 0.00113*
 − 0.00184***
 − 0.00163***
 − 0.00123*
(− 8.00)
(− 7.76)
(− 2.42)
(− 6.18)
(− 6.39)
(− 2.20)
t-statistics in parentheses
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Table 6
Coefficients of the fixed effects model (3), no attrition subsample
  
No controls
All controls
Life satisfaction
Age
0.171***
0.204***
(5.72)
(3.50)
Age2
 − 0.00128***
 − 0.00154***
(− 5.61)
(− 3.60)
CASP-12
Age
0.204***
0.357***
(7.83)
(6.24)
Age2
 − 0.00143***
 − 0.00250***
(− 7.13)
(− 5.95)
EURO-D
Age
0.243***
0.240***
(7.85)
(3.32)
Age2
 − 0.00197***
 − 0.00184***
(− 8.28)
(− 3.46)
t-statistics in parentheses
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
As a further robustness check, we run the weighted pooled OLS again, this time interacting the aforementioned in all waves dummy with the age and age squared variables (see Table 11 in the appendix and Table S37 in the online supplement). These interactions effects, as well as the in all waves dummy itself are in most cases insignificant. However, the coefficients for age and age squared still exhibit the same pattern in our main analysis. A notable exception is the CASP-12: Including the interaction effects here renders the in all waves dummy itself significant, but negative. The interaction effects with age and age squared are significant, and are also positive and negative, respectively. In other words, even in this exception, respondents that took part in all waves exhibit the same age-happiness pattern as in the main analysis. If anything, the pattern emerges even stronger here.

3.3 Subsample Analyses

3.3.1 Gender Differences

Looking at men and women separately, the results of the dummy regressions in Fig. 7 already indicate that the age-happiness relation follows a comparable path for both genders (see Tables S38–S39 in the online supplement for the full regressions). Table 7 shows the coefficients and t-statistics for the pooled OLS model (1) (see Tables S40–S41 in the online supplement for the full regressions). In general, the results look similar for both men and women. However, for women, the pattern is less pronounced, falling just short of reaching significance for some of the coefficients in the pooled OLS model, except for the CASP-12. We run the same regression with interaction terms (see Table 12 in the appendix and Table S42 in the online supplement). None of the interaction terms are significant, corroborating that the fundamental pattern is similar for men and women.
Table 7
Coefficients of the pooled OLS model (2), men and women
  
Men
Women
Life satisfaction
Age
0.153***
0.0681
(3.79)
(1.88)
Age2
 − 0.001000**
 − 0.000319
(− 3.16)
(− 1.12)
CASP-12
Age
0.183***
0.135***
(5.11)
(4.24)
Age2
 − 0.00138***
 − 0.00102***
(− 4.96)
(− 4.10)
EURO-D
Age
0.132**
0.0959*
(2.99)
(2.24)
Age2
 − 0.00100**
 − 0.000648
(− 2.84)
(− 1.91)
t-statistics in parentheses
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

3.3.2 Country Differences

Next, we turn to the differences between the countries of the SHARE data set. For the dummy regression plots for the 20 individual countries, see Fig. 8 in the appendix (full dummy regressions in Tables S43–S62 in the online supplement). For an overview of all age and age squared coefficients of the pooled OLS how to best measure happiness best model, see Table 13 in the appendix (full dummy regressions in Tables S63–S82 in the online supplement). Evidence from the pooled OLS model here is mixed, with some countries not observing a significant correlation between age and happiness at all (or only for some of the happiness measures used). Still, for all countries and measures for which a significant correlation is observed, the positive trend for happiness with age and the negative with age squared is obtained. Notably, however, the pooled OLS with age coefficients and pooled OLS dummy regressions do not always agree in terms of the significance level. Belgium (panel 2 in Fig. 8) for example exhibits a positive relation between age and happiness for life satisfaction and the CASP-12, while the corresponding coefficients in the pooled OLS regression fail to reach significance.
Of course, conducting the analysis for each country separately with the full control set additionally atomises the data. This is only exacerbated by different countries having differing sample sizes in the data set to begin with. As measures such as the question on life satisfaction appear in many questionnaires, our results could be complemented by studying larger national data sets. Alternatively, future waves of SHARE might include further data to answer the question if the observed insignificances are caused here by a lack of data points or by some countries not exhibiting a positive relation between age and happiness.

4 Discussion

Studies measuring happiness and well-being over the life cycle have found mixed results, and in particular the U-shape of happiness is a controversial finding. Consistent with a U-shape around middle age, we find that happiness increases after the age of 50, irrespective of the specification used. Furthermore, our results indicate that happiness tends to stagnate or even decrease at very high age. When conducting our analysis on country- or gender-specific subsamples, a more varied picture emerges. Where we find significant results in these subsamples, however, it is always consistent with a U-shape. These findings are also robust when accounting for differences due to mortality selection effects. While selection effects are indeed at work, with happier respondents being more likely to be alive at the time the next wave is elicited, CASP-12 is the only measure where the pattern is affected: selection makes the observed pattern more pronounced in this case. The result could potentially stem from the CASP-12 measuring control and agency, which decrease towards the end of one’s life (Oliver et al., 2021; Ribeiro et al., 2020; Rodríguez-Blázquez et al., 2020). This might also help to explain why we find lower turning points for CASP-12 and EURO-D in Table 4 in contrast to life satisfaction, when including additional controls. One reason why life satisfaction might continue to increase in high age is that older people might give up on aspiration and enjoy life more (Blanchflower & Oswald, 2004; Frey & Stutzer, 2010). CASP-12 and EURO-D, on the other hand, measure elements related to control and mental health, which might be more negatively affected by age. Different happiness measures might capture different aspects of life, highlighting the importance of looking at multiple measures at the same time.
Importantly, the observed age-happiness relation is consistently obtained using different approaches that have been used in both research that found and did not find the happiness dip in middle age. Additionally, the happiness-age relationship does not only hold for measures of subjective well-being (life satisfaction), but also for affective/eudemonic (CASP-12) and mental health measures (EURO-D). We are thus confident that our findings are meaningful for a substantial number of European countries.
Naturally, we can make no predictions about the trajectory of the happiness-age relation under the age of 50, as the SHARE data set only provides data for older Europeans. However, as other studies have indicated, there is support for the overall U-shape in various European countries (Blanchflower, 2021). We find that happiness indeed increases after middle age, compared to other studies finding a decrease after middle age (Easterlin, 2006; Mroczek & Spiro, 2005) or an overall decrease (Frijters & Beatton, 2012; Kassenboehmer & Haisken-DeNew, 2012). These differences could reflect regional differences, as Easterlin (2006) and Mroczek and Spiro (2005) use US data. Alternatively, methodological differences might drive these divergences. Kassenboehmer and Haisken-DeNew (2012) utilize respondents leaving the survey panel temporarily, to differentiate between age and years in the survey. Both should still be correlated, however. Frijters and Beatton’s (2012) main result is based on fixed effects regressions, which might ultimately not be reliable enough to deal with the age-period-cohort problem (Heckman & Robb Jr, 1985; Yang & Land, 2008). Mrozcek and Spiro’s (2005) use of a demeaned variable in their specification might similarly be problematic (McIntosh & Schlenker, 2006).
Our results are in line with previous studies indicating an increase of happiness after 50 (Morgan & O’Connor, 2017) or an upward profile for affective measures (Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998). However, similar to other studies, our results also provide evidence that happiness, depending on the measure used, stagnates or even decreases later in life (Blanchflower, 2021; Blanchflower & Graham, 2020; Gwozdz & Sousa-Poza, 2010). Our results support the view that people go through a period of relatively low happiness (relative to happiness at older age) around the midpoint of their life. For policy makers, it is important to further explore why this dip occurs and how it can be alleviated.
Going forward, it is important to highlight that proving or disproving the U-shape of happiness, or as in our case components of it, should not be a goal in itself. While knowing the average path happiness takes over the course of a human life is important, even more so is understanding which life events affect the emerging trajectory (Bjørnskov et al., 2008; Galambos et al., 2020, 2021; Lachman, 2015; Morgan & O’Connor, 2020). Past research has shown the happiness effects of marriage (Grover & Helliwell, 2019), parenthood (Nelson et al., 2013), social networks in general (Becker et al., 2019), income (Easterlin, 1974), social support (Siedlecki et al., 2014), permanent employment (Piper, 2021), the quality of formal institutions (Bjørnskov et al., 2010), giving up on aspirations (Schwandt, 2016), and health (Bussière et al., 2021; Gwozdz & Sousa-Poza, 2010; Oliver et al., 2021). Mapping the evolution of these events over the life course may help to better understand the emergence of the U-shape of happiness.

Acknowledgements

Declarations

Conflicts of interest

Not applicable.

Data Availability

Data is provided free for scientific use. Researchers need to apply here: http://​www.​share-project.​org/​data-access.​html.

Code Availability

Analyses have been conducted in Stata 17. Replication package (without the data) is available upon request.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Appendix

See and Tables 8, 9, 10, 11,12, 13, Fig. 8.
Table 8
Survey questions for well-being and mental health measures
Measure
Question
Life satisfaction
On a scale from 0 to 10 where 0 means completely dissatisfied and 10 means completely satisfied, how satisfied are you with your life?
CASP-12a
How often, if at all, have you experienced the following feelings and thoughts over the past four weeks:
Control
How often do you think your age prevents you from doing the things you would like to do?
How often do you feel that what happens to you is out of your control?
How often do you feel left out of things?
Autonomy
How often do you think that you can do the things that you want to do?
How often do you think that family responsibilities prevent you from doing what you want to do?
How often do you think that shortage of money stops you from doing the things you want to do?
Pleasure
How often do you look forward to each day?
How often do you feel that your life has meaning?
How often, on balance, do you look back on your life with a sense of well-being?
Self-realization
How often do you feel full of energy these days?
How often do you feel that life is full of opportunities?
How often do you feel that the future looks good for you?
EURO-Db
Earlier we talked about your physical health. Another measure of health is your emotional
Depression
health or well-being that is, how you feel about things that happen around you
Pessimism
In the last month, have you been sad or depressed?
Suicidality
What are your hopes for the future?
Guilt
In the last month, have you felt that you would rather be dead?
Sleep
Do you tend to blame yourself or feel guilty about anythingc?
Interest
Have you had trouble sleeping recently?
Irritability
In the last month, what is your interest in thingsd?
Appetite
Have you been irritable recently?
Fatigue
What has your appetite been likee?
Concentration
In the last month, have you had too little energy to do the things you wanted to do?
How is your concentration? For example, can you concentrate on a television program, film or radio program?
Enjoyment
Can you concentrate on something you read?
Tearfulness
What have you enjoyed doing recently?
aIndex generated from questions on 4 different dimensions. The total score ranges from 12 (low quality of life) to 48 (high quality of life). The response options for each item are: 1. Often, 2. Sometimes, 3. Rarely, and 4. Never
bIndex generated from questions on 12 different dimensions. The total score ranges from 0 (not depressed) to 12 (very depressed). The responses are coded as: 0. No indication and 1. There is indication of the respective dimension
cIf the answer is unclear the follow-up question is: So, for what do you blame yourself?
dIf the answer is unclear the follow-up question is: So, do you keep up your interests?
eIf the answer is unclear the follow-up question is: So, have you been eating more or less than usual?
Table 9
Coefficients of the pooled OLS model (2) (using year dummies)
 
Unweighted
Using sampling weights
No controls
Ex. controls
All controls
No controls
Ex. controls
All controls
Life satisfaction
Age
0.173***
0.131***
0.0659***
0.146***
0.115***
0.0684**
(17.28)
(13.61)
(6.34)
(6.57)
(5.41)
(3.12)
Age2
 − 0.00136***
 − 0.00102***
 − 0.000353***
 − 0.00123***
 − 0.000928***
 − 0.000402*
(− 17.39)
(− 13.74)
(− 4.38)
(− 7.10)
(− 5.63)
(− 2.36)
CASP-12
Age
0.242***
0.213***
0.153***
0.250***
0.205***
0.161***
(25.45)
(24.32)
(16.15)
(12.41)
(10.53)
(8.50)
Age2
 − 0.00204***
 − 0.00180***
 − 0.00115***
 − 0.00218***
 − 0.00181***
 − 0.00127***
(− 27.55)
(− 26.47)
(− 15.60)
(− 13.99)
(− 11.99)
(− 8.66)
EURO-D
Age
0.274***
0.233***
0.154***
0.272***
0.210***
0.149***
(26.59)
(23.25)
(13.37)
(11.58)
(9.42)
(6.51)
Age2
 − 0.00225***
 − 0.00194***
 − 0.00113***
 − 0.00227***
 − 0.00178***
 − 0.00112***
(− 27.77)
(− 24.68)
(− 12.64)
(− 12.35)
(− 10.24)
(− 6.27)
t-statistics in parentheses
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Table 10
Correlation between happiness measures and death, dependent variable is probability of dying between waves, logit model (4)
 
Life satisfaction
CASP-12
EURO-D
Life satisfaction
 − 0.0576
  
(− 1.80)
  
CASP-12
 
 − 0.0768*
 
 
(− 2.04)
 
EURO-D
  
 − 0.0773**
  
(− 2.58)
Marriage/registered partnership
 − 0.0126
 − 0.0205
 − 0.0359
(− 0.10)
(− 0.15)
(− 0.29)
Income brackets
[1] Average monthly income per hh, low to mid bracket
0.392**
0.411**
0.557***
(2.59)
(2.62)
(3.82)
[2] Average monthly income per hh, mid to high bracket
0.357*
0.386*
0.488**
(2.24)
(2.35)
(3.22)
[3] Average monthly income per hh, more than high bracket
 − 0.322
 − 0.291
 − 0.274
(− 1.81)
(− 1.57)
(− 1.62)
Education
[1] Primary school
 − 0.257
 − 0.405
 − 0.411
(− 0.90)
(− 1.40)
(− 1.52)
[2] Lower secondary school
 − 0.472
 − 0.550
 − 0.483
(− 1.65)
(− 1.92)
(− 1.79)
[3] Upper secondary school
 − 0.784**
 − 0.842**
 − 0.767**
(− 2.76)
(− 2.95)
(− 2.87)
[4] Post-secondary non-tertiary education
 − 0.366
 − 0.499
 − 0.377
(− 1.00)
(− 1.34)
(− 1.09)
[5] First stage tertiary education
 − 0.670*
 − 0.708*
 − 0.675*
(− 2.20)
(− 2.31)
(− 2.34)
[6] Second stage tertiary education
 − 1.429
 − 1.454
 − 1.413
(− 1.26)
(− 1.28)
(− 1.28)
Subjective health
[1] Fair
 − 1.498***
 − 1.505***
 − 1.465***
(− 9.99)
(− 9.83)
(− 10.19)
[2] Good
 − 2.431***
 − 2.436***
 − 2.378***
(− 13.70)
(− 13.09)
(− 13.79)
[3] Very good
 − 2.653***
 − 2.745***
 − 2.628***
(− 10.27)
(− 10.05)
(− 10.37)
[4] Excellent
 − 2.341***
 − 2.296***
 − 2.343***
(− 7.42)
(− 7.01)
(− 7.57)
Constant
 − 6.300***
 − 6.199***
 − 5.364***
(− 12.92)
(− 12.51)
(− 12.74)
N
164,134
160,305
131,920
t-statistics in parentheses
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Table 11
Coefficients of the pooled OLS model (2) with interaction terms for the in all waves dummy
 
Life satisfaction
CASP-12
EURO-D
In all waves
 − 1.700
 − 4.015*
 − 0.0567
(− 0.94)
(− 2.17)
(− 0.03)
Age
0.0885***
0.149***
0.131***
(6.52)
(12.06)
(8.14)
In all waves # Age
0.0536
0.116*
0.00859
(1.00)
(2.11)
(0.14)
Age2
 − 0.000502***
 − 0.00110***
 − 0.000959***
(− 4.72)
(− 11.33)
(− 7.57)
In all waves # Age2
 − 0.000409
 − 0.000808*
 − 0.0000980
(− 1.04)
(− 2.00)
(− 0.22)
N
164,125
160,296
131,913
R2
0.236
0.349
0.253
t-statistics in parentheses
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Table 12
Coefficients of the pooled OLS model (2) with interaction terms for the female dummy
 
Life satisfaction
CASP-12
EURO-D
Female
2.140
0.659
0.574
(1.53)
(0.55)
(0.39)
Age
0.140***
0.166***
0.137***
(3.85)
(5.16)
(3.55)
Female # Age
 − 0.0664
 − 0.0209
 − 0.0386
(− 1.51)
(− 0.55)
(− 0.84)
Age sq
 − 0.000890**
 − 0.00126***
 − 0.00101***
(− 3.13)
(− 5.01)
(− 3.32)
Female # Age2
0.000528
0.000162
0.000326
(1.54)
(0.56)
(0.92)
N
163,703
159,880
131,523
R2
0.236
0.324
0.251
t-statistics in parentheses
*p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001
Table 13
Coefficients of the pooled OLS model (2) for different countries, t-statistics in brackets
  
Life satisfaction
CASP-12
EURO-D
AUT (N = 5159) Austria
Age
0.206**
0.268***
0.192**
(3.19)
(4.27)
(3.12)
Age2
 − 0.00143**
 − 0.00207***
 − 0.00142**
(− 2.80)
(− 4.17)
(− 2.91)
BEL (N = 7913) Belgium
Age
0.0516
0.0569
0.0241
(0.94)
(0.88)
(0.28)
Age2
 − 0.000190
 − 0.000133
 − 0.000104
(− 0.45)
(− 0.27)
(− 0.16)
CHE (N = 3686) Switzerland
Age
0.0440
0.168***
0.173**
(0.95)
(3.51)
(3.02)
Age2
 − 0.000177
 − 0.00125***
 − 0.00131**
(− 0.49)
(− 3.37)
(− 2.92)
CZE (N = 7156) Chech Republic
Age
0.128*
0.256***
0.234***
(2.05)
(5.29)
(3.64)
Age2
 − 0.000691
 − 0.00191***
 − 0.00168***
(− 1.44)
(− 5.15)
(− 3.41)
DEU (N = 7524) Germany
Age
0.263***
0.166***
0.180**
(4.92)
(3.51)
(2.90)
Age2
 − 0.00170***
 − 0.00118**
 − 0.00126**
(− 4.08)
(− 3.20)
(− 2.64)
DEN (N = 4734) Denmark
Age
0.0682
0.194***
0.135*
(1.40)
(4.35)
(2.16)
Age2
 − 0.000353
 − 0.00135***
 − 0.000932
(− 0.92)
(− 3.82)
(− 1.92)
ESP (N = 6560) Spain
Age
0.0552
0.0453
 − 0.0205
(0.55)
(0.43)
(− 0.14)
Age2
 − 0.000140
 − 0.000254
0.000163
(− 0.18)
(− 0.31)
(0.14)
EST (N = 6178) Estonia
Age
0.157**
0.164***
0.122*
(3.24)
(4.06)
(2.35)
Age2
 − 0.000948*
 − 0.00123***
 − 0.000868*
(− 2.50)
(− 3.88)
(− 2.11)
FRA (N = 6367) France
Age
0.0993
0.185***
0.144*
(1.77)
(3.58)
(2.18)
Age2
 − 0.000654
 − 0.00142***
 − 0.000964
(− 1.51)
(− 3.51)
(− 1.86)
GRC (N = 5087) Greece
Age
0.00368
0.0433
0.293
(0.03)
(0.39)
(1.58)
Age2
0.000357
 − 0.000442
 − 0.00225
(0.31)
(− 0.51)
(− 1.55)
HRV (N = 2579) Croatia
Age
 − 0.129
 − 0.0433
0.187
(− 0.96)
(− 0.39)
(1.14)
Age2
0.00113
0.000301
 − 0.00146
(1.09)
(0.35)
(− 1.13)
HUN (N = 2694) Hungary
Age
 − 0.0836
0.298**
0.0238
(− 0.63)
(3.07)
(0.20)
Age2
0.000932
 − 0.00229**
 − 0.000283
(0.89)
(− 3.00)
(− 0.30)
ISR (N = 3166) Israel
Age
 − 0.0994
 − 0.0775
 − 0.0955
(− 0.48)
(− 0.44)
(− 0.32)
Age2
0.000768
0.000330
0.000657
(0.46)
(0.24)
(0.27)
ITA (N = 7068) Italy
Age
0.0650
0.0545
0.0476
(1.06)
(0.95)
(0.64)
Age2
 − 0.000449
 − 0.000321
 − 0.000338
(− 0.96)
(− 0.73)
(− 0.60)
LUX (N = 1847) Luxembourg
Age
 − 0.0343
 − 0.0675
0.211
(− 0.29)
(− 0.62)
(1.35)
Age2
0.000628
0.000855
 − 0.00143
(0.66)
(1.01)
(− 1.15)
NLD (N = 5592) The Netherlands
Age
0.208***
0.183*
0.113
(3.54)
(2.45)
(1.39)
Age2
 − 0.00149***
 − 0.00135*
 − 0.000696
(− 3.34)
(− 2.33)
(− 1.11)
POL (N = 5377) Poland
Age
0.372**
0.338**
0.453*
(2.70)
(2.71)
(2.20)
Age2
 − 0.00268*
 − 0.00250**
 − 0.00354*
(− 2.52)
(− 2.59)
(− 2.20)
PRT (N = 1854) Portugal
Age
0.0491
0.128
0.0951
(0.17)
(0.68)
(0.37)
Age2
 − 0.000898
 − 0.00122
 − 0.000736
(− 0.40)
(− 0.80)
(− 0.35)
SVN (N = 4508) lovenia
Age
 − 0.0186
0.140**
0.0876
(− 0.30)
(2.74)
(1.24)
Age2
0.000122
 − 0.00136***
 − 0.000786
(0.25)
(− 3.44)
(− 1.41)
SWE (N = 5306) Sweden
Age
0.227***
0.254***
0.237**
(3.50)
(4.21)
(3.12)
Age2
 − 0.00149**
 − 0.00186***
 − 0.00172**
(− 3.07)
(− 4.13)
(− 3.02)

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Fußnoten
1
We use a simple fixed-effects specification here, comparable to Frijters and Beatton (2012) and in line with our simple pooled OLS specification. Fixed effects specifications can be sensitive to the baseline and still suffer from the identification problem, i.e., that age, time and cohort are perfectly collinear. There are attempts to rectify these problems, such as Dijk and Mierau (2018), Cheng et al. (2017), Van Landeghem (2012) and De Ree and Alessie (2011), wich go beyond the scope of this paper.
 
2
In the fixed effects specification (3) time-invariant factors such as country of origin and or birth year are of course demeaned and thus eliminated from the estimation.
 
3
The measure for life satisfaction was only added in wave 2, hence there is no data for wave 1.
 
4
Sampling weights are inversely proportional to the probability of being sampled from the underlying population, based on demographic factors, such as nationality or gender. Sampling weights in SHARE are calculated using the procedure of Deville and Särndal (1992).
 
5
Here we again run in the identification problem as age, cohort, and year are perfectly collinear and cannot be included simultaneously. Results using year dummies instead of cohort dummies are included in Table 9 in the appendix and in Tables S13–S18 in the online supplement. The estimates are overall qualitatively close to the cohort dummy specification.
 
6
Longitudinal sampling weights for the fixed effects model require the respondents to be weighted over all waves. Hence, applying weights from wave 1 to wave 7 leads to all respondents that dropped out of the survey in between being dropped from the sample. The remaining sample is thus equal to the no attrition subsample and the weighted fixed effects results will be reported accordingly in Sect. 3.2.2.
 
7
Figure 6 shows the graphs without confidence intervals for better visibility. For the graph including the confidence intervals (which also illustrates the loss of significance), see Fig. S7 in the online supplement.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Ameriks, K., & Clarke, D. M. (2000). Aristotle: Nicomachean ethics. Cambridge University Press. Ameriks, K., & Clarke, D. M. (2000). Aristotle: Nicomachean ethics. Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Becker, C., Kirchmaier, I., & Trautmann, S. T. (2019). Marriage, parenthood and social network: Subjective well-being and mental health in old age. PLoS ONE, 14(7), e0218704.CrossRef Becker, C., Kirchmaier, I., & Trautmann, S. T. (2019). Marriage, parenthood and social network: Subjective well-being and mental health in old age. PLoS ONE, 14(7), e0218704.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bell, D. N., & Blanchflower, D. G. (2020). The U-shape of happiness in Scotland. National Bureau of Economic Research, (No. w28144). Bell, D. N., & Blanchflower, D. G. (2020). The U-shape of happiness in Scotland. National Bureau of Economic Research, (No. w28144).
Zurück zum Zitat Bjørnskov, C., Dreher, A., & Fischer, J. A. (2008). Cross-country determinants of life satisfaction: Exploring different determinants across groups in society. Social Choice and Welfare, 30(1), 119–173.CrossRef Bjørnskov, C., Dreher, A., & Fischer, J. A. (2008). Cross-country determinants of life satisfaction: Exploring different determinants across groups in society. Social Choice and Welfare, 30(1), 119–173.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bjørnskov, C., Dreher, A., & Fischer, J. A. (2010). Formal institutions and subjective well-being: Revisiting the cross-country evidence. European Journal of Political Economy, 26(4), 419–430.CrossRef Bjørnskov, C., Dreher, A., & Fischer, J. A. (2010). Formal institutions and subjective well-being: Revisiting the cross-country evidence. European Journal of Political Economy, 26(4), 419–430.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Blanchflower, D. G., & Graham, C. L. (2020). The mid-life dip in well-being: Economists (Who Find It) versus psychologists (Who Don’t)! National Bureau of Economic Research. Blanchflower, D. G., & Graham, C. L. (2020). The mid-life dip in well-being: Economists (Who Find It) versus psychologists (Who Don’t)! National Bureau of Economic Research.
Zurück zum Zitat Blanchflower, D. G., & Graham, C. L. (2021). The U-shape of happiness: A response. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16, 1435–1446.CrossRef Blanchflower, D. G., & Graham, C. L. (2021). The U-shape of happiness: A response. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16, 1435–1446.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Blanchflower, D. G., & Piper, A. (2021). The well-being age U-shape effect in Germany is not flat. GLO Discussion Paper. Blanchflower, D. G., & Piper, A. (2021). The well-being age U-shape effect in Germany is not flat. GLO Discussion Paper.
Zurück zum Zitat Blanchflower, D. G. (2021). Is happiness U-shaped everywhere? Age and subjective well-being in 145 countries. Journal of Population Economics, 34(2), 575–624.CrossRef Blanchflower, D. G. (2021). Is happiness U-shaped everywhere? Age and subjective well-being in 145 countries. Journal of Population Economics, 34(2), 575–624.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2004). Well-being over time in Britain and the USA. Journal of Public Economics, 88(7–8), 1359–1386.CrossRef Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2004). Well-being over time in Britain and the USA. Journal of Public Economics, 88(7–8), 1359–1386.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2008). Is well-being U-shaped over the life cycle? Social Science & Medicine, 66(8), 1733–1749.CrossRef Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2008). Is well-being U-shaped over the life cycle? Social Science & Medicine, 66(8), 1733–1749.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2009). The U-shape without controls: A response to Glenn. Social Science & Medicine, 69(4), 486–488.CrossRef Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2009). The U-shape without controls: A response to Glenn. Social Science & Medicine, 69(4), 486–488.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Börsch-Supan, A., Brandt, M., Hunkler, C., Kneip, T., Korbmacher, J., Malter, F., et al. (2013). Data resource profile: The survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe (SHARE). International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(4), 992–1001. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt088CrossRef Börsch-Supan, A., Brandt, M., Hunkler, C., Kneip, T., Korbmacher, J., Malter, F., et al. (2013). Data resource profile: The survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe (SHARE). International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(4), 992–1001. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1093/​ije/​dyt088CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bussière, C., Sirven, N., & Tessier, P. (2021). Does ageing alter the contribution of health to subjective well-being? Social Science & Medicine, 268, 113456.CrossRef Bussière, C., Sirven, N., & Tessier, P. (2021). Does ageing alter the contribution of health to subjective well-being? Social Science & Medicine, 268, 113456.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Castro-Costa, E., Dewey, M., Stewart, R., Banerjee, S., Huppert, F., Mendonca-Lima, C., et al. (2008). Ascertaining late-life depressive symptoms in Europe: An evaluation of the survey version of the EURO-D scale in 10 nations. The SHARE project. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 12–29. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.236CrossRef Castro-Costa, E., Dewey, M., Stewart, R., Banerjee, S., Huppert, F., Mendonca-Lima, C., et al. (2008). Ascertaining late-life depressive symptoms in Europe: An evaluation of the survey version of the EURO-D scale in 10 nations. The SHARE project. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 12–29. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​mpr.​236CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cheng, T. C., Powdthavee, N., & Oswald, A. J. (2017). Longitudinal evidence for a midlife nadir in human well-being: Results from four data sets. The Economic Journal, 127(599), 126–142.CrossRef Cheng, T. C., Powdthavee, N., & Oswald, A. J. (2017). Longitudinal evidence for a midlife nadir in human well-being: Results from four data sets. The Economic Journal, 127(599), 126–142.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Clark, A. E., & Oswald, A. J. (2006). The curved relationship between subjective well-being and age. halshs-00590404. Clark, A. E., & Oswald, A. J. (2006). The curved relationship between subjective well-being and age. halshs-00590404.
Zurück zum Zitat Clark, A. E. (2019). Born to be mild? Cohort effects don’t (fully) explain why well-being is U-shaped in age. The economics of happiness (pp. 387–408). Springer.CrossRef Clark, A. E. (2019). Born to be mild? Cohort effects don’t (fully) explain why well-being is U-shaped in age. The economics of happiness (pp. 387–408). Springer.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat De Ree, J., & Alessie, R. (2011). Life satisfaction and age: Dealing with underidentification in age-period-cohort models. Social Science & Medicine, 73(1), 177–182.CrossRef De Ree, J., & Alessie, R. (2011). Life satisfaction and age: Dealing with underidentification in age-period-cohort models. Social Science & Medicine, 73(1), 177–182.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Deaton, A. (2008). Income, health, and well-being around the world: Evidence from the Gallup World Poll. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22(2), 53–72.CrossRef Deaton, A. (2008). Income, health, and well-being around the world: Evidence from the Gallup World Poll. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22(2), 53–72.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Deville, J.-C., & Särndal, C.-E. (1992). Calibration estimators in survey sampling. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 87(418), 376–382.CrossRef Deville, J.-C., & Särndal, C.-E. (1992). Calibration estimators in survey sampling. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 87(418), 376–382.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dijk, H. H., & Mierau, J. O. (2018). Mental health over the life course: Evidence for a U-shape? European Journal of Public Health, 28(4(suppl_4)), 213–885. Dijk, H. H., & Mierau, J. O. (2018). Mental health over the life course: Evidence for a U-shape? European Journal of Public Health, 28(4(suppl_4)), 213–885.
Zurück zum Zitat Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. Nations and households in economic growth (pp. 89–125). Elsevier. Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. Nations and households in economic growth (pp. 89–125). Elsevier.
Zurück zum Zitat Easterlin, R. A., & Schaeffer, C. M. (1999). Income and subjective well-being over the life cycle. The Self and Society in Aging Processes. New York: Springer Publishing Co Easterlin, R. A., & Schaeffer, C. M. (1999). Income and subjective well-being over the life cycle. The Self and Society in Aging Processes. New York: Springer Publishing Co
Zurück zum Zitat Easterlin, R. A. (2006). Life cycle happiness and its sources: Intersections of psychology, economics, and demography. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(4), 463–482.CrossRef Easterlin, R. A. (2006). Life cycle happiness and its sources: Intersections of psychology, economics, and demography. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(4), 463–482.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). What can economists learn from happiness research? Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 402–435.CrossRef Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). What can economists learn from happiness research? Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 402–435.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2010). Happiness and economics. Princeton University Press. Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2010). Happiness and economics. Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Frijters, P., & Beatton, T. (2012). The mystery of the U-shaped relationship between happiness and age. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 82(2–3), 525–542.CrossRef Frijters, P., & Beatton, T. (2012). The mystery of the U-shaped relationship between happiness and age. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 82(2–3), 525–542.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Galambos, N. L., Krahn, H. J., Johnson, M. D., & Lachman, M. E. (2020). The U shape of happiness across the life course: Expanding the discussion. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 898–912.CrossRef Galambos, N. L., Krahn, H. J., Johnson, M. D., & Lachman, M. E. (2020). The U shape of happiness across the life course: Expanding the discussion. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 898–912.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Galambos, N. L., Krahn, H. J., Johnson, M. D., & Lachman, M. E. (2021). Another attempt to move beyond the cross-sectional U shape of happiness: A reply. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(6), 1447–1455.CrossRef Galambos, N. L., Krahn, H. J., Johnson, M. D., & Lachman, M. E. (2021). Another attempt to move beyond the cross-sectional U shape of happiness: A reply. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(6), 1447–1455.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gerdtham, U.-G., & Johannesson, M. (2001). The relationship between happiness, health, and socio-economic factors: Results based on Swedish microdata. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 30(6), 553–557.CrossRef Gerdtham, U.-G., & Johannesson, M. (2001). The relationship between happiness, health, and socio-economic factors: Results based on Swedish microdata. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 30(6), 553–557.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Glenn, N. (2009). Is the apparent U-shape of well-being over the life course a result of inappropriate use of control variables? A commentary on Blanchflower and Oswald (66: 8, 2008, 1733–1749). Social Science & Medicine, 69(4), 481–485.CrossRef Glenn, N. (2009). Is the apparent U-shape of well-being over the life course a result of inappropriate use of control variables? A commentary on Blanchflower and Oswald (66: 8, 2008, 1733–1749). Social Science & Medicine, 69(4), 481–485.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grover, S., & Helliwell, J. F. (2019). How’s life at home? New evidence on marriage and the set point for happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 20(2), 373–390.CrossRef Grover, S., & Helliwell, J. F. (2019). How’s life at home? New evidence on marriage and the set point for happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 20(2), 373–390.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Guven, C., & Saloumidis, R. (2014). Life satisfaction and longevity: Longitudinal evidence from the German socio-economic panel. German Economic Review, 15(4), 453–472.CrossRef Guven, C., & Saloumidis, R. (2014). Life satisfaction and longevity: Longitudinal evidence from the German socio-economic panel. German Economic Review, 15(4), 453–472.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gwozdz, W., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2010). Ageing, health and life satisfaction of the oldest old: An analysis for Germany. Social Indicators Research, 97(3), 397–417.CrossRef Gwozdz, W., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2010). Ageing, health and life satisfaction of the oldest old: An analysis for Germany. Social Indicators Research, 97(3), 397–417.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Heckman, J. J., & Robb, R., Jr. (1985). Alternative methods for evaluating the impact of interventions: An overview. Journal of Econometrics, 30(1–2), 239–267.CrossRef Heckman, J. J., & Robb, R., Jr. (1985). Alternative methods for evaluating the impact of interventions: An overview. Journal of Econometrics, 30(1–2), 239–267.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hellevik, O. (2017). The U-shaped age–happiness relationship: Real or methodological artifact? Quality & Quantity, 51(1), 177–197.CrossRef Hellevik, O. (2017). The U-shaped age–happiness relationship: Real or methodological artifact? Quality & Quantity, 51(1), 177–197.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hudomiet, P., Hurd, M. D., & Rohwedder, S. (2021). The age profile of life satisfaction after age 65 in the US. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 189, 431–442.CrossRef Hudomiet, P., Hurd, M. D., & Rohwedder, S. (2021). The age profile of life satisfaction after age 65 in the US. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 189, 431–442.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kassenboehmer, S. C., & Haisken-DeNew, J. P. (2012). Heresy or enlightenment? The well-being age U-shape effect is flat. Economics Letters, 117(1), 235–238.CrossRef Kassenboehmer, S. C., & Haisken-DeNew, J. P. (2012). Heresy or enlightenment? The well-being age U-shape effect is flat. Economics Letters, 117(1), 235–238.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kim, E. S., Delaney, S. W., Tay, L., Chen, Y., Diener, E. D., & Vanderweele, T. J. (2021). Life satisfaction and subsequent physical, behavioral, and psychosocial health in older adults. The Milbank Quarterly, 99(1), 209–239.CrossRef Kim, E. S., Delaney, S. W., Tay, L., Chen, Y., Diener, E. D., & Vanderweele, T. J. (2021). Life satisfaction and subsequent physical, behavioral, and psychosocial health in older adults. The Milbank Quarterly, 99(1), 209–239.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kratz, F., & Brüderl, J. (2021). The Age Trajectory of Happiness. Kratz, F., & Brüderl, J. (2021). The Age Trajectory of Happiness.
Zurück zum Zitat Laaksonen, S. (2018). A research note: Happiness by age is more complex than U-shaped. Journal of Happiness Studies, 19(2), 471–482. Laaksonen, S. (2018). A research note: Happiness by age is more complex than U-shaped. Journal of Happiness Studies, 19(2), 471–482.
Zurück zum Zitat Lachman, M. E. (2015). Mind the gap in the middle: A call to study midlife. Research in Human Development, 12(3–4), 327–334.CrossRef Lachman, M. E. (2015). Mind the gap in the middle: A call to study midlife. Research in Human Development, 12(3–4), 327–334.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lee, H., & Singh, G. K. (2020). Inequalities in Life expectancy and all-cause mortality in the United States by levels of happiness and life satisfaction: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Maternal and Child Health and AIDS, 9(3), 305. Lee, H., & Singh, G. K. (2020). Inequalities in Life expectancy and all-cause mortality in the United States by levels of happiness and life satisfaction: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Maternal and Child Health and AIDS, 9(3), 305.
Zurück zum Zitat McIntosh, C. T., & Schlenker, W. (2006). Identifying non-linearities in fixed effects models. UC-San Diego Working Paper. McIntosh, C. T., & Schlenker, W. (2006). Identifying non-linearities in fixed effects models. UC-San Diego Working Paper.
Zurück zum Zitat Morgan, R., & O’Connor, K. J. (2017). Experienced life cycle satisfaction in Europe. Review of Behavioral Economics, 4(4), 371–396.CrossRef Morgan, R., & O’Connor, K. J. (2017). Experienced life cycle satisfaction in Europe. Review of Behavioral Economics, 4(4), 371–396.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Morgan, R., & O’Connor, K. J. (2020). Does the U-shape pattern in life cycle satisfaction obscure reality? A response to blanchflower. Review of Behavioral Economics, 7(2), 201–206.CrossRef Morgan, R., & O’Connor, K. J. (2020). Does the U-shape pattern in life cycle satisfaction obscure reality? A response to blanchflower. Review of Behavioral Economics, 7(2), 201–206.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mroczek, D. K., & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1333.CrossRef Mroczek, D. K., & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1333.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A. (2005). Change in life satisfaction during adulthood: Findings from the veterans affairs normative aging study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1), 189.CrossRef Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A. (2005). Change in life satisfaction during adulthood: Findings from the veterans affairs normative aging study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1), 189.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Oliver, A., Sentandreu-Mañó, T., Tomás, J. M., Fernández, I., & Sancho, P. (2021). Quality of life in European older adults of SHARE wave 7: Comparing the old and the oldest-old. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10(13), 2850.CrossRef Oliver, A., Sentandreu-Mañó, T., Tomás, J. M., Fernández, I., & Sancho, P. (2021). Quality of life in European older adults of SHARE wave 7: Comparing the old and the oldest-old. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10(13), 2850.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Piper, A. (2021). Temps dip deeper: Temporary employment and the midlife nadir in human well-being. The Journal of the Economics of Ageing, 19, 100323.CrossRef Piper, A. (2021). Temps dip deeper: Temporary employment and the midlife nadir in human well-being. The Journal of the Economics of Ageing, 19, 100323.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Prince, M. J., Beekman, A. T. F., Deeg, D. J. H., Fuhrer, R., Kivela, S.-L., Lawlor, B. A., et al. (1999a). Depression symptoms in late life assessed using the EURO–D scale. Effect of age, gender and marital status in 14 European centres. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174(04), 339–345. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.174.4.339CrossRef Prince, M. J., Beekman, A. T. F., Deeg, D. J. H., Fuhrer, R., Kivela, S.-L., Lawlor, B. A., et al. (1999a). Depression symptoms in late life assessed using the EURO–D scale. Effect of age, gender and marital status in 14 European centres. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174(04), 339–345. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1192/​bjp.​174.​4.​339CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Prince, M. J., Reischies, F., Beekman, A. T. F., Fuhrer, R., Jonker, C., Kivela, S.-L., et al. (1999b). Development of the EURO–D scale – a European Union initiative to compare symptoms of depression in 14 European centres. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174(04), 330–338. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.174.4.330CrossRef Prince, M. J., Reischies, F., Beekman, A. T. F., Fuhrer, R., Jonker, C., Kivela, S.-L., et al. (1999b). Development of the EURO–D scale – a European Union initiative to compare symptoms of depression in 14 European centres. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174(04), 330–338. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1192/​bjp.​174.​4.​330CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ribeiro, O., Teixeira, L., Araújo, L., Rodríguez-Blázquez, C., Calderón-Larrañaga, A., & Forjaz, M. J. (2020). Anxiety, depression and quality of life in older adults: Trajectories of influence across age. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(23), 9039.CrossRef Ribeiro, O., Teixeira, L., Araújo, L., Rodríguez-Blázquez, C., Calderón-Larrañaga, A., & Forjaz, M. J. (2020). Anxiety, depression and quality of life in older adults: Trajectories of influence across age. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(23), 9039.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rodríguez-Blázquez, C., Ribeiro, O., Ayala, A., Teixeira, L., Araújo, L., & Forjaz, M. J. (2020). Psychometric properties of the CASP-12 scale in Portugal: An analysis using SHARE data. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(18), 6610.CrossRef Rodríguez-Blázquez, C., Ribeiro, O., Ayala, A., Teixeira, L., Araújo, L., & Forjaz, M. J. (2020). Psychometric properties of the CASP-12 scale in Portugal: An analysis using SHARE data. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(18), 6610.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schwandt, H. (2016). Unmet aspirations as an explanation for the age U-shape in wellbeing. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 122, 75–87.CrossRef Schwandt, H. (2016). Unmet aspirations as an explanation for the age U-shape in wellbeing. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 122, 75–87.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Deaton, A. (2010). A snapshot of the age distribution of psychological well-being in the United States. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(22), 9985–9990.CrossRef Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Deaton, A. (2010). A snapshot of the age distribution of psychological well-being in the United States. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(22), 9985–9990.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ulloa, B. F. L., Møller, V., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2013). How does subjective well-being evolve with age? A literature review. Journal of Population Ageing, 6(3), 227–246.CrossRef Ulloa, B. F. L., Møller, V., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2013). How does subjective well-being evolve with age? A literature review. Journal of Population Ageing, 6(3), 227–246.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van Landeghem, B. (2012). A test for the convexity of human well-being over the life cycle: Longitudinal evidence from a 20-year panel. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81(2), 571–582.CrossRef Van Landeghem, B. (2012). A test for the convexity of human well-being over the life cycle: Longitudinal evidence from a 20-year panel. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81(2), 571–582.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yang, Y., & Land, K. C. (2008). Age–period–cohort analysis of repeated cross-section surveys: Fixed or random effects? Sociological Methods & Research, 36(3), 297–326.CrossRef Yang, Y., & Land, K. C. (2008). Age–period–cohort analysis of repeated cross-section surveys: Fixed or random effects? Sociological Methods & Research, 36(3), 297–326.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Does Happiness Increase in Old Age? Longitudinal Evidence from 20 European Countries
verfasst von
Christoph K. Becker
Stefan T. Trautmann
Publikationsdatum
02.09.2022
Verlag
Springer Netherlands
Erschienen in
Journal of Happiness Studies / Ausgabe 7/2022
Print ISSN: 1389-4978
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-7780
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-022-00569-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 7/2022

Journal of Happiness Studies 7/2022 Zur Ausgabe