Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Group Decision and Negotiation 6/2015

Open Access 01.11.2015

Bounded Benefits of Representative Cooperativeness in Intergroup Negotiations

verfasst von: Özüm Saygı, Lindred L. Greer, Gerben A. Van Kleef, Carsten K. W. De Dreu

Erschienen in: Group Decision and Negotiation | Ausgabe 6/2015

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Although cooperation among representatives in intergroup negotiation can improve intergroup relations, when cooperation in such competitive settings is attributed to strategic goals of the outgroup, it may actually harm intergroup relations. Here we investigate the possibility that representative’s characteristics (prototypicality and competence) determine whether an outgroup representative’s cooperation (as opposed to competition) improves or harms intergroup relations. Study 1 showed that a cooperative outgroup representative (compared to a competitive representative) produced more favorable perceptions of the entire outgroup, and triggered constructive behavioral tendencies towards the outgroup when the outgroup representative was seen as prototypical, yet decreased such constructive tendencies when the representative was seen as peripheral. Study 2 showed that the outgroup representative’s cooperation triggered constructive behavioral tendencies only when the representative appeared as low in competence; when high in competence, the positive effect of representative cooperativeness on trust and constructive behavioral tendencies was mitigated. Implications for representative negotiation and intergroup relations are discussed.

1 Introduction

Intergroup conflict is ubiquitous across cultures, and is often regulated through some form of negotiation among representatives from both sides (De Dreu et al. 2014). Although representatives often help to resolve intergroup conflicts (e.g., Kelman 1995, 2005) and to establish more positive intergroup relations (Saygi et al. 2014), their behavior may also, deliberately or inadvertently, escalate the conflict and damage the relationships among competing groups.
Using a bottom-up approach to representative negotiation (De Dreu et al. 2014), we examine when and why cooperative behavior by the outgroup representative will improve or harm intergroup relations, including constructive behavioral tendencies defined as group members’ tendency to help and protect the outgroup (e.g., Cuddy et al. 2008; Fiske et al. 1999, 2002). One example of bottom-up influences in representative negotiations is when Kelman (1997) applied the problem solving approach to the Cyprus conflict by bringing together political leaders, negotiators, public-political constituencies through personal contact, speeches and interviews which led participants to change their attitude about the conflict as well as the opposing group in a positive way (Kelman 1995, 2005; Davidson and Montville 1981; Druckman et al. 1988). However recent poll data from Cyprus suggest that even if the outgroup representative is cooperative in the negotiations and make concessions on important issues, citizens would still vote “No” in a likely referendum due to lack of trust towards the outgroup representative’ cooperative intentions (Seeds of Peace 2013). As such, we would like to identify the boundary conditions for the positive effects of cooperativeness on constructive behavioral tendencies.
Our point of departure is that in order for cooperative behavior by the outgroup representative to improve intergroup relations, first the representative needs to be perceived as a typical outgroup member to ensure that the perception of the representative will spill over to outgroup perceptions and influence intergroup relations positively. Second, the cooperative behavior of the outgroup representative should not raise any doubt about the true intentions of the outgroup. We identify two key characteristics of the outgroup representative that block the positive effects of cooperativeness on intergroup relations, namely (1) the representative’s peripherality to the represented group (Oakes et al. 1991) and (2) the representative’s competence.

1.1 Theoretical Model and Overview of Studies

In our overall conceptual model, we define two pathways that explain why the cooperative behavior of an outgroup representative may influence the ingroup members’ constructive behavioral tendencies towards the outgroup members who were not even involved in the negotiations. Furthermore we identified two boundary conditions that may block the positive effects of representative cooperativeness on constructive behavioral tendencies. In two studies we investigated whether or not the representative characteristics, namely representative prototypicality and competence will interact with the cooperative behavior of the outgroup representative to influence constructive behavioral tendencies towards the outgroup. In part 1 we focus on the moderating role of representative prototypicality and in the second part of our model we focused on the moderating role of representative competence. Please see Fig. 1 for our overall conceptual model wherein part 1 and 2 are tested in Study 1 and 2 respectively.

1.2 Bottom-up Effects of Representative Cooperation on Intergroup Relations

Representative negotiation involves a two-level situation, in which the negotiation among individual representatives is embedded in a broader intergroup context, and the broader intergroup relations in turn depend on the way representatives negotiate (e.g., Knowles and Bassett 1976; Lickel et al. 2000; Saygi et al. 2014). Especially the second component, or the degree to which representatives’ behaviors impact broader intergroup relations, is poorly understood (De Dreu et al. 2014).
Current insights rest on relatively indirect lines of inquiry and evidence. First, the study of problem solving workshops suggests that constructive, face-to-face discussions between representatives are vital for the development of constructive intergroup relations (Burton 1969; Druckman et al. 1988; Fisher 2006; Tam et al. 2009). For example, Kelman (1995, 2005) applied this approach to the Israeli–Palestinian conflict by bringing together political leaders, negotiators, and constituencies through personal contact, speeches, and interviews.1 Consequently, cooperation between the representatives improved ingroup members’ view of the conflict as well as their perception of the entire outgroup (Davidson and Montville 1981; Fisher 2006). Second, the notion that a cooperative negotiation process among representatives can improve relations between groups resonates with the main principles of the intergroup contact theory (Pettigrew and Tropp 2006). The theory proposes that under four conditions - equal status among group members, common goals, intergroup cooperation, and support from authorities, a positive contact with an outgroup member can improve the perception of the entire outgroup (Allport 1954; Druckman 1968; Pettigrew 1998). For example, in the context of the Northern Ireland conflict between Catholics and Protestants, researchers examined the link between intergroup contact and action tendencies towards the outgroup. In addition to direct contact between members of opposing groups, they also examined “extended contact” which is defined as the cross-group friendship between another ingroup member and an outgroup member (Wright et al. 1997). In line with contact theory and the extended-contact hypothesis (Wright et al. 1997), they found that trust mediated the effect of both direct and extended contact on action tendencies towards the outgroup (Tam et al. 2009), suggesting that positive contact, such as through cooperation, can improve intergroup relations and resolve intergroup conflicts.
Although these related lines of inquiry are not directly concerned with representative negotiation, they suggest that cooperative communication during negotiation by representatives may positively influence intergroup relations. Negotiation processes are usually categorized as competitive or cooperative (Deutsch 1949; Pruitt 1967). When negotiation processes are competitive, parties use cheating, act hostile, d o not exchange information, and try to exploit one another. When negotiation processes are cooperative, there is exchange of information, parties make concessions, and refrain from hostile exchanges and deceitful tactics (Bazerman et al. 2008; De Dreu et al. 2007). Previous research mostly focused on the effect of cooperative/competitive communication on the negotiated outcome or outcome satisfaction. For example, when negotiation processes were competitive, even if the final outcome was the same, negotiators were less satisfied with the negotiated outcome when the negotiation process was marked by competitive rather than cooperative communication processes (e.g., Curhan et al. 2006; Druckman et al. 1988; Schei 2008; also see Van den Bos et al. 1998). Other research showed that communicating competitively increases fear and intergroup anxiety (Stephan and Stephan 1984; Wildschut et al. 2003), the cognitive salience of the intergroup boundary (Wilder 1986), and confirms the categorization of ingroup and outgroup (Tajfel and Turner 1979). Given this we would expect that competitive communication of the outgroup representative will negatively influence ingroup members’ outgroup perceptions regardless of the negotiated outcome.
Conversely, we would expect cooperative communication to positively influence ingroup members’ outgroup perceptions, regardless of the negotiated outcome. Indeed, in a first study of the effects of representative behavior on intergroup relations, Saygi et al. (2014) found that cooperative tendencies by the outgroup representative promoted positive perceptions of, and behavioral tendencies towards, the outgroup as a whole. From this it follows that when the outgroup representative is perceived as cooperative (rather than non-cooperative), ingroup members should engage in more constructive behavioral tendencies towards outgroup members, such as helping, protecting, and associating with the outgroup (e.g., Cuddy et al. 2008; Fiske et al. 1999, 2002).
However, as we argue in this paper, this should only be the case when the outgroup representative’s behavior is taken as a reliable signal of the entire outgroup’s intentions. Cooperation may not always be trusted (e.g., Kramer 1994, 2004a, b). Cooperation under certain conditions may be seen as manipulative and unauthentic, and could potentially backfire on the ingroup. We postulate here that one source of information that groups may use to infer the authenticity of the other group’s intentions is the type of representative the group chooses to send to represent itself. When groups send a representative that looks to be there for strategic purposes and to exact gains—such as a highly competent negotiator, the authenticity of the group’s cooperative behavior may be doubted, and the positive effects of cooperativeness on intergroup relations may be blocked. As such, it’s critical for groups to know which representative to send if they want to cooperate with the outgroup and want this cooperation to be seen as sincere. We therefore offer an important contribution to the literature on intergroup negotiations by identifying characteristics of a representative, which may make groups see cooperative behavior by the representative as being more or less authentic.

1.3 When Cooperation Backfires: The Role of Perceived Negotiator Characteristics

Both problem-solving workshops and contact theory suggest that a group member can create a positive image for the entire group by behaving in a cooperative manner during an exchange with the opposing group, which may in turn ameliorate intergroup relations (e.g., Brown et al. 1999). However, such member-to-group generalization rests on the assumption that the representing group member actually represents cooperative intentions of the entire outgroup. Signs that contradict such an assumption may mitigate generalization processes and abolish the positive effects of cooperative representative behavior on outgroup perceptions. One such representative characteristic that may block generalization processes is the representative’s perceived peripherality to the represented group (Oakes et al. 1991). Another such characteristic is the representative’s perceived competence. We explain below why these two perceived negotiator characteristics may be critical in determining whether or not representative cooperation is seen as authentic and representative of outgroup intentions.

1.3.1 Representative Prototypicality

In Study 1 we examined representative-outgroup generalization effects as a function of the outgroup representative’s prototypicality. According to self-categorization theory (Turner et al. 1987), group members vary in the extent to which they share common characteristics of their group that differentiate the group from other groups. Some group members are more prototypical, in that they possess more of those common characteristics, and hence they can be considered better examples of the group than others (Hogg et al. 1993). Group members who strongly match group prototypes can be referred to as prototypical group members and those who are less prototypical examples of their group can be referred to as peripheral group members (e.g., Jetten et al. 2002, 2003, 2006; Van Knippenberg 2000). Accordingly, representatives in intergroup negotiations may vary in the degree to which they are prototypical or peripheral members of the group they represent (Van Kleef et al. 2007).
Previous research has shown that positive contact with an individual outgroup member can generalize to the entire outgroup (e.g., Johnston and Hewstone 1992; Saygi et al. 2014; Weber and Crocker 1983), and this effect has been found even in the absence of direct contact (e.g., Turner et al. 2007). However, this generalization process is most likely when group membership is made salient (Vivian et al. 1997), such as by the prototypicality of the outgroup members (Oakes and Turner 1986; Oakes et al. 1991). For example, Brown et al. (1999) showed that positive attitudinal generalization was facilitated by encounters with prototypical outgroup members. Similarly, in stereotype change studies, stereotype-disconfirming information had a stronger effect on generalization when the information was associated with a prototypical outgroup member (Johnston and Hewstone 1992) or with an outgroup member who was a good fit to the category prototype (Rothbart and Lewis 1988).
The above suggests that, in representative negotiations, the cooperative behavior of the outgroup representative may spill over to influence the perceived cooperativeness of the entire outgroup, but only when the outgroup representative is a prototypical member. If the outgroup representative is a peripheral member, such generalization may be mitigated, and hence outgroup cooperativeness may not lead to increased outgroup cooperativeness (Hypothesis 1). Furthermore, we propose a moderated mediation model wherein cooperativeness of the outgroup representative indirectly (through perceived outgroup cooperativeness) influences constructive behavioral tendencies towards the outgroup. These mediated effects are expected to be stronger when the outgroup representative is a prototypical member rather than a peripheral member (Hypothesis 2). However, prototypicality of the outgroup representative can potentially influence the spread of any kind of representative characteristic to the entire outgroup. The effect is not specific to the cooperativeness of the outgroup representative. For example, if a prototypical outgroup representative is acting unethically in the negotiations, it would influence the perceived unethicality of the entire outgroup. Hence, we aimed to identify a moderator that specifically influences the effect of cooperativeness on intergroup relations.

1.3.2 Representative Competence

In Study 2, we examined the second boundary condition, which concerns the fact that intergroup negotiations are mostly marked by ample opportunities for distrust and hostile attributions given the mixed motive feature of negotiation (e.g., Fein and Hilton 1994; Kramer 1994, 2004a, b; Messick and Mackie 1989). Hence, even if a representative is a cooperative prototypical member of his group, the opposing party first needs to trust the cooperative intentions of the representative as well as the outgroup in order to improve outgroup perceptions. For example, in a recent report prepared for policy makers in Cyprus it was suggested that one of the main constraining factors for conflict resolution in Cyprus is that both Turkish and Greek Cypriots do not trust in the cooperative intentions of each other’s groups. They believe that there is an ulterior motive behind the outgroup representative’s cooperative behaviors. Hence, even if representatives behave in a cooperative way, due to lack of trust in the outgroup, cooperation during the negotiations fails to facilitate conflict resolution (Seeds of Peace 2013).
Along similar lines, Kramer developed a construct called intergroup paranoia which is defined as “beliefs-either false or exaggerated-that are held by members of one group that cluster around ideas of being harassed, threatened, harmed, subjugated, persecuted, accused, mistreated, wronged, tormented, disparaged, or vilified by a malevolent outgroup” (Kramer 2004a, b). He suggests that intergroup paranoia in the context of negotiations leads negotiators and their constituents to overly examine the meaning of each move by the other party and also to obsessively reanalyze both past and anticipated interactions with the other party. In fact, previous research shows that a history of hostility is not even essential to have a more biased and negative view of the outgroup. Brewer and her colleague showed that even a mere categorization of individuals into distinct groups can create an ingroup bias which translates into negative perceptions of the outgroup members such as untrustworthy, dishonest, and competitive (Brewer 1979; Brewer and Silver 1978). In a negotiation context such negative outgroup perception leads representatives and their constituents to be distrustful and suspicious of outgroup members and also to anticipate competitive negotiation behaviors from them (Insko and Schopler 1997). Ironically, this means that cooperative negotiation behaviors by an outgroup representative are counter-intuitive and rather unexpected (Benton and Druckman 1974; Steinel et al. 2010), and such counter-intuitive and unexpected behavior by the negotiators may lead outgroup members to overly examine the meaning of each move. Because cooperation compared to competition by the out-group representative is more unexpected and counter-intuitive, it should trigger additional processing of information and a deeper search for possible explanations. If true, additional cues that may explain the (unexpected) event should be picked up more and integrated better in judgments and decisions.
One such cue is whether the out-group representative appears competent. Highly competent people are in control of their behavior, they know what they are doing, and they pursue their goals effectively (Fiske et al. 2002). When unexpected and counter-intuitive behavior is performed by highly competent individuals, internal attributions to deliberate strategizing become more likely, and a highly competent outgroup representative may mitigate the positive effect of cooperative representative behavior on outgroup trust by creating a sneaky and untrustworthy image for his group rather than favorable perceptions (Boon and Holmes 1991). Thus, we hypothesized that the outgroup representative’s cooperation increases outgroup trust only when the outgroup representative has low competence, and the positive effect of representative cooperativeness on outgroup trust may be mitigated when the outgroup representative has high competence (Hypothesis 3). Furthermore, we predicted that cooperativeness of the outgroup representative leads to higher constructive behavioral tendencies through trust when the outgroup representative has low competence, but not when the outgroup representative has high competence (Hypothesis 4).

2 Study 1: Method

2.1 Sample and Design

Sixty-nine undergraduate students (39 female; \(M=22.42\,years, { SD}=5.8\)) at the University of Amsterdam participated for monetary compensation. Using a double blind procedure, participants were randomly assigned to a 2 (cooperative vs. competitive outgroup representative) \(\times \) 2 (peripheral vs. prototypical outgroup representative) between-subjects factorial design. Main dependent variables were perceived outgroup cooperativeness and constructive behavioral tendencies towards the outgroup.

2.2 Procedure and Materials

Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants were seated in separate cubicles in front of a computer. They read that they would be divided into two three-person groups based on their responses to a personality survey. After they completed the survey, all participants were told that they belonged to Group O, together with two other participants who responded similarly to the survey, and that they would engage in an intergroup negotiation with three other students who formed Group P. Participants were further told that one of the members in each group would be randomly chosen to represent their group in the intergroup negotiation, and that the other two members in each group would be able to observe the negotiation (see Van Kleef et al. 2007).
All participants were then shown the instructions for a multi-issue negotiation between Group O and Group P (adopted from De Dreu et al. 2006, see Appendix 1 for the pay-off schedule), in which they had to negotiate about the salary, salary increase, and duration of the contract. After they read the instructions, all participants were in fact placed in the observer role, and informed that they would receive feedback from their representative about the negotiation with the outgroup representative.
To manipulate the prototypicality of the outgroup representative, we gave participants additional information after the representative selection procedure (Van Kleef et al. 2007; Van Knippenberg and Van Knippenberg 2005). Participants in the prototypical [peripheral] condition received the following message: “A member of Group P is randomly selected by the computer to be the representative of Group P. There is one more thing about this group member that we want to share with you. According to his score on the personality test, he is [not] a typical member of Group P, which means that he thinks and acts [does not think and act] like other members of Group P. He is a very good example [not a good example] of a person with a P-type personality”. Finally we added an additional sentence to the peripheral condition to prevent participants from perceiving the peripheral outgroup member as pro-outgroup deviant. The sentence was “According to his score, he also does not have an O-type personality”. Next, participants learned that they could earn up to 30 euro as a group (10 euro maximum per person depending on the total number of points they earned on each issue) if the representatives managed to reach an agreement on all three issues within 10 min. If not, neither of the groups would receive any extra money. Finally, participants read that they would see the offers made by their own representative but not the offers made by the outgroup representative. However, they would be shown any messages from the outgroup representative to their representative. Participants did not actually interact with another person; rather, the outgroup representative’s behavior was simulated via pre-programmed messages sent over the computer (see Van Kleef et al. 2007).
The negotiation started and messages sent by the outgroup representative were shown to the participants. We manipulated the cooperativeness of the outgroup representative through these messages. Participants observed three cooperative/competitive messages by the outgroup representative in each round (e.g., “I not only want my group to be satisfied, I also want your group to be satisfied”, “I hear often that I am very nice”, and “Not everything is about money, I do not want anyone to be hurt in this negotiation” for cooperation; “I only want my group to be satisfied, it does not interest me how your group likes it”, “I hear often that I can be tough”, “Everything is about money here, not about whether we have a good time or not” for competition). Following the messages, they were told that their ingroup representative made an offer to the other group. The ingroup representative’s offers were linear concessions and were kept constant across the conditions. (Round 1: 20,000 salary/2.5 years contract/ 7 % salary increase; Round 2: 20,000 salary/2.5 years contract/6 % salary increase; Round 3:19,000 salary/ 2.5 years contract/ 6 % salary increase). In round 3, participants were told that while the representatives would continue to negotiate, they would answer a series of questions about their impressions thus far (see below). Because we were not interested in their reactions to the negotiated outcome but rather the effect of communication during the negotiation, we measured their behavioral tendencies towards the outgroup as well as perceived outgroup cooperativeness before the negotiations ended.
Outgroup cooperativeness was measured by five items (“cooperative”, “friendly”, “nice”, “trustworthy”, “honest”) (1 \(=\) Not at all, 7 \(=\) Very much) (Cuddy et al. 2008; \(\alpha =0.90\)). Constructive behavioral tendencies were measured with three items from the facilitation tendencies scale of Cuddy et al. (2007). Participants indicated to what extent they thought their group members would have (1) a tendency to help the outgroup, (2) to protect the outgroup, and finally (3) to associate with the outgroup (1 \(=\) Not at all, 7 \(=\) Very much). We asked participants about their group members’ tendencies, rather than their own tendencies, to reduce social desirability concerns (Cuddy et al. 2008). Please note that outgroup was defined as Group P members excluding the Group P representative.
Finally, participants completed a manipulation check for the cooperativeness and prototypicality of the outgroup representative. First, participants indicated to what extent they perceived the outgroup representative as “cooperative”, “friendly”, “nice”, “trustworthy”, and “honest”(1 \(=\) Not at all, 7 \(=\) Very much), \((\alpha =0.87)\). Secondly, we used nine items from Van Kleef et al. (2007) to check for the prototypicality manipulation (1 \(=\) Not at all, 7 \(=\) Very much), \((\alpha =\,0.92)\). All items are listed in Appendix 2.

3 Study 1: Results and Discussion

3.1 Manipulation Checks

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed that participants in the cooperative conditions perceived the outgroup representative as more cooperative \((M=4.95,\,SD=1.03)\) compared to participants in the competitive condition \((M=3.77,\,SD=1.20), F(1, 65)=19.02,\,p\,<\,.001, \upeta _{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=0.23\). ANOVA on perceived typicality of the outgroup representative showed that participants in the prototypical condition perceived the outgroup representative as more typical \((M=4.20,\,SD=0.89)\) compared to participants in the peripheral representative condition \((M=3.14,\,SD=1.28), F(1, 65)=16.05,\,p\,<\,.001, \upeta _{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=0.20\). No other effects reached significance, all \(F\hbox {s} < 2\), all \(p\hbox {s} > .05\). Thus, our manipulations were successful.

3.2 Hypothesis Testing

Table 1 shows means, standard deviations and correlations between the central variables in the current study. To test our proposed model (see Fig. 1), we used a regression based path analysis with the aid of the PROCESS macro developed by Hayes (2013) in order to estimate interactions and conditional indirect effects. Figure 2 includes three sub-models that we tested separately. Although our primary interest is the moderating role of prototypicality on the relationship between representative cooperativeness and outgroup cooperativeness, for the preciseness of our predictions (see Edwards and Lambert 2007) we also tested the moderating role of prototypicality on the relationship between representative cooperativeness and constructive behavioral tendencies (see Fig. 2 panel A). In Fig. 2 panel B, we tested Model 2 and 3 wherein we examined whether or not the prototypicality of the outgroup representative moderates the relationship between representative cooperativeness and outgroup cooperativeness (Hypothesis 1). Furthermore we estimated the conditional indirect effect of representative cooperativeness on constructive behavioral tendencies (via outgroup cooperativeness), contingent on representative prototypicality, which is calculated by multiplying a paths by path b. (Hypothesis 2; Hayes 2013).
Table 1
Means, standard deviations, and correlations among dependent variables (Study 1)
 
Mean (SD)
1
2
Cooperative representative
Competitive representative
Prototypical
Peripheral
Prototypical
Peripheral
1. Outgroup cooperativeness
5.14 (1.45)
3.30 (1.08)
3.27  (1.21)
4.34 (0.97)
1
 
2. Constructive behavioral tendencies (CBT)
3.95 (1.04)
3.25 (1.04)
3.14  (0.94)
2.80 (1.25)
.327**
1
\(=\) 69, SDs \(=\) standard deviations, ** p \(<\) .01
Consistent with Hypothesis 1, we found a significant interaction effect of representative cooperativeness and prototypicality on perceived outgroup cooperativeness (see Table 2). In line with our predictions, the conditional effect of representative cooperativeness on perceived outgroup cooperativeness in the prototypical representative condition was significant and estimated to be 1.867 with a bias adjusted and accelerated 95 % CI ranging from 1.066 to 2.666. The conditional effect of representative cooperativeness on perceived outgroup cooperativeness in the peripheral representative condition was significant and estimated a negative effect of \(-\)1.041 with a bias adjusted and accelerated 95 % CI ranging from \(-\)1.876 to \(-\)0.207. This confirms that a prototypical cooperative representative can improve perceived outgroup cooperativeness compared to a competitive prototypical representative. However, a peripheral cooperative outgroup representative actually produced lower perceived outgroup cooperativeness than a peripheral competitive outgroup representative. Furthermore, testing model 3 in Fig. 2 showed that in fact the conditional indirect effect of representative cooperativeness on constructive behavioral tendencies via outgroup cooperativeness was positive and significant in the prototypical representative condition, and negative and significant in the peripheral representative condition. These results support Hypotheses 1 and 2 (Table 3).
Table 2
Ordinary least squares regression model coefficients (standard errors in parentheses) (Study1)
Outcome
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
 
Constructive behavioral tendencies
Outgroup cooperativeness
Constructive behavioral tendencies
Predictor
 
Coefficient
p
 
Coefficient
p
 
Coefficient
p
Intercept
 
2.804 (0.26)
\(<\).001
 
4.341 (0.291)
\(<\).001
 
1.778 (0.533)
.001
Representative cooperativeness
\(c_{1 \rightarrow }\)
0.446 (0.374)
.238
\(a_{1 \rightarrow }\)
\(-\)1.041 (0.418)
.0153
\({{c_{1}}^{\prime }}{\rightarrow }\)
0.692 (0.381)
.074
Representative prototypicality
\(c_{2 \rightarrow }\)
0.333 (0.369)
.369
\(a_{2 \rightarrow }\)
\(-\)1.071 (0.412)
.012
\({{c_{2}}^{\prime }}{\rightarrow }\)
0.586 (0.377)
.124
Outgroup cooperativeness
      
\(b _{\rightarrow }\)
0.236 (0.108)
.032
Cooperativeness \(\times \) prototypicality
\(c_{3 \rightarrow }\)
0.364 (0.518)
.702
\(a_{3 \rightarrow }\)
2.907 (0.579)
\(<\).001
\({{c_{3}}^{\prime }}{\rightarrow }\)
\(-\)0.323 (0.594)
.589
Model \(R^{2}\)
 
0.144
.017
 
0.316
\(<\).001
 
0.204
.005
\(=\) 69, Representative cooperativeness is coded as 1 for cooperative and 0 for competitive representative. Representative prototypicality is coded as 1 for prototypical and 0 for peripheral representative. All coefficients are unstandardized
Table 3
Model coefficients for the conditional process model in Fig. 1
Moderator
Indirect effect
Direct effect
 
Effect (Boot SE)
95 % Bias-corrected Bootstrap CI
Effect
SE
p
Peripheral representative (coded as 0)
\(-\)0.246 (0.130)
\(-\)0.640 to \(-\)0.069
0.369
0.403
.363
Prototypical representative (coded as 1)
0.441 (0.441)
0.099 to 1.087
0.692
0.381
.074
Taken together, cooperation by the outgroup representative evoked higher constructive behavioral tendencies towards the entire outgroup because of improved cooperative perceptions of the entire outgroup, but only when the outgroup representative was a prototypical outgroup member. When the outgroup representative was a peripheral member, cooperativeness of the outgroup representative actually decreased perceived outgroup cooperativeness and hence lowered constructive behavioral tendencies. Clearly, cooperative negotiation strategies by out-group representatives do not always boost intergroup relations, but can also undermine favorable intergroup perceptions and constructive behavioral tendencies. It depends, first of all, on the representative’s prototypicality. In Study 2, we tested representative competence as the second boundary condition for the positive effects of cooperative behavior by the outgroup representative on constructive behavioral tendencies. In the study 2 we tested Hypothesis 3 and 4.

4 Study 2: Method

4.1 Sample and Design

Eighty-six undergraduate students (60 females; M \(=\) 21.12 years, SD \(=\) 4.5) at the University of Amsterdam participated in the study for monetary compensation. Using a double blind procedure, participants were randomly assigned to a 2 (cooperative/competitive outgroup representative) by 2 (low/high competence outgroup representative) between-subjects factorial design. Main dependent variables were outgroup trust and constructive behavioral tendencies towards the outgroup.

4.2 Procedure and Materials

The procedure was the same as Study 1, with one exception: In addition to cooperative/competitive messages, participants also saw 3 high/low competence related messages. These messages were: “my friends always tell me that I should be in politics, because I am really good at negotiations”, “people always tell me that I am really efficient because I can do multiple things at the same time”, “I do not easily lose control in this kind of situations” for high competence, and “people always tell me that I should not be in politics because I am not good at negotiations”, “people always tell me that I am not efficient because I cannot do multiple things at the same time”, “I easily lose control in this kind of situations” for low competence. We assessed constructive behavioral tendencies \((\alpha =0.73)\) as before and also included a 3-item outgroup trust scale (“To what extent do you believe the outgroup has a strong sense of justice?”, “To what extent do you believe the outgroup is trustworthy?”, “To what extent do you believe the outgroup is sneaky?” (reversed item) \((\alpha =0.77)\). We checked for the manipulation of cooperativeness of the outgroup representative as before \((\alpha =0.91)\), and for competence, by asking to what extent participants perceived the outgroup representative as experienced, skillful, competent, intelligent, efficient, and confident \((1=Not\, at\, all,\, 7=Very\, much; \alpha =0.85)\).

5 Study 2: Results

5.1 Manipulation Checks

Participants perceived the outgroup representative as more cooperative in the cooperative \((M=4.78,\,SD=0.96)\) compared to the competitive condition \((M=3.97,\,SD=1.29), F(1, 82)=11.17, p=.001, \upeta _{\mathrm{p}}^{2 }=.12\). Furthermore, participants in the high competence condition perceived the outgroup representative as more competent \((M=4.89,\,SD=0.76)\) than those in the low competence condition \((M=4.07,\,SD=1.07), F(1, 82)=16.63, p\,<\,.001, \upeta _{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=0.17\). No other effects reached significance, all \(F\hbox {s} < 2\), all \(p\hbox {s} > .05\). Thus our manipulation of cooperation and competence was successful.

5.2 Hypothesis Testing

Table 4 presents means, standard deviations and correlations between the central variables in Study 2. To test our proposed model in Fig. 1 we used the same analysis as we did in Study 1. Figure 3 shows the three sub-models that we tested separately. Similarly to Study 1, as a stringent test of our predictions (see Edwards and Lambert 2007), we tested the moderating role of competence both on the relationship between representative cooperativeness and outgroup trust and the relationship between representative cooperativeness and constructive behavioral tendencies despite the fact that our primary interest is the former. In Fig. 3 panel B, we tested Model 2 and 3 wherein we examined whether or not the competence of the outgroup representative moderates the relationship between representative cooperativeness and outgroup trust (Hypothesis 3). Furthermore we estimated the conditional indirect effect of representative cooperativeness on constructive behavioral tendencies (via outgroup cooperativeness), contingent on representative competence (Hypothesis 4).
Table 4
Means, standard deviations, and correlations among dependent variables (Study 2)
 
Mean (SD)
1
2
Cooperative representative
Competitive representative
High competence
Low competence
High competence
Low competence
1.Outgroup trust
3.76 (1.26)
4.85 (1.15)
3.35 (0.85)
2.94 (1.14)
1
 
2. Constructive behavioral tendencies (CBT)
3.52 (1.07)
4.08 (1.09)
3.00 (0.97)
3.27 (1.21)
.384**
1
\(=\) 86, SDs \(=\) standard deviations, ** p \(<\) .01
Consistent with Hypothesis 3, we found a significant interaction effect of representative cooperativeness and competence on outgroup trust (see Table 5). As predicted, the conditional effect of representative cooperativeness on outgroup trust in the low competence condition was significant and estimated to be 1.911 with a bias adjusted and accelerated 95 % CI ranging from 1.228 to 2.594. The conditional effect of representative cooperativeness on outgroup trust in the high competence condition was non-significant. This confirms that a highly competent cooperative outgroup representative did not improve trust compared to a competitive outgroup representative. Furthermore, testing model 3 in Fig. 3 showed that in fact the conditional indirect effect of representative cooperativeness on constructive behavioral tendencies via outgroup trust was positive and significant under low competent representative conditions, and non-significant under high competent representative conditions (see Table 6). These results support Hypotheses 3 and 4.
Table 5
Ordinary least squares regression model coefficients (standard errors in parentheses) (Study 2)
Outcome
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
 
Constructive behavioral tendencies
Outgroup trust
Constructive behavioral tendencies
Predictor
 
Coefficient
\(p\)
 
Coefficient
\(p\)
 
Coefficient
\(p\)
Intercept
 
3.273 (0.232)
\(<\).001
 
2.939 (0.237)
\(<\).001
 
2.482 (0.381)
.001
Representative cooperativeness
\(c_{1 \rightarrow }\)
0.812 (0.337)
.018
\(a_{1 \rightarrow }\)
1.912 (0.343)
\(<\).001
\({{c_{1}}^{\prime }}{\rightarrow }\)
0.297 (0.382)
.440
Representative competence
\(c_{2 \rightarrow }\)
\(-\)0.273 (0.329)
.409
\(a_{2 \rightarrow }\)
0.409 (0.335)
.226
\({{c_{2}}^{\prime }}{\rightarrow }\)
\(-\)0.383 (0.321)
.236
Outgroup trust
      
\(b \, _{\rightarrow }\)
0.269 (0.105)
.012
Cooperativeness \(\times \) competence
\(c_{3 \rightarrow }\)
\(-\)0.296 (0.471)
.532
\(a_{3 \rightarrow }\)
\(-\)1.502 (0.480)
.002
\({{c_{3}}^{\prime }}{\rightarrow }\)
0.109 (0.482)
.822
Model \(R^{2}\)
 
0.120
.015
 
0.293
\(<\).001
 
0.186
.002
\(=\) 86, Representative cooperativeness is coded as 1 for cooperative and 0 for competitive representative. Representative competence is coded as 1 for high and 0 for low competence. All coefficients are unstandardized
Table 6
Model coefficients for the conditional process model in Fig. 1
Moderator
Indirect effect
Direct effect
 
Effect (Boot SE)
95 % Bias-corrected bootstrap CI
Effect
SE
p
High competence representative (coded as 1)
0.110 (0.130)
\(-\)0.033 to 0.390
0.405
0.321
.210
Low competence representative (coded as 0)
0.514 (0.219)
0.124 to 0.960
0.297
0.382
.440

6 General Discussion

The idea that cooperation improves intergroup relations is widespread (e.g., Brown et al. 1999). Qualifying this, however, the current paper shows that cooperation by a peripheral outgroup representative decreased perceived outgroup cooperativeness and lowered constructive behavioral tendencies (as opposed to a competitive peripheral representative), and cooperation by a highly competent representative (as opposed to a low-competent representative) mitigated the positive effects of representative cooperativeness on outgoup trust and constructive tendencies. Furthermore, we observed that outgroup representative cooperativeness increased constructive behavioral tendencies via perceived outgroup cooperativeness (Study 1) and trust (Study 2) only when the outgroup representative was a prototypical member of the outgroup (Study 1) or had low competence (Study 2). Finally, outgroup representative cooperativeness lowered constructive behavioral tendencies via perceived outgroup cooperativeness when the outgroup representative was a peripheral member of the outgroup (Study 1).
In Study 1, we predicted that the effect of cooperative representative behavior on constructive behavioral tendencies would be weaker when the outgroup representative is a peripheral member of the group. But in fact we found that the effect of cooperative representative behavior on constructive behavioral tendencies even reversed and led to decreased constructive tendencies when the outgroup representative was a peripheral member of the outgroup. This suggests that perhaps there is a contrast effect between the perception of a peripheral representative and the perception of the outgroup (e.g., Simpson and Ostrom 1976). This indicates that appointing an external representative to a group to which he does not necessarily belong to may create a contrast effect on the perceptions of the outgroup and hence cooperative behavior of the representative may backfire.
In sum, our results show that cooperative behavior in intergroup negotiations can both improve and undermine intergroup relations. Intriguingly, it is especially when cooperative outgroup representatives are seen as peripheral and/or highly competent that intergroup relations tend to sour rather than flourish. Implications for negotiation and intergroup relations as well as some limitations of our studies are discussed below.
First, the present study provides first evidence that cooperative behavior by the out-group representative can have negative effects on in-group members’ perceptions of that outgroup, the extent to which they trust out-group members, and their inclination to approach out-group members in a constructive way. These findings are at odds with the common assumption that cooperation improves intergroup relations (e.g., Pettigrew and Tropp 2006), and indicate that cooperation by out-group representatives may backfire when the representative is perceived as peripheral and a-typical, and/or highly competent. Whereas cooperative negotiations may promote intergroup relations by creating more benign perceptions of, and behavioral tendencies towards, uninvolved out-group members, cooperative strategies may backfire and deteriorate intergroup relations when representatives are not seen as typical for their group, or when their high competence allows for alternative attributions. Future work in this area could seek to further illuminate the underlying mechanisms that explain when and why cooperative behavior by out-group representatives promotes, or blocks, the development of constructive intergroup relations.
Second, our findings also contribute to extent research that investigated the relationship between warmth (cooperative) and competence in person or group perceptions (for a review please see Kervyn et al. 2010). In this line of research, some researchers showed that warmth and competence are positively related (eg., Kelley 1950; Asch 1946; Anderson 1974), whereas others demonstrated that there is an orthogonal relationship between them (for a review, see Fiske et al. 2007) or that they are negatively related to each other (e.g.,Yzerbyt et al. 2005; Cuddy et al. 2004). Differently from previous research we examined the interactive relationship between cooperativeness and competence and found that higher competence mitigates the positive effects of representative cooperativeness on behavioral tendencies of the ingroup towards the outgroup. Our findings call for more research to explore the interactive relationship between cooperativeness and competence on person or group perception.
As such, our findings may also have relevance for representative selection. Whereas in-group members want competent leaders and representatives (Sears and Kinder 1985), our results suggest that communicating such competence while also seeking to cooperate with the out-group may actually undermine the positive effects of cooperation on intergroup relations. Likewise, Teixeira et al. (2011) uncovered that ingroup members sometimes prefer a representative who is pro-outgroup deviant member and, accordingly, an a-typical in-group member. Whereas such a pro-outgroup deviant may indeed help negotiations with the out-group, our results suggest ironic adverse effects on intergroup relations. Relatedly, although competence, power and status are sometimes used interchangeably in the literature, we believe that a clear distinction must be made when interpreting our results. As status may derive from respect to the person (e.g., Bendersky and Hays 2012), higher status of the outgroup representative may even facilitate trust and hence improve intergroup relations. Future research should investigate whether or not the cooperative behavior of high status outgroup representative would facilitate trust and hence improve intergroup relations.
Given the contrived laboratory settings, studies reported in this article may have limited ecological validity due to the lack of real groups or decisions with high stakes in the laboratory. However, both these challenges can be addressed: actual groups or their representatives can be brought into the laboratories; negotiating representatives can be confronted with high-stakes scenarios as developed by Cuhadar and Druckman (2014). In some real life contexts, ingroup members would be able to see their representative and be in contact with their fellow group members, which may potentially influence their perception of the outgroup (e.g., Castelli et al. 2003; Cialdini and Goldstein 2004). The ecological validity of research on representative negotiations would be improved by taking these factors into account. The trade off is between the internal validity obtained through control over variables and the external or ecological validity obtained through field studies. We chose to emphasize the former in our experiments but encourage further studies that achieve a balance between these validities. (See Druckman 2005, for a discussion of these issues in conflict research.)
For if the current findings hold, cooperation by a representative can indeed have counterproductive effects on intergroup relations. Having a-typical, or highly competent representatives may not be the best strategy to create constructive exchange among competing groups.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.
Anhänge

Appendix 1

See Table 7.
Table 7
Pay-off schedule for Group O
Salary
Duration of the contract
Salary increase
14,000 (00)
0.5 years (00)
1 % (00)
15,000 (90)
1 year (30)
2 % (15)
16,000 (180)
1.5 years (60)
3 % (30)
17,000 (270)
2 years (90)
4 % (45)
18,000 (360)
2.5 years (120)
5 % (60)
19,000 (450)
 
6 % (75)
20,000 (540)
 
7 % (90)
Numbers in parentheses represent the number of points that can be earned if representatives agree on that level

Appendix 2: Manipulation Check Items for Prototypicality

On a scale from 1(not at all) to 7(very much), please indicate to what extent you would agree with the following statements:
1.
He is a very good example of his group.
 
2.
He embodies the P-type personality very well.
 
3.
He thinks and behaves like other Group P-members.
 
4.
He is a very typical member of Group P.
 
5.
He is very similar to Group P-members with respect to P-type personality.
 
6.
According to his score, he would not be a good example of how a typical person with P-type personality thinks and behaves. (reversed-coded)
 
7.
He is a good reflection of the people in his group.
 
8.
He represents what is characteristic of his group.
 
9.
He has a lot in common with his group.
 
Fußnoten
1
Because these intervention studies lacked proper baseline conditions, did not control for the specifics of the interactions between protagonists, and did not take place in the explicitly competitive and conflictual negotiation context, they are suggestive rather than conclusive about the possibility that representative negotiations can have a bottom-up influence on broader intergroup relations. Furthermore, these investigations did not specifically focus on the cooperative behavior of the representative but rather on the broader sense of positive contact between representatives. However, they are suggestive of the benefits cooperative representative behavior may have for intergroup relations.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Allport GW (1954) The nature of prejudice. Addison-Wesley, Cambridge, MA Allport GW (1954) The nature of prejudice. Addison-Wesley, Cambridge, MA
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson NH (1974) Cognitive algebra integration theory applied to social attribution. In: Berkowitz L (ed) Advances in experimental social psychology, vol 7. Academic Press, New York, pp 1–101 Anderson NH (1974) Cognitive algebra integration theory applied to social attribution. In: Berkowitz L (ed) Advances in experimental social psychology, vol 7. Academic Press, New York, pp 1–101
Zurück zum Zitat Asch SE (1946) Forming impressions of personality. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 41:258–290CrossRef Asch SE (1946) Forming impressions of personality. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 41:258–290CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bazerman MH, Tenbrunsel A, Wade-Benzoni K (2008) When “sacred” issues are at stake. Negot J 24(1):113–117CrossRef Bazerman MH, Tenbrunsel A, Wade-Benzoni K (2008) When “sacred” issues are at stake. Negot J 24(1):113–117CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bendersky C, Hays NA (2012) Status conflict in groups. Organ Sci 23(2):323–340CrossRef Bendersky C, Hays NA (2012) Status conflict in groups. Organ Sci 23(2):323–340CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Benton AA, Druckman D (1974) Constituent’s bargaining orientation and intergroup negotiations. J Appl Soc Psychol 4:141–150CrossRef Benton AA, Druckman D (1974) Constituent’s bargaining orientation and intergroup negotiations. J Appl Soc Psychol 4:141–150CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Boon SD, Holmes JG (1991) The dynamics of interpersonal trust: resolving uncertainty in the face of risk. Coop Prosocial Behav 190–211 Boon SD, Holmes JG (1991) The dynamics of interpersonal trust: resolving uncertainty in the face of risk. Coop Prosocial Behav 190–211
Zurück zum Zitat Brown R, Vivian J, Hewstone M (1999) Changing attitudes through intergroup contact: the effects of group membership salience. Eur J Soc Psychol 29(5–6):741–764CrossRef Brown R, Vivian J, Hewstone M (1999) Changing attitudes through intergroup contact: the effects of group membership salience. Eur J Soc Psychol 29(5–6):741–764CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Burton JW (1969) Conflict and communication: the use of controlled communication in international relations. MacMillan, London Burton JW (1969) Conflict and communication: the use of controlled communication in international relations. MacMillan, London
Zurück zum Zitat Castelli L, Arcuri L, Zogmaister C (2003) Perceiving ingroup members who use stereotypes: implicit conformity and similarity. Eur J Soc Psychol 33(2):163–175. doi:10.1002/ejsp.138 CrossRef Castelli L, Arcuri L, Zogmaister C (2003) Perceiving ingroup members who use stereotypes: implicit conformity and similarity. Eur J Soc Psychol 33(2):163–175. doi:10.​1002/​ejsp.​138 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ (2004) Social influence: compliance and conformity. Annu Rev Psychol 55:591–621CrossRef Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ (2004) Social influence: compliance and conformity. Annu Rev Psychol 55:591–621CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cuddy AJ, Fiske ST, Glick P (2004) When professionals become mothers, warmth doesn’t cut the ice. J Soc Issues 60(4):701–718 Cuddy AJ, Fiske ST, Glick P (2004) When professionals become mothers, warmth doesn’t cut the ice. J Soc Issues 60(4):701–718
Zurück zum Zitat Cuddy AJ, Fiske ST, Glick P (2007) The BIAS map: behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. J Pers Soc Psychol 92(4):631CrossRef Cuddy AJ, Fiske ST, Glick P (2007) The BIAS map: behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. J Pers Soc Psychol 92(4):631CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cuddy AJ, Fiske ST, Glick P (2008) Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: the stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 40:61–149CrossRef Cuddy AJ, Fiske ST, Glick P (2008) Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: the stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 40:61–149CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cuhadar E, Druckman D (2014) Representative decision-making: challenges to democratic peace theory. In: Glluccio M (ed) Handbook of international negotiation: interpersonal, intercultural, and diplomatic perspectives. Springer, New York Cuhadar E, Druckman D (2014) Representative decision-making: challenges to democratic peace theory. In: Glluccio M (ed) Handbook of international negotiation: interpersonal, intercultural, and diplomatic perspectives. Springer, New York
Zurück zum Zitat Davidson W, Montville J (1981) Foreign policy according to Freud. Foreign Policy 45:145–157CrossRef Davidson W, Montville J (1981) Foreign policy according to Freud. Foreign Policy 45:145–157CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat De Dreu CKW, Aaldering H, Saygi O (2014) Conflict within and between groups. In: Dovidio J, Simpson J (eds) APA handbook of interpersonal and group relations, vol 2. APA Press, Washington, DC, pp 151–176 De Dreu CKW, Aaldering H, Saygi O (2014) Conflict within and between groups. In: Dovidio J, Simpson J (eds) APA handbook of interpersonal and group relations, vol 2. APA Press, Washington, DC, pp 151–176
Zurück zum Zitat De Dreu CKW, Beersma B, Steinel W, Van Kleef GA (2007) The psychology of negotiation: principles and basic processes. In: Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET (eds) Social psychology: handbook of basic principles, 2nd edn. Guilford, New York, pp 608–629 De Dreu CKW, Beersma B, Steinel W, Van Kleef GA (2007) The psychology of negotiation: principles and basic processes. In: Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET (eds) Social psychology: handbook of basic principles, 2nd edn. Guilford, New York, pp 608–629
Zurück zum Zitat De Dreu CK, Beersma B, Stroebe K, Euwema MC (2006) Motivated information processing, strategic choice, and the quality of negotiated agreement. J Pers Soc Psychol 90(6):927CrossRef De Dreu CK, Beersma B, Stroebe K, Euwema MC (2006) Motivated information processing, strategic choice, and the quality of negotiated agreement. J Pers Soc Psychol 90(6):927CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Deutsch M (1949) A theory of cooperation and competition. Hum Relat 2:129–151CrossRef Deutsch M (1949) A theory of cooperation and competition. Hum Relat 2:129–151CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman D (1968) Ethnocentrism in the inter-nation simulation. J Confl Resolut 12(1):45–68CrossRef Druckman D (1968) Ethnocentrism in the inter-nation simulation. J Confl Resolut 12(1):45–68CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman D (2005) Doing research: methods of inquiry for conflict analysis. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA Druckman D (2005) Doing research: methods of inquiry for conflict analysis. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman D, Broome B, Korber S (1988) Value differences and conflict resolution: facilitation or delinking? J Confl Resolut 3:489–510CrossRef Druckman D, Broome B, Korber S (1988) Value differences and conflict resolution: facilitation or delinking? J Confl Resolut 3:489–510CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Edwards JR, Lambert LS (2007) Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: a general analytical framework using moderated path analysis. Psychol Methods 12:1–22 Edwards JR, Lambert LS (2007) Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: a general analytical framework using moderated path analysis. Psychol Methods 12:1–22
Zurück zum Zitat Fein S, Hilton JL (1994) Judging others in the shadow of suspicion. Motiv Emot 18(2):167–198CrossRef Fein S, Hilton JL (1994) Judging others in the shadow of suspicion. Motiv Emot 18(2):167–198CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fisher RJ (2006) The problem solving workshop as a method of research. In: Carnevale PJ, De Dreu CKW (eds) Methods of negotiation research. Martinus Nijhoff, Boston, pp 49–60 Fisher RJ (2006) The problem solving workshop as a method of research. In: Carnevale PJ, De Dreu CKW (eds) Methods of negotiation research. Martinus Nijhoff, Boston, pp 49–60
Zurück zum Zitat Fiske ST, Cuddy AJ, Glick P, Xu J (2002) A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J Pers Soc Psychol 82(6):878–902CrossRef Fiske ST, Cuddy AJ, Glick P, Xu J (2002) A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J Pers Soc Psychol 82(6):878–902CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fiske ST, Xu J, Cuddy AJC, Glick P (1999) (Dis)Respecting versus (dis)liking: status and interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of competence and warmth. J Soc Issues 55:473–489CrossRef Fiske ST, Xu J, Cuddy AJC, Glick P (1999) (Dis)Respecting versus (dis)liking: status and interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of competence and warmth. J Soc Issues 55:473–489CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fiske ST, Cuddy AJ, Glick P (2007) Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence. Trends Cogn Sci 11(2):77–83 Fiske ST, Cuddy AJ, Glick P (2007) Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence. Trends Cogn Sci 11(2):77–83
Zurück zum Zitat Hayes AF (2013) Introduction to mediation, moderation and conditional process analysis. Guilford Press, New York Hayes AF (2013) Introduction to mediation, moderation and conditional process analysis. Guilford Press, New York
Zurück zum Zitat Hogg MA, Cooper-Shaw L, Holzworth DW (1993) Group prototypicality and depersonalized attraction in small interactive groups. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 19:452–465CrossRef Hogg MA, Cooper-Shaw L, Holzworth DW (1993) Group prototypicality and depersonalized attraction in small interactive groups. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 19:452–465CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Insko CA, Schopler J (1997) Differential distrust of groups and individuals. In: Sedikides C, Schopler J, Insko C (eds) Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp 75–108 Insko CA, Schopler J (1997) Differential distrust of groups and individuals. In: Sedikides C, Schopler J, Insko C (eds) Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp 75–108
Zurück zum Zitat Jetten J, Branscombe NR, Spears R (2002) On being peripheral: effects of insecurity on personal and collective self-esteem. Eur J Soc Psychol 32:105–123CrossRef Jetten J, Branscombe NR, Spears R (2002) On being peripheral: effects of insecurity on personal and collective self-esteem. Eur J Soc Psychol 32:105–123CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jetten J, Branscombe NR, Spears R, McKimmie BM (2003) Predicting the paths of peripherals: the interaction of identification and future possibilities. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 29:130–140CrossRef Jetten J, Branscombe NR, Spears R, McKimmie BM (2003) Predicting the paths of peripherals: the interaction of identification and future possibilities. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 29:130–140CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jetten J, Hornsey MJ, Adarves-Yorno I (2006) When group members admit to being conformist: the role of relative intragroup status in conformity self-reports. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 32:162–173CrossRef Jetten J, Hornsey MJ, Adarves-Yorno I (2006) When group members admit to being conformist: the role of relative intragroup status in conformity self-reports. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 32:162–173CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Johnston L, Hewstone M (1992) Cognitive models of stereotype change: 3. Subtyping and the perceived typicality of disconfirming group members. J Exp Soc Psychol 28(4):360–386CrossRef Johnston L, Hewstone M (1992) Cognitive models of stereotype change: 3. Subtyping and the perceived typicality of disconfirming group members. J Exp Soc Psychol 28(4):360–386CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kelley HH (1950) The warm–cold variable in first impressions of persons. J Pers 18:431–439CrossRef Kelley HH (1950) The warm–cold variable in first impressions of persons. J Pers 18:431–439CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kelman HC (1995) Contributions of an unofficial conflict resolution effort to the Israeli–Palestinian breakthrough. Negot J 11:19–27CrossRef Kelman HC (1995) Contributions of an unofficial conflict resolution effort to the Israeli–Palestinian breakthrough. Negot J 11:19–27CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kelman H (1997) Social-psychological dimensions of international conflict. In: Zartman IW, Rasmussen JL (eds) Peacemaking in international conflict: methods and techniques. U.S. Institute of Peace, Washington, DC, pp 191–237 Kelman H (1997) Social-psychological dimensions of international conflict. In: Zartman IW, Rasmussen JL (eds) Peacemaking in international conflict: methods and techniques. U.S. Institute of Peace, Washington, DC, pp 191–237
Zurück zum Zitat Kelman HC (2005) Interactive problem solving in the Israeli–Palestinian case: past contributions and present challenges. In: Fisher RJ (ed) Paving the way: contributions of interactive conflict resolution to peacemaking. Lexington Books, Lanham, MD, pp 41–63 Kelman HC (2005) Interactive problem solving in the Israeli–Palestinian case: past contributions and present challenges. In: Fisher RJ (ed) Paving the way: contributions of interactive conflict resolution to peacemaking. Lexington Books, Lanham, MD, pp 41–63
Zurück zum Zitat Kervyn N, Yzerbyt V, Judd CM (2010) Compensation between warmth and competence: antecedents and consequences of a negative relation between the two fundamental dimensions of social perception. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 21(1):155–187CrossRef Kervyn N, Yzerbyt V, Judd CM (2010) Compensation between warmth and competence: antecedents and consequences of a negative relation between the two fundamental dimensions of social perception. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 21(1):155–187CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Knowles ES, Bassett R (1976) Groups and crowds as social entities: effects of activity, size, and member similarity on nonmembers. J Pers Soc Psychol 34:837–845CrossRef Knowles ES, Bassett R (1976) Groups and crowds as social entities: effects of activity, size, and member similarity on nonmembers. J Pers Soc Psychol 34:837–845CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kramer RM (1994) The sinister attribution error: paranoid cognition and collective distrust in organizations. Motiv Emot 18(2):199–230CrossRef Kramer RM (1994) The sinister attribution error: paranoid cognition and collective distrust in organizations. Motiv Emot 18(2):199–230CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kramer RM (2004a) Collective paranoia: distrust between social groups. In: Hardin R (ed) Distrust. Russell Sage, New York, NY, pp 136–166 Kramer RM (2004a) Collective paranoia: distrust between social groups. In: Hardin R (ed) Distrust. Russell Sage, New York, NY, pp 136–166
Zurück zum Zitat Kramer RM (2004b) The “dark side” of social context: the role of intergroup paranoia in intergroup negotiations. Handb Negot Cult 219–237 Kramer RM (2004b) The “dark side” of social context: the role of intergroup paranoia in intergroup negotiations. Handb Negot Cult 219–237
Zurück zum Zitat Lickel B, Hamilton DL, Wieczorkowska G, Lewis A, Sherman SJ, Uhles AN (2000) Varieties of groups and the perception of group entitativity. J Pers Soc Psychol 78:223–246CrossRef Lickel B, Hamilton DL, Wieczorkowska G, Lewis A, Sherman SJ, Uhles AN (2000) Varieties of groups and the perception of group entitativity. J Pers Soc Psychol 78:223–246CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Oakes P, Turner JC (1986) Distinctiveness and the salience of social category memberships: is there an automatic perceptual bias towards novelty? Eur J Soc Psychol 16(4):325–344 Oakes P, Turner JC (1986) Distinctiveness and the salience of social category memberships: is there an automatic perceptual bias towards novelty? Eur J Soc Psychol 16(4):325–344
Zurück zum Zitat Oakes PJ, Turner JC, Haslam SA (1991) Perceiving people as group members: the role of fit in the salience of social categorizations. Br J Soc Psychol 30(2):125–144CrossRef Oakes PJ, Turner JC, Haslam SA (1991) Perceiving people as group members: the role of fit in the salience of social categorizations. Br J Soc Psychol 30(2):125–144CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pettigrew TF, Tropp LR (2006) A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. J Pers Soc Psychol 90(5):751CrossRef Pettigrew TF, Tropp LR (2006) A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. J Pers Soc Psychol 90(5):751CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rothbart M, Lewis S (1988) Inferring category attributes from exemplar attributes: geometric shapes and social categories. J Pers Soc Psychol 55(6):861CrossRef Rothbart M, Lewis S (1988) Inferring category attributes from exemplar attributes: geometric shapes and social categories. J Pers Soc Psychol 55(6):861CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Saygi O, Greer LL, van Kleef GA, De Dreu CKW (2014) Competitive representative negotiations worsen intergroup relations. Group Process Intergroup Relat 17(2):143–160CrossRef Saygi O, Greer LL, van Kleef GA, De Dreu CKW (2014) Competitive representative negotiations worsen intergroup relations. Group Process Intergroup Relat 17(2):143–160CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sears DO, Kinder D (1985) Public opinion and political action. Handb Soc Psychol 2(3):659–661 Sears DO, Kinder D (1985) Public opinion and political action. Handb Soc Psychol 2(3):659–661
Zurück zum Zitat Steinel W, Van Kleef GA, Van Knippenberg D, Hogg MA, Homan AC, Moffitt G (2010) How intragroup dynamics affect behavior in intergroup conflict: the role of group norms, prototypicality, and need to belong. Group Process Intergroup Relat 13(6):779–794CrossRef Steinel W, Van Kleef GA, Van Knippenberg D, Hogg MA, Homan AC, Moffitt G (2010) How intragroup dynamics affect behavior in intergroup conflict: the role of group norms, prototypicality, and need to belong. Group Process Intergroup Relat 13(6):779–794CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stephan WG, Stephan CW (1984) The role of ignorance in intergroup relations. In: Miller N, Brewer MB (eds) Groups in contact: the psychology of desegregation. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp 229–257CrossRef Stephan WG, Stephan CW (1984) The role of ignorance in intergroup relations. In: Miller N, Brewer MB (eds) Groups in contact: the psychology of desegregation. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp 229–257CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tam T, Hewstone M, Kenworthy JB, Cairns E (2009) Intergroup trust in Northern Ireland. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 35:45–59CrossRef Tam T, Hewstone M, Kenworthy JB, Cairns E (2009) Intergroup trust in Northern Ireland. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 35:45–59CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tajfel H, Turner JC (1979) An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In: Austin W, Worchel S (eds) The social psychology of intergroup relations. Brooks/Cole, Monterey, CA, pp 33–47 Tajfel H, Turner JC (1979) An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In: Austin W, Worchel S (eds) The social psychology of intergroup relations. Brooks/Cole, Monterey, CA, pp 33–47
Zurück zum Zitat Teixeira CP, Demoulin S, Yzerbyt VY (2011) Choosing the best means to an end: the influence of ingroup goals on the selection of representatives in intergroup negotiations. J Exp Soc Psychol 47(1):228–234CrossRef Teixeira CP, Demoulin S, Yzerbyt VY (2011) Choosing the best means to an end: the influence of ingroup goals on the selection of representatives in intergroup negotiations. J Exp Soc Psychol 47(1):228–234CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Turner JC, Hogg MA, Oakes PJ, Reicher SD, Wetherell MS (1987) Rediscovering the social group: a self-categorization theory. Basil Blackwell Turner JC, Hogg MA, Oakes PJ, Reicher SD, Wetherell MS (1987) Rediscovering the social group: a self-categorization theory. Basil Blackwell
Zurück zum Zitat Turner RN, Hewstone M, Voci A (2007) Reducing explicit and implicit outgroup prejudice via direct and extended contact: the mediating role of self-disclosure and intergroup anxiety. J Pers Soc Psychol 93(3):369–388CrossRef Turner RN, Hewstone M, Voci A (2007) Reducing explicit and implicit outgroup prejudice via direct and extended contact: the mediating role of self-disclosure and intergroup anxiety. J Pers Soc Psychol 93(3):369–388CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van den Bos K, Wilke HAM, Lind EA, Vermunt R (1998) Evaluating outcomes by means of the fair process effect: evidence for different processes in fairness and satisfaction judgments. J Pers Soc Psychol 74(6):1493–1503. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1493 CrossRef Van den Bos K, Wilke HAM, Lind EA, Vermunt R (1998) Evaluating outcomes by means of the fair process effect: evidence for different processes in fairness and satisfaction judgments. J Pers Soc Psychol 74(6):1493–1503. doi:10.​1037/​0022-3514.​74.​6.​1493 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van Kleef GA, Steinel W, Van Knippenberg D, Hogg M, Svensson A (2007) Group member prototypicality and intergroup negotiation: how one’s standing in the group affects negotiation behaviour. Br J Soc Psychol 46:129–154CrossRef Van Kleef GA, Steinel W, Van Knippenberg D, Hogg M, Svensson A (2007) Group member prototypicality and intergroup negotiation: how one’s standing in the group affects negotiation behaviour. Br J Soc Psychol 46:129–154CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van Knippenberg D (2000) Work motivation and performance: a social identity perspective. Appl Psychol 49(3):357–371CrossRef Van Knippenberg D (2000) Work motivation and performance: a social identity perspective. Appl Psychol 49(3):357–371CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van Knippenberg B, Van Knippenberg D (2005) Leader self-sacrifice and leadership effectiveness: the moderating role of leader prototypicality. J Appl Psychol 90(1):25–37CrossRef Van Knippenberg B, Van Knippenberg D (2005) Leader self-sacrifice and leadership effectiveness: the moderating role of leader prototypicality. J Appl Psychol 90(1):25–37CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Vivian J, Hewstone M, Brown R (1997) Intergroup contact: theoretical and empirical developments. In: Ben-Ari R, Rich Y (eds) Enhancing education in heterogeneous classrooms: theory and application. Studies in memory of Yehuda Amir. Bar-Ilan University Press, Ramat-Gan, pp 13–46 Vivian J, Hewstone M, Brown R (1997) Intergroup contact: theoretical and empirical developments. In: Ben-Ari R, Rich Y (eds) Enhancing education in heterogeneous classrooms: theory and application. Studies in memory of Yehuda Amir. Bar-Ilan University Press, Ramat-Gan, pp 13–46
Zurück zum Zitat Weber R, Crocker J (1983) Cognitive processes in the revision of stereotypic beliefs. J Pers Soc Psychol 45(5):961–977CrossRef Weber R, Crocker J (1983) Cognitive processes in the revision of stereotypic beliefs. J Pers Soc Psychol 45(5):961–977CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wilder DA (1986) Social categorization: implications for creation and reduction of intergroup bias. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 19:291–355CrossRef Wilder DA (1986) Social categorization: implications for creation and reduction of intergroup bias. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 19:291–355CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wildschut T, Pinter B, Vevea JL, Insko CA, Schopler J (2003) Beyond the group mind: a quantitative review of the interindividual–intergroup discontinuity effect. Psychol Bull 129(5):698–722CrossRef Wildschut T, Pinter B, Vevea JL, Insko CA, Schopler J (2003) Beyond the group mind: a quantitative review of the interindividual–intergroup discontinuity effect. Psychol Bull 129(5):698–722CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yzerbyt V, Provost V, Corneille O (2005) Not competent but warm.. really? Compensatory stereotypes in the French-speaking world. Group Process Intergroup Relat 8(3):291–308CrossRef Yzerbyt V, Provost V, Corneille O (2005) Not competent but warm.. really? Compensatory stereotypes in the French-speaking world. Group Process Intergroup Relat 8(3):291–308CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Bounded Benefits of Representative Cooperativeness in Intergroup Negotiations
verfasst von
Özüm Saygı
Lindred L. Greer
Gerben A. Van Kleef
Carsten K. W. De Dreu
Publikationsdatum
01.11.2015
Verlag
Springer Netherlands
Erschienen in
Group Decision and Negotiation / Ausgabe 6/2015
Print ISSN: 0926-2644
Elektronische ISSN: 1572-9907
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10726-014-9426-2

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 6/2015

Group Decision and Negotiation 6/2015 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner