Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 2/2012

01.06.2012 | Original Paper

Not That There’s Anything Wrong with That: The Effect of Personalized Appeals on Marriage Equality Campaigns

verfasst von: Brian F. Harrison, Melissa R. Michelson

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 2/2012

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

An increasingly predominant strategy used by organizations seeking to increase support for gay marriage is to personalize the issue by focusing on individuals in the LGBT community. However, competing theoretical traditions (e.g., Allport’s contact theory, group threat, implicit bias) raise questions about whether this strategy has the desired effect. This paper presents results from an original field experiment conducted in coordination with a marriage equality organization. Callers who self-identified as a member of the LGBT community were less effective in soliciting donations compared to callers who did not self-identify, suggesting that personalization has a negative effect on persuasion efforts. The findings cut against the grain of the Allport (The nature of prejudice, 1954) hypothesis and have important implications for social advocacy organizations in terms of rhetorical and message strategy.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
Missouri voters approved a same-sex marriage-ban amendment August 2, 2004; Louisiana voters on September 18, 2004. Voters in Arkansas, Georgia, Kentucky, Michigan, Mississippi, Montana, North Dakota, Ohio, Oklahoma, Oregon and Utah approved their amendments on Election Day (Peterson 2004). Although pundits claimed that the presence of ballot initiatives proposing gay marriage bans led to increased turnout among conservative voters and thus contributed to the victory of Republican candidate George W. Bush, scholars have found mixed evidence as to whether or not such claims are warranted (see Smith et al. 2006; Campbell and Monson 2008).
 
2
According to the New York Times, gay marriage is supported by a majority of residents of 17 states, while as recently as 2004 it did not have majority support in any state (Gellman et al. 2010). Also, Gallup Poll data from May 3–6, 2010 found that support for gay marriage was up to 46%, significantly larger than the 27% level of support measured when Gallup first asked the question in 1996. The same poll found that in 2010, the percentage of Americans who believe homosexual relations are morally acceptable crossed the symbolic 50% line for the first time (Saad 2010).
 
3
Personal Communication with Thomas Wheatley, Organizing Director, Basic Rights Oregon, February 8, 2010.
 
4
A number of previous field experiments have been used to explore how best to increase charitable giving. Miller and Krosnick (2004) found that appeals to donate to an abortion rights group that included a “policy change threat” message were more likely to result in monetary donations than were appeals that included a “policy change opportunity” message. Frey and Meier (2004) find that individuals are more likely to contribute when informed that a majority of other individuals have already done so; Shang and Croson (2009) find that individuals are influenced by information about how much others have contributed. Karlan and List (2007) find that the rate and amount of giving is positively influenced when large matching gifts are available.
 
5
Group versus non-group sources provides another element of personalization that may complicate the premises of contact theory. Extant research in political science and in social psychology finds that group members tend to be more credible than non-group sources (Calvert 1985; Aroopala 2011). Calvert (1985) goes so far as to suggest that otherwise rational actors may even prefer biased sources provided those sources share their worldview. When a caller self-identifies as a member of the gay community, the emphasis and salience may shift to the impact of marriage equality laws on that particular individual and away from the impact on and needs of the group, the validity of the arguments presented, etc. This could lead to diffused results for several reasons. A participant may feel they are helping one person (i.e., the caller) rather than the organization or a potential donor may feel they are giving to the individual and not feel the social benefits of group membership.
 
6
Similarly, cognitive reactance, a social psychology theory, seeks to explain counter-effects of persuasion by suggesting that if one aspect (i.e., message, theme or tactic) of persuasion is over-emphasized, there is an increased likelihood that respondents will choose another alternative (Sherman et al. 2004).
 
7
The full script of the supporter identification phone bank effort can be found in Appendix C in the electronic supplemental material.
 
8
The full scripts for both the treatment and control conditions can be found in Appendix A and Appendix B in the electronic supplemental material. The treatment script provided other language in addition to personalization, including language about pride in being an Iowan and a mention of the role of the state Supreme Court in the recent marriage equality decision; thus, it is possible that those aspects of the treatment script also affected the behavior of contacted individuals. However, given that similar language regarding the role of the Supreme Court and the focus on Iowa appears elsewhere in the two scripts, we believe the coming-out aspect of the treatment paragraph is the key difference.
 
9
These contact rates are comparable to those found in field experiments in voter mobilization using live phone banks (see Gerber et al. 2008); comparable contact rates are not available for other fundraising field experiments due to the different methodologies employed (e.g., mailers, radio, or approaching random pedestrians). The different rates of contact between the two scripts reflect the pool of callers available to One Iowa. Many were straight allies and thus were not able to be assigned to the treatment script; also, not all LGBT individuals were willing to use the coming-out script. Although the organization did not keep detailed records of the sexual orientation of callers, their impression is that there were more callers assigned to the control group than to the treatment script.
 
10
We also modeled willingness to volunteer using a variable we calculated indicating the distance to each individual’s city of residence from the One Iowa headquarters in Des Moines. Consistent with the other results presented above, distance was a statistically significant negative predictor of willingness to volunteer, regardless of whether or not other control variables (age, gender, etc.) were also included in the model.
 
11
The full scripts for both the treatment and control conditions can be found in Appendix A and Appendix B in the electronic supplemental material.
 
12
We also calculated Models 2 and 3 with variables designed to be more precise than the dichotomous “Des Moines” resident variable, either indicating the population density of each individual’s home address or whether or not their home city is considered urban or rural (according to U.S. Census designations). Neither of these variables were statistically significant predictors and the results otherwise remain relatively unchanged.
 
13
Stein et al. (2000) find that in areas where Whites lived with larger proportions of Hispanics nearby (and thus were available to be in contact with them), contact had a positive effect on attitudes towards Hispanics. They find a curvilinear relationship between attitudes and the interaction of contact and context. Similarly, Overby and Barth (2002) find that citizens living in areas with higher gay populations demonstrate warmer attitudes toward homosexuals, a finding seemingly at odds with theories of contextual threat. In a subsequent study, Barth et al. (2009) find that impersonal and superficial interactions with members of an outgroup that result from their presence in the general community increase negative attitudes, and that voluntary contact (e.g., having close friends who are gays or lesbians) has a much more powerful effect on attitudes than involuntary contact (e.g., with gay or lesbian family or co-workers).
 
14
Prestwich et al. (2008) find that contact with outgroup members produces more favorable explicit attitudes and can produce fairly rapid change but that shifting implicit attitudes is a slower process that requires repeated contact. Similarly, Lemm finds that even among individuals who explicitly “disavow prejudice against gay people, bias may be present at an implicit or unconscious level” (2006: 80). She finds no relationship between contact with gay or lesbian individuals and external motivation to be non-prejudiced; this relationship is moderated by explicit attitudes but not by implicit attitudes toward homosexuals.
 
15
See Moreno and Bodenhausen (2001) regarding individuals with egalitarian beliefs and their likelihood to display discriminatory behavior when provided a socially acceptable way to do so. Hegarty et al. (2004) find that heterosexual participants have demonstrated ambivalent reactions to expressions of discomfort displayed by gay and straight targets in gay or straight social situations. These findings suggest that when an opportunity presents itself (i.e., appropriate amount of interpersonal space, length of conversation, amount of criticism leveled, evaluation of rudeness, judgment of responsibility, and simple liking or disliking), implicit forms of sexual prejudice can manifest. Individuals who claim to be supporters of gay rights and marriage equality may still hold implicit biases, biases that come into play when asked to take action.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. New York: Doubleday. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. New York: Doubleday.
Zurück zum Zitat Aroopala, C. (2011). Are group sources always credible? An experimental study of sources, stakes, and participation. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 23(1), 87–110.CrossRef Aroopala, C. (2011). Are group sources always credible? An experimental study of sources, stakes, and participation. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 23(1), 87–110.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Barth, J., Overby, L. M., & Huffmon, S. H. (2009). Community context, personal contact, and support for an anti-gay rights referendum. Political Research Quarterly, 62(2), 355–365.CrossRef Barth, J., Overby, L. M., & Huffmon, S. H. (2009). Community context, personal contact, and support for an anti-gay rights referendum. Political Research Quarterly, 62(2), 355–365.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Boudreau, C. (2009). Closing the gap: When do cues eliminate differences between sophisticated and unsophisticated citizens? Journal of Politics, 71, 1–13.CrossRef Boudreau, C. (2009). Closing the gap: When do cues eliminate differences between sophisticated and unsophisticated citizens? Journal of Politics, 71, 1–13.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brown, R., Vivian, J., & Hewstone, M. (1999). Changing attitudes through intergroup contact: The effects of group membership salience. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 131–142.CrossRef Brown, R., Vivian, J., & Hewstone, M. (1999). Changing attitudes through intergroup contact: The effects of group membership salience. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 131–142.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Calvert, R. L. (1985). Robustness of the multidimensional voting model: Candidate motivations, uncertainty, and convergence. American Journal of Political Science, 29, 69–95.CrossRef Calvert, R. L. (1985). Robustness of the multidimensional voting model: Candidate motivations, uncertainty, and convergence. American Journal of Political Science, 29, 69–95.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell, D. E., & Monson, J. Q. (2008). The religion card: Gay marriage and the 2004 presidential election. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72, 399–419.CrossRef Campbell, D. E., & Monson, J. Q. (2008). The religion card: Gay marriage and the 2004 presidential election. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72, 399–419.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007a). Framing theory. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103–126.CrossRef Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007a). Framing theory. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103–126.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007b). A theory of framing and opinion formation in competitive elite environments. Journal of Communication, 57, 99–118. Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007b). A theory of framing and opinion formation in competitive elite environments. Journal of Communication, 57, 99–118.
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J. N. (2001). On the limits of framing effects: Who can frame? Journal of Politics, 63, 1041–1066. Druckman, J. N. (2001). On the limits of framing effects: Who can frame? Journal of Politics, 63, 1041–1066.
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J. N. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (Ir)relevance of framing effects. American Political Science Review, 98, 671–686.CrossRef Druckman, J. N. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (Ir)relevance of framing effects. American Political Science Review, 98, 671–686.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J. N., & Lupia, A. (2000). Preference formation. Annual Review of Political Science, 3, 1–24.CrossRef Druckman, J. N., & Lupia, A. (2000). Preference formation. Annual Review of Political Science, 3, 1–24.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J. N., & Nelson, K. R. (2003). Framing and deliberation: How citizens’ conversations limit elite influence. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 729–745.CrossRef Druckman, J. N., & Nelson, K. R. (2003). Framing and deliberation: How citizens’ conversations limit elite influence. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 729–745.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Frey, B., & Meier, S. (2004). Social comparisons and pro-social behavior: Testing “conditional cooperation” in a field experiment. American Economic Review, 94, 1717–1722.CrossRef Frey, B., & Meier, S. (2004). Social comparisons and pro-social behavior: Testing “conditional cooperation” in a field experiment. American Economic Review, 94, 1717–1722.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P., & Larimer, C. W. (2008). Social pressure and voter turnout: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment. American Political Science Review, 102, 33–48.CrossRef Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P., & Larimer, C. W. (2008). Social pressure and voter turnout: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment. American Political Science Review, 102, 33–48.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grack, C., & Richman, C. L. (1996). Reducing general and specific heterosexism through cooperative contact. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 8, 59–68.CrossRef Grack, C., & Richman, C. L. (1996). Reducing general and specific heterosexism through cooperative contact. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 8, 59–68.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Green, D. P. (2009). Regression adjustments to experimental data: Do David Freedman’s concerns apply to political science? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, Sept. 3, Toronto. Available at http://ssrn.com/abstract=1466886. Green, D. P. (2009). Regression adjustments to experimental data: Do David Freedman’s concerns apply to political science? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, Sept. 3, Toronto. Available at http://​ssrn.​com/​abstract=​1466886.
Zurück zum Zitat Han, H. C. (2009). Does the content of political appeals matter in motivating participation? A field experiment on self-disclosure in political appeals. Political Behavior, 31, 103–116.CrossRef Han, H. C. (2009). Does the content of political appeals matter in motivating participation? A field experiment on self-disclosure in political appeals. Political Behavior, 31, 103–116.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hegatry, P., Pratto, F., & Lemieux, A. F. (2004). Heterosexist ambivalence and heterocentric norms: Drinking in intergroup discomfort. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7, 119–130.CrossRef Hegatry, P., Pratto, F., & Lemieux, A. F. (2004). Heterosexist ambivalence and heterocentric norms: Drinking in intergroup discomfort. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7, 119–130.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. P. (1996). Some of my best friends: Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 412–424.CrossRef Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. P. (1996). Some of my best friends: Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 412–424.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Herek, G. M., & Glunt, E. K. (1993). Interpersonal contact and heterosexual’s attitudes toward gay men: Results from a national survey. Journal of Sex Research, 30, 239–244.CrossRef Herek, G. M., & Glunt, E. K. (1993). Interpersonal contact and heterosexual’s attitudes toward gay men: Results from a national survey. Journal of Sex Research, 30, 239–244.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hewstone, M., & Brown, R. J. (1986). Contact is not enough: An intergroup perspective on the ‘contact hypothesis’. In M. Hewstone & R. Brown (Eds.), Contact and conflict in intergroup encounters. New York: Basil Blackwell. Hewstone, M., & Brown, R. J. (1986). Contact is not enough: An intergroup perspective on the ‘contact hypothesis’. In M. Hewstone & R. Brown (Eds.), Contact and conflict in intergroup encounters. New York: Basil Blackwell.
Zurück zum Zitat Hopper, R. (1998). What do we know about telephone conversation? In H. Sawhney & G. A. Barnett (Eds.), Progress in communication sciences: Advances in communications (Vol. 15, pp. 29–44). Stamford, CT: Ablex Publishing Corporation. Hopper, R. (1998). What do we know about telephone conversation? In H. Sawhney & G. A. Barnett (Eds.), Progress in communication sciences: Advances in communications (Vol. 15, pp. 29–44). Stamford, CT: Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Zurück zum Zitat Karlan, D., & List, J. A. (2007). Does price matter in charitable giving? Evidence from a large-scale natural experiment. American Economic Review, 97, 1774–1793.CrossRef Karlan, D., & List, J. A. (2007). Does price matter in charitable giving? Evidence from a large-scale natural experiment. American Economic Review, 97, 1774–1793.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Key, V. O. (1949). Southern politics in state and nation. New York: A.A. Knopf. Key, V. O. (1949). Southern politics in state and nation. New York: A.A. Knopf.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim, W. (2002). Personalization: Definition, status, and challenges ahead. Journal of Object Technology, 1, 29–40.CrossRef Kim, W. (2002). Personalization: Definition, status, and challenges ahead. Journal of Object Technology, 1, 29–40.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lemm, K. M. (2006). Among interpersonal contact, motivation, and implicit and explicit attitudes toward gay men. Journal of Homosexuality, 51, 79–99.CrossRef Lemm, K. M. (2006). Among interpersonal contact, motivation, and implicit and explicit attitudes toward gay men. Journal of Homosexuality, 51, 79–99.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lupia, A. (1992). Busy voters, agenda control, and the power of information. American Political Science Review, 86, 390–403.CrossRef Lupia, A. (1992). Busy voters, agenda control, and the power of information. American Political Science Review, 86, 390–403.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lupia, A. (1994). Shortcuts versus encyclopedias: Information and voting behavior in California insurance reform elections. American Political Science Review, 88, 63–76.CrossRef Lupia, A. (1994). Shortcuts versus encyclopedias: Information and voting behavior in California insurance reform elections. American Political Science Review, 88, 63–76.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lupia, A., & McCubbins, M. D. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know?. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lupia, A., & McCubbins, M. D. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know?. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, N., Brewer, M. B., & Edwards, K. (1985). Cooperative interaction in desegregated settings: A laboratory analogue. Journal of Social Issues, 41, 63–79.CrossRef Miller, N., Brewer, M. B., & Edwards, K. (1985). Cooperative interaction in desegregated settings: A laboratory analogue. Journal of Social Issues, 41, 63–79.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2004). Threat as a motivator of political activism: A field experiment. Political Psychology, 25, 507–523.CrossRef Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2004). Threat as a motivator of political activism: A field experiment. Political Psychology, 25, 507–523.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Moreno, K. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2001). Intergroup affect and social judgment: Feelings as inadmissible information. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 4, 21–29.CrossRef Moreno, K. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2001). Intergroup affect and social judgment: Feelings as inadmissible information. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 4, 21–29.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson, T. E., Sanbonmatsu, K., & McClerking, H. K. (2007). Playing a different race card: Examining the limits of elite influence on perceptions of racism. Journal of Politics, 69, 416–429. Nelson, T. E., Sanbonmatsu, K., & McClerking, H. K. (2007). Playing a different race card: Examining the limits of elite influence on perceptions of racism. Journal of Politics, 69, 416–429.
Zurück zum Zitat Overby, L. M., & Barth, J. (2002). Contact, community context, and public attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Polity, 34, 433–456. Overby, L. M., & Barth, J. (2002). Contact, community context, and public attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Polity, 34, 433–456.
Zurück zum Zitat Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751–783.CrossRef Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751–783.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Prestwich, A., Kenworthy, J. B., Wilson, M., & Kwan-Tat, N. (2008). Differential relations between two types of contact and implicit and explicit racial attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 575–588.CrossRef Prestwich, A., Kenworthy, J. B., Wilson, M., & Kwan-Tat, N. (2008). Differential relations between two types of contact and implicit and explicit racial attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 575–588.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Scarberry, N. C., Ratcliff, C. D., Lord, C. G., Lanicek, D. L., & Desforges, D. M. (1997). Effects of individuating information on the generalization of Allport’s contact hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1291–1299.CrossRef Scarberry, N. C., Ratcliff, C. D., Lord, C. G., Lanicek, D. L., & Desforges, D. M. (1997). Effects of individuating information on the generalization of Allport’s contact hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1291–1299.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shang, J., & Croson, R. (2009). Field experiments in charitable contribution: The impact of social influence on the voluntary provision of public goods. The Economic Journal, 119(54), 1422–1439.CrossRef Shang, J., & Croson, R. (2009). Field experiments in charitable contribution: The impact of social influence on the voluntary provision of public goods. The Economic Journal, 119(54), 1422–1439.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sherman, S. J., Crawford, M. T., & McConnell, A. R. (2004). Looking ahead as a technique to reduce resistance to persuasive attempts. In E. S. Knowles & J. A. Linn (Eds.), Resistance and persuasion (pp. 149–174). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Inc. Sherman, S. J., Crawford, M. T., & McConnell, A. R. (2004). Looking ahead as a technique to reduce resistance to persuasive attempts. In E. S. Knowles & J. A. Linn (Eds.), Resistance and persuasion (pp. 149–174). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Inc.
Zurück zum Zitat Skipworth, S. A., Garner, A., & Dettrey, B. J. (2010). Limitations of the contact hypothesis: Heterogeneity in the contact effect on attitudes toward gay rights. Politics & Policy, 38(5), 887–906.CrossRef Skipworth, S. A., Garner, A., & Dettrey, B. J. (2010). Limitations of the contact hypothesis: Heterogeneity in the contact effect on attitudes toward gay rights. Politics & Policy, 38(5), 887–906.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Smith, D. A., DeSantis, M., & Kassel, J. (2006). Same-sex marriage ballot measures and the 2004 presidential election. State & Local Government Review, 38, 78–91.CrossRef Smith, D. A., DeSantis, M., & Kassel, J. (2006). Same-sex marriage ballot measures and the 2004 presidential election. State & Local Government Review, 38, 78–91.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Soule, S. A. (2004). Going to the Chapel? Same-sex marriage bans in the United States, 1973–2000. Social Problems, 51, 453–477.CrossRef Soule, S. A. (2004). Going to the Chapel? Same-sex marriage bans in the United States, 1973–2000. Social Problems, 51, 453–477.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Spears, R., Postmes, T., Lea, M., & Wolbert, A. (2002). When are net effects gross products? The power of influence and the influence of power in computer-mediated communication. Journal of Social Issues, 58(1), 91–107.CrossRef Spears, R., Postmes, T., Lea, M., & Wolbert, A. (2002). When are net effects gross products? The power of influence and the influence of power in computer-mediated communication. Journal of Social Issues, 58(1), 91–107.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stein, R. M., Post, S. S., & Rinden, A. L. (2000). Reconciling context and contact effects on racial attitudes. Political Research Quarterly, 53(2), 285–303. Stein, R. M., Post, S. S., & Rinden, A. L. (2000). Reconciling context and contact effects on racial attitudes. Political Research Quarterly, 53(2), 285–303.
Metadaten
Titel
Not That There’s Anything Wrong with That: The Effect of Personalized Appeals on Marriage Equality Campaigns
verfasst von
Brian F. Harrison
Melissa R. Michelson
Publikationsdatum
01.06.2012
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 2/2012
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-011-9169-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2012

Political Behavior 2/2012 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner