Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Management Control 4/2020

04.02.2020 | Original Paper

A strategy map’s effect on the feedback that middle managers pass along to upper management

verfasst von: Brian D. Knox

Erschienen in: Journal of Management Control | Ausgabe 4/2020

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Feedback from lower-level employees helps upper management know when a strategy revision is necessary. Middle managers have significant control over upper management’s access to lower-level employee feedback because they decide whether to pass along that feedback when they receive it. A strategy map may help mitigate bias in these middle managers’ feedback reporting decisions. In my experiment, participants act as middle managers who receive one of two types of lower-level employee feedback: positive feedback or negative feedback. Without a strategy map, participants over-report positive feedback, under-report negative feedback, and exhibit motivated reasoning toward negative feedback. With a strategy map, participants over-report positive feedback less, under-report negative feedback at the same rate, and their motivated reasoning against negative feedback no longer affects their reporting willingness. Thus, a strategy map could help de-bias some of the feedback middle managers pass along to upper management.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
Strategy maps are part of the balanced scorecard, a framework that organizes strategic goals and performance measurements into three non-financial categories and one financial category (Kaplan and Norton 2000).
 
2
This is sometimes called double-loop feedback, i.e. feedback about both the implementation of the strategy and the validity of the strategy’s underlying assumptions. Firms that do not examine their strategy’s underlying assumptions can drift off course considerably even while faithfully implementing the firm’s strategy (see Campbell et al. 2015).
 
3
As explained in Sect. 3, I ask this question twice: in a relatively informal setting and then again in a relatively formal setting.
 
4
For much of this paper, I focus on ingratiation behavior in response to subjective performance measures because such measures amplify and formalize the value of ingratiation. Even so, I acknowledge that subjective performance measures are not the only reason middle managers might try to ingratiate themselves to upper management. Middle managers likely also value a wide variety of informal favors from upper management, including recommendations for future positions, help with building their professional network, and self-image maintenance or validation.
 
5
Demographic measures generally do not differ between conditions, with p values > 0.24 two-tailed for all but one variable: gender. Gender does not differ significantly between strategy map conditions (p > 0.18, two-tailed) but does vary significantly between feedback conditions (p = 0.04, two-tailed). As a supplemental test, I add an indicator variable for gender (to control for potential gender differences) to my main statistical test of H1 and to my mediation analysis related to H1 (as reported later in Sect. 4). These H1-related results are unchanged in direction and significance, and the gender indicator variable itself is non-significant. As such, it is unlikely that gender differences drive my results.
 
6
To avoid the risk of a participant being exposed to both between-subject strategy map conditions, I segregate my six experimental sessions by strategy map manipulation (i.e. participants in an experimental session are either all in the strategy map condition or all in the non-causal list condition). This is necessary because of the highly visual nature of the strategy map manipulation and the risk that a participant might even momentarily glance at another participant’s screen and then be able to guess the manipulation. I also space participants far enough apart in the computer lab and supervise them closely enough to materially eliminate the risk that participants are exposed to both between-subject feedback type conditions (this manipulation is in-text and takes much longer to be exposed to).
 
7
Price et al. (2017) suggest that within-subject studies are the default for psychology studies because of their greater control and statistical power (see also Maxwell et al. 2017, 741).
 
8
Participants assume the role of a newly hired manager, and I provide them with a new strategy from the CEO. This way participants do not need to assume what prior beliefs they might have had earlier in the scenario, which otherwise could create confounding motivated reasoning-related behavior (see Cheng and Humphreys 2012 and Tayler 2010).
 
9
I do not vary the order of the reporting setting. All participants answer the reporting willingness question in the informal reporting setting first and in the formal reporting setting second. This, again, is to maintain realism in a high-context experimental setting (see Haynes and Kachelmeier 1998). Counterbalancing the order of my within-subject conditions would create an excessively artificial setting. It would be exceptionally rare for a middle manager in practice not to have an opportunity to informally pass along feedback at some point before a formal strategy review meeting, especially given the instantaneous nature of modern global communication. An experimental setting where the formal reporting setting precedes any opportunity for informal reporting could lead participants to react in unusual ways that are unrelated to the underlying psychological constructs of interest.
 
10
There are abundant examples of this from experimental accounting research. For example, Cardinaels (2016) pools results across periods and “the reporting period is treated as a within-subject effect” (56). Similar indicator variables also account for within-subject variation in Kuang and Moser’s (2009) analysis (see their footnote 10).
 
11
When I re-run my main tests for H1 through H3 (see Sects. 4’s Tables 2 Panel A, 3 Panel A, and 4) with an added interaction term between reporting setting and the hypothesized predictor, I find no significant reporting setting interactions (p > 0.19 one-tailed for all such interaction terms).
 
12
I only ask participants these questions as they relate to their reporting willingness in the formal setting. I do this to reduce noise that might arise from trying to differentiate their thoughts between two very similar reporting willingness questions (especially since participants would not know I plan to ask them to recall these thoughts later). I expect the general direction of ingratiation intention for reporting willingness in the formal setting to be mostly comparable to what the direction would be for reporting willingness in the informal setting, although the magnitude might be greater because of the increased impression management stakes in the formal setting.
 
13
These questions include upper management and peers, as inspired by one of the questions that Webb et al. (2010) use to measure impression management intentions. It also fits with the setting in which I ask the questions. The public nature of a formal strategy evaluation meeting means one can impress secondary targets, such as peer middle managers. Peers’ impressions can be important. Peers can play an indirect role in subjective performance measures (by sharing their impressions with upper management), they can help one’s objective performance (through cooperation and teamwork), and they might be promoted to the upper management team in the future. I have no reason to expect the direction of ingratiation intentions to be substantially different based on the target: participants likely want to impress the CEO and peer middle managers in much the same way.
 
14
Even though no hypothesis test includes responses to my stage five question about participants’ willingness to give a negative performance review to the branch manager providing the feedback, it is worthwhile to briefly describe results from this question in this footnote. The question asks participants’ agreement that “If [they] reviewed the performance of the branch manager that reported the trend, [they] would rate it negatively.” I phrase the question this way because I assume participants will naturally gravitate toward leniency (see Bol 2008, 12). I ask participants’ level of agreement on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Mean (standard deviation) response is 1.85 (1.03).
 
15
I do not ask a manipulation check question related to the strategy map manipulation.
 
16
These two-sample t-tests use 61 participant-level observations rather than 122 reporting willingness-level observations.
 
17
I use one-tailed tests for all directional expectations (two-tailed tests otherwise). This is common practice in experimental accounting research (e.g., Kelly et al. 2017, 172). One-tailed significance testing “makes sense when we have good reason to expect the sample mean will differ from the hypothetical population mean in a particular direction” (Price et al. 2017, Chapter 13.2 emphasis added).
 
18
I choose regression over ANOVA because some predictors are continuous rather than categorical (see supplemental tests of H1, H2, and H3). Cobb (1998) recommends regression over ANOVA in cases where predictors are continuous. Keeping statistical tests consistent across hypotheses makes the overall results more readable. So, I use regression for hypothesis tests that have only categorical predictors as well.
 
19
Baron and Kenny’s (1986) procedure has been criticized over the years (see Zhao et al. 2010). First, it has been criticized for being too low-powered and ignoring indirect effects. This is less of a concern in this case, given that I find significant effects using their procedure. Second, Baron and Kenny’s last step simply labels the mediation “partial” or “complete” based on the significance of the direct effect in the final regression. Critics consider this labelling simplistic. They focus more on the significance of the indirect path in its own right, without worrying about how “complete” the mediation is. To test the indirect pathway’s significance, i.e. feedback type to ingratiation intention to reporting willingness, I perform a Sobel test, which returns a significant result (z = − 2.22, p = 0.01 one-tailed).
 
20
The effect of feedback type on ingratiation intention is stronger for one of the questions that make up ingratiation intention than the other. Feedback type has a significant negative effect on participants’ responses about their hope “to impress” (t = − 3.24, p < 0.01 one-tailed) and a non-significant positive effect on participants’ responses about their hope “not to upset” (t = 1.45, p = 0.08 one-tailed).
 
21
The effect of strategy map on ingratiation intention does not appear to be specific to either of the questions that make up ingratiation intention. Strategy map has a non-significant negative effect on participants’ responses about their hope “to impress” (t = − 1.32, p = 0.10 one-tailed) and a non-significant positive effect on participants’ responses about their hope “not to upset” (t = 1.18, p = 0.12 one-tailed).
 
22
To test the indirect pathway’s significance, i.e. strategy map to ingratiation intention to reporting willingness, I perform a Sobel test, which returns a significant result (z = − 2.03, p = 0.02 one-tailed).
 
23
H2 and H3 could also be conceptualized together as predicting a main effect for feedback type and an interaction term for strategy map in a single regression. However, when I test the two hypotheses together like this, the interaction term is not significant (p = 0.21 one-tailed). This could be due to the lack of a significant H3 effect.
 
24
Through a two-sample t-test among participants in the negative feedback condition, I find that the moderator, strategy map, is not significantly correlated with perceived strategy ineffectiveness (p = 0.45 two-tailed). The two variables are also uncorrelated across the entire sample of participants (p = 0.25 two-tailed).
 
25
Through a two-sample t-test among participants in the negative feedback condition, I find that the moderator, strategy map, is not significantly correlated with reporting time (p = 0.49 two-tailed). The two variables are also uncorrelated across the entire sample of participants (p = 0.59 two-tailed).
 
26
I find no evidence that a strategy map affects the tendency to give a negative performance review to the branch manager providing negative feedback (p = 0.33, one-tailed). My supplemental tests of motivated reasoning and H3 (see Table 5) are still supported if I add ingratiation intention as a predictor of reporting willingness. Only one hypothesis-testing coefficient differs in direction or significance: when I add ingratiation intention, the coefficient for perceived strategy ineffectiveness moves just outside of standard significance levels (z = − 1.61, p = 0.054 one-tailed). Simple effects regressions are unchanged in direction and significance when I add ingratiation intention.
 
27
Epstein and Wisner (2001) argue that external disclosure of a balanced scorecard is “supported by Balanced Scorecard co-founder David Norton and by several academic studies” (6).
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,51(6), 1173.CrossRef Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,51(6), 1173.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bentley MacLeod, W. (2003). Optimal contracting with subjective evaluation. American Economic Review,93(1), 216–240.CrossRef Bentley MacLeod, W. (2003). Optimal contracting with subjective evaluation. American Economic Review,93(1), 216–240.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bol, J. C. (2008). Subjectivity in compensation contracting. Journal of Accounting Literature,27, 1. Bol, J. C. (2008). Subjectivity in compensation contracting. Journal of Accounting Literature,27, 1.
Zurück zum Zitat Bozeman, D. P., & Kacmar, K. M. (1997). A cybernetic model of impression management processes in organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,69(1), 9–30.CrossRef Bozeman, D. P., & Kacmar, K. M. (1997). A cybernetic model of impression management processes in organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,69(1), 9–30.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell, D., Datar, S. M., Kulp, S. L., & Narayanan, V. G. (2015). Testing strategy with multiple performance measures: Evidence from a balanced scorecard at Store24. Journal of Management Accounting Research,27(2), 39–65.CrossRef Campbell, D., Datar, S. M., Kulp, S. L., & Narayanan, V. G. (2015). Testing strategy with multiple performance measures: Evidence from a balanced scorecard at Store24. Journal of Management Accounting Research,27(2), 39–65.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cardinaels, E. (2016). Earnings benchmarks, information systems, and their impact on the degree of honesty in managerial reporting. Accounting, Organizations and Society,52, 50–62.CrossRef Cardinaels, E. (2016). Earnings benchmarks, information systems, and their impact on the degree of honesty in managerial reporting. Accounting, Organizations and Society,52, 50–62.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chen, Y., Jermias, J., & Panggabean, T. (2016). The role of visual attention in the managerial judgment of balanced-scorecard performance evaluation: Insights from using an eye-tracking device. Journal of Accounting Research,54(1), 113–146.CrossRef Chen, Y., Jermias, J., & Panggabean, T. (2016). The role of visual attention in the managerial judgment of balanced-scorecard performance evaluation: Insights from using an eye-tracking device. Journal of Accounting Research,54(1), 113–146.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cheng, M. M., & Humphreys, K. A. (2012). The differential improvement effects of the strategy map and scorecard perspectives on managers’ strategic decisions. The Accounting Review,87(3), 899–924.CrossRef Cheng, M. M., & Humphreys, K. A. (2012). The differential improvement effects of the strategy map and scorecard perspectives on managers’ strategic decisions. The Accounting Review,87(3), 899–924.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cobb, G. W. (1998). Introduction to design and analysis of experiments. Berlin: Springer. Cobb, G. W. (1998). Introduction to design and analysis of experiments. Berlin: Springer.
Zurück zum Zitat Demidenko, E. (2004). Mixed models: Theory and applications. Wiley series in probability and statistics. New York: Wiley.CrossRef Demidenko, E. (2004). Mixed models: Theory and applications. Wiley series in probability and statistics. New York: Wiley.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dutton, J. E., Ashford, S. J., O’Neill, R. M., Hayes, E., & Wierba, E. E. (1997). Reading the wind: How middle managers assess the context for selling issues to top managers. Strategic Management Journal,18(5), 407–423.CrossRef Dutton, J. E., Ashford, S. J., O’Neill, R. M., Hayes, E., & Wierba, E. E. (1997). Reading the wind: How middle managers assess the context for selling issues to top managers. Strategic Management Journal,18(5), 407–423.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Edwards, K., & Smith, E. E. (1996). A disconfirmation bias in the evaluation of arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,71(1), 5.CrossRef Edwards, K., & Smith, E. E. (1996). A disconfirmation bias in the evaluation of arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,71(1), 5.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Epstein, M. J., & Wisner, P. S. (2001). Increasing corporate accountability: The external disclosure of balanced scorecard measures. Balanced Scorecard Report,3(4), 937. Epstein, M. J., & Wisner, P. S. (2001). Increasing corporate accountability: The external disclosure of balanced scorecard measures. Balanced Scorecard Report,3(4), 937.
Zurück zum Zitat Ewerhart, C., & Schmitz, P. W. (2000). “Yes men”, integrity, and the optimal design of incentive contracts. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization,43(1), 115–125.CrossRef Ewerhart, C., & Schmitz, P. W. (2000). “Yes men”, integrity, and the optimal design of incentive contracts. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization,43(1), 115–125.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ferreira, D., & Rezende, M. (2007). Corporate strategy and information disclosure. The Rand Journal of Economics,38(1), 164–184.CrossRef Ferreira, D., & Rezende, M. (2007). Corporate strategy and information disclosure. The Rand Journal of Economics,38(1), 164–184.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ferris, G. R., & Judge, T. A. (1991). Personnel/human resources management: A political influence perspective. Journal of Management,17(2), 447–488.CrossRef Ferris, G. R., & Judge, T. A. (1991). Personnel/human resources management: A political influence perspective. Journal of Management,17(2), 447–488.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gneezy, U., Gravert, C., Saccardo, S., & Tausch, F. (2017). A must lie situation–avoiding giving negative feedback. Games and Economic Behavior,102, 445–454.CrossRef Gneezy, U., Gravert, C., Saccardo, S., & Tausch, F. (2017). A must lie situation–avoiding giving negative feedback. Games and Economic Behavior,102, 445–454.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Haynes, C. M., & Kachelmeier, S. J. (1998). The effects of accounting contexts on accounting decisions: A synthesis of cognitive and economic perspectives in accounting experimentation. Journal of Accounting Literature,17, 97. Haynes, C. M., & Kachelmeier, S. J. (1998). The effects of accounting contexts on accounting decisions: A synthesis of cognitive and economic perspectives in accounting experimentation. Journal of Accounting Literature,17, 97.
Zurück zum Zitat Huelsbeck, D. P., Merchant, K. A., & Sandino, T. (2011). On testing business models. The Accounting Review,86(5), 1631–1654.CrossRef Huelsbeck, D. P., Merchant, K. A., & Sandino, T. (2011). On testing business models. The Accounting Review,86(5), 1631–1654.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jensen, M. C., & Meckling, W. H. (1992). Specific and general knowledge and organizational structure. Knowledge Management and Organizational Design,28, 17–18. Jensen, M. C., & Meckling, W. H. (1992). Specific and general knowledge and organizational structure. Knowledge Management and Organizational Design,28, 17–18.
Zurück zum Zitat Jones, E. E. (1964). Ingratiation. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Jones, E. E. (1964). Ingratiation. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Zurück zum Zitat Jones, E. E., Gergen, K. J., & Davis, K. E. (1962). Some determinants of reactions to being approved or disapproved as a person. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied,1, 613. Jones, E. E., Gergen, K. J., & Davis, K. E. (1962). Some determinants of reactions to being approved or disapproved as a person. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied,1, 613.
Zurück zum Zitat Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic self-presentation. Psychological Perspectives on the Self,1(1), 231–262. Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic self-presentation. Psychological Perspectives on the Self,1(1), 231–262.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaplan, R. S., & Norton, D. P. (2000). Having trouble with your strategy? Then map it. Harvard Business Review,78(5), 167. Kaplan, R. S., & Norton, D. P. (2000). Having trouble with your strategy? Then map it. Harvard Business Review,78(5), 167.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaplan, R. S., & Norton, D. P. (2001). The strategy-focused organization: How balanced scorecard companies thrive in the new business environment. Harvard: Harvard Business Press.CrossRef Kaplan, R. S., & Norton, D. P. (2001). The strategy-focused organization: How balanced scorecard companies thrive in the new business environment. Harvard: Harvard Business Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kaplan, R. S., & Norton, D. P. (2008). The execution premium: linking strategy to operations for competitive advantage. Harvard: Harvard Business Press. Kaplan, R. S., & Norton, D. P. (2008). The execution premium: linking strategy to operations for competitive advantage. Harvard: Harvard Business Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly, K., Presslee, A., & Webb, R. A. (2017). The effects of tangible rewards versus cash rewards in consecutive sales tournaments: A field experiment. The Accounting Review,92(6), 165–185.CrossRef Kelly, K., Presslee, A., & Webb, R. A. (2017). The effects of tangible rewards versus cash rewards in consecutive sales tournaments: A field experiment. The Accounting Review,92(6), 165–185.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kuang, X., & Moser, D. V. (2009). Reciprocity and the effectiveness of optimal agency contracts. The Accounting Review,84(5), 1671–1694.CrossRef Kuang, X., & Moser, D. V. (2009). Reciprocity and the effectiveness of optimal agency contracts. The Accounting Review,84(5), 1671–1694.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin,108(3), 480.CrossRef Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin,108(3), 480.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,37(11), 2098.CrossRef Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,37(11), 2098.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mastilak, C., Matuszewski, L., Miller, F., & Woods, A. (2012). Evaluating conflicting performance on driver and outcome measures: The effect of strategy maps. Journal of Management Control,23(2), 97–114.CrossRef Mastilak, C., Matuszewski, L., Miller, F., & Woods, A. (2012). Evaluating conflicting performance on driver and outcome measures: The effect of strategy maps. Journal of Management Control,23(2), 97–114.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Maxwell, S. E., Delaney, H. D., & Kelley, K. (2017). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective. Abingdon: Routledge.CrossRef Maxwell, S. E., Delaney, H. D., & Kelley, K. (2017). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective. Abingdon: Routledge.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Myers, J. L., Shinjo, M., & Duffy, S. A. (1987). Degree of causal relatedness and memory. Journal of Memory and Language,26(4), 453–465.CrossRef Myers, J. L., Shinjo, M., & Duffy, S. A. (1987). Degree of causal relatedness and memory. Journal of Memory and Language,26(4), 453–465.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Park, S. H., Westphal, J. D., & Stern, I. (2011). Set up for a fall: The insidious effects of flattery and opinion conformity toward corporate leaders. Administrative Science Quarterly,56(2), 257–302.CrossRef Park, S. H., Westphal, J. D., & Stern, I. (2011). Set up for a fall: The insidious effects of flattery and opinion conformity toward corporate leaders. Administrative Science Quarterly,56(2), 257–302.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Prendergast, C. (1993). A theory of “yes men”. The American Economic Review,83, 757–770. Prendergast, C. (1993). A theory of “yes men”. The American Economic Review,83, 757–770.
Zurück zum Zitat Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science,50(3), 755–769.CrossRef Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science,50(3), 755–769.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tayler, W. B. (2010). The balanced scorecard as a strategy-evaluation tool: The effects of implementation involvement and a causal-chain focus. The Accounting Review,85(3), 1095–1117.CrossRef Tayler, W. B. (2010). The balanced scorecard as a strategy-evaluation tool: The effects of implementation involvement and a causal-chain focus. The Accounting Review,85(3), 1095–1117.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Webb, A., Jeffrey, S. A., & Schulz, A. (2010). Factors affecting goal difficulty and performance when employees select their own performance goals: Evidence from the field. Journal of Management Accounting Research,22(1), 209–232.CrossRef Webb, A., Jeffrey, S. A., & Schulz, A. (2010). Factors affecting goal difficulty and performance when employees select their own performance goals: Evidence from the field. Journal of Management Accounting Research,22(1), 209–232.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., Jr., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research,37(2), 197–206.CrossRef Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., Jr., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research,37(2), 197–206.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
A strategy map’s effect on the feedback that middle managers pass along to upper management
verfasst von
Brian D. Knox
Publikationsdatum
04.02.2020
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Journal of Management Control / Ausgabe 4/2020
Print ISSN: 2191-4761
Elektronische ISSN: 2191-477X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00187-020-00293-1

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2020

Journal of Management Control 4/2020 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner