Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 2/2012

01.06.2012 | Original Paper

Mobilizing Collective Identity: Frames & Rational Individuals

verfasst von: Christy Aroopala

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 2/2012

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Mobilization of collective identities is a common tool in election campaigns and policy debates. Frames that target group identity can mobilize groups; however it is unclear when these group frames are likely to be successful. This project explores whether moderators, or factors that limit framing effects, can help predict whether individuals will respond to group mobilization attempts. Drawing on the rational choice approach, I assess whether the presence of thresholds (i.e. rules that determines how far the group is from attaining its goal) works as a moderator of framing effects. Using a voting game laboratory experiment, I analyze the impact of group frames when distance from a fixed threshold varies and when we account for differences in group identity strength. The findings indicate that the interaction of group identity strength, group frames, and moderators of frames has an important impact on participation, suggesting that environmental factors play a significant role in group mobilization.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
See Chong (2000) and Chai (2001) for rational explanations of the origins and maintenance of group identity, which is very different from the focus here on mobilization influences on moderate identifiers.
 
2
This view is consistent with framing theory in that framing theory explains framing as well as priming using cues (Chong and Druckman 2007a). As Chong and Druckman (2007a) discuss, framing can work via accessibility or applicability, and framing and priming are theoretically indistinguishable.
 
3
The social movement literature does provide important concepts for understanding mobilization, such as preconditions to framing movements in the form of political opportunity structures (Diani 1996; McCarthy and Zald 1996; Polletta and Ho 2006). However, the theoretical causal mechanisms have yet to be fully explored or empirically tested.
 
4
Previous work on group identity and behavior do not address the question of when group identity will have influence. Many scholars have studied whether ingroup bias leads to increased support for same-race candidates (Barreto 2007; Canon 1999; Lublin 1997; Terkildsen 1993). However, in most cases, group-based mobilization is often assumed but not explicitly accounted for. Instead, ingroup bias alone, rather than campaign effects, is predicted to drive turnout and participation. Also, rational choice approaches to group identity focus on other-regarding preferences, such as levels of altruism or group utility (Edlin 2007; Fowler and Kam 2007) or on group coordination, known as the ethical voter approach (Coate and Conlin 2004; Feddersen and Sandroni, A theory of participation in elections, Unpublished Paper, 2002). However, these approaches not only overlook mobilization effects, but also fail to predict which individuals will gain utility from the group. Therefore, they explain only the behavior of strong identifiers—the very same people who are most likely to mobilize regardless of the circumstances. A second rational choice approach to group identity argues that group leaders increase the costs for not participating as well as promising benefits to those who participate (Morton 1987; Uhlaner 1989) or can increase mobilization efforts in close elections or elections with high stakes, which increases turnout (Shachar and Nalebuff 1999). However, these approaches fail to explain how leaders successfully mobilize group members or increase costs for not participating. In other words, we still cannot answer questions related to how or why certain mobilization strategies are successful sometimes and yet fail in other circumstances.
 
5
Strong identifiers are not included in this study since they are likely to participate regardless of mobilization, and so are unlikely targets of mobilization. However, other work suggests that strong identifiers actually respond most to the group frame in a minority context because an underdog effect is triggered, whereas they are confident enough that fellow members will turn out in parity & majority that they free-ride (Aroopala 2010).
 
6
There are also pieces that critique the “p term” in the rational choice equation, and argue that the likelihood of actually being pivotal is so small that it likely has no effect (see Aldrich 1993 for a discussion). However, the pivotality I discuss here has to do with the perception of being pivotal, as induced by parity of group size, rather than the small likelihood of actually being pivotal. I argue that the perception of being pivotal, regardless of the actual likelihood of being pivotal, increases participation, as evidenced in various studies of threshold effects (Suleiman and Rapoport 1992).
 
7
See Rabbie et al. 1989 and Karp et al. 1993 for more discussion on how expectations of reciprocity can play a large role in the decision to take costly action, particularly in a public goods environment, which is akin to voting.
 
8
These arguments are in keeping with Ethnic Conflict literature stating that individuals are more likely to take action based on group identity when they perceive benefits associated with their identity and when they receive targeted mobilization from elites geared towards enhancing group-based behavior for political gain (Green and Seher 2003; Chai 2001; Brass 2003). However, this literature does not specify conditions, such as threshold effects or empirically test for them.
 
9
This, however, is dependent upon the strength of the frame: a weak frame will likely fail without the added threshold effect, whereas a strong frame may work independently of the threshold effect as well.
 
10
As noted above and in the conclusion, the rational choice explanation that a feeling of being pivotal is induced by being near a threshold is not inconsistent with a psychological viewpoint that being near a threshold increases the interest in and perceived importance of the event. Whichever view of the psychological explanation for the observed threshold effects on behavior is taken, my central argument here remains the same: this threshold effect can help us better predict when group frames will work because it either reinforces or undermines the group frame. In other words, accounting for threshold effects and group frames will better predict successful mobilization than will group frames alone, which suggests that group identity’s role in political behavior varies according to environmental factors such as frames and instrumental factors.
 
11
As the focus here is on in-group mobilization, I do not include the option of voting for the out-group.
 
12
This payoff Table is from the instructions for the Neutral condition. The Rivalry condition replaced group names with “Your Group…” and “Rival Group…” Similarly, the entire instructions set for the game were presented in either this Neutral or Rivalry language, dependent upon the frame treatment.
 
13
The conversion rate was .37 cents for each point.
 
14
Note, however, that Levine and Palfrey (2007) have two sessions with fifty rounds each, totaling 100 rounds.
 
15
This identity assignment differs significantly from Levine and Palfrey (2007) who randomly switched subjects between Alpha and Beta identities in each round. As such, their experimental design does not allow a real test of identity-based behavior, which is the primary focus of this project.
 
16
See the sample decision screens in the Appendix.
 
17
See footnote 2 on framing theory as it relates to cues and priming.
 
18
The frame used here is a subtle frame. As Chong and Druckman (2007a) highlight, frames can vary in strength and may have differing effects based on strength. Therefore, it is quite possible that an overt frame would have even stronger effects.
 
19
Note, the size treatments here differ from those in Levine and Palfrey (2007) in two important ways. First, each subject was in only one of two main treatments in which the size of both groups was fixed: landslide majority/minority difference or toss-up (small majority/minority difference). Further, within these treatments, each subject was randomly reassigned between minority and majority in each round, and real numbers were used instead of percentages (i.e. 4 Alpha and 5 Beta). Since my focus in this project is on the individual’s perception of pivotality, which more closely approximates actual voter feelings in real elections, I use percentage of total electorate rather than raw numbers.
 
20
Statistical analysis shows that pooling the minority cases of 15–25% and 20–30% and pooling the majority cases of 75–85% and 70–80% does not affect the results.
 
21
Table 8 in the Appendix gives an example of the composition for one of the smaller groups with six total participants. In a round with six total subjects, there can be 0 Alpha that are also Blue, 1 Alpha that is also Green, 3 Beta that are also Blue, and 2 Beta that are also Green. The decision screen would show that 15–25% of the participants in that round are Alpha and 75–85% of the participants are Beta (1 Alpha and 5 Beta). Similarly, the screen would also show that 45-55% of the participants are Blue and that 45–55% of the participants in that round are Green (3 Blue and 3 Green). Please see Figs. 4 and 5 in the Appendix for sample decision screens.
 
22
The order of the groups listed on the screen (i.e. the percentages) are randomly reversed half of the time. For example, sometimes the Alpha/Beta identity is listed first and sometimes the Blue/Green identity is listed first. Please see the sample Decision Screens in the Appendix (Figs. 4, 5).
 
23
In the 1st, 20th,, and 35th rounds, manipulation checks were given before subjects made their choice for that round asking subjects to indicate which groups they were in, which two groups determining earnings in that round, and what the relative sizes of each of the groups in that round were. The results indicate that subjects were attentive and comprehended the rules & instructions.
 
24
In a second control session, only one group identity, in addition to the respective sizes of each of the two groups, is shown on the screen. In this way, I am able to isolate whether showing the sizes of both group identities on the screen (a total of four groups) has any effect on the likelihood of voting for X.
 
25
Similar findings can be seen when the whole sample is pooled (across both identities and all group sizes).
 
26
A variety of diagnostics were performed to check for the proper modeling of learning effects, such as including time period dummies. None of these alternative models gave support for these hypotheses, indicating that controlling for period sufficiently controls for learning effects. For ease of interpretation, the period variable is transformed to range from 1 to 5.
 
27
There is some debate about whether altruism generally refers to feelings towards an entire population or towards the individual’s own group. In this context of competing groups, it seems the latter explanation is more viable as it goes against a vast literature on ingroup/outgroup behavior to argue that altruists are more likely help the outgroup at the expense of their ingroup. Additionally, altruism is included here as recent work points towards its role in turnout decisions (Fowler and Kam 2007).
 
28
Some may be concerned that the Altruism measures were taken post-treatment, however dropping Altruism from the models does not significantly affect the main findings. Altruism is included in the models, as is gender, due to its established importance by previous literature.
 
29
Though the results are not presented here, a second control condition was conducted in which only a single identity at a time was shown on the decision screen. This control helps us isolate the effect of size and rule out alternative explanations for the size findings such as showing double identities on the screen drive the findings. As expected, in Control 2, parity matters even when only one identity is shown on the screen and this result is significant at the .01 level on a two-tailed test.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Aldrich, J. H. (1993). Rational choice & turnout. American Journal of Political Science, 37(1), 246–278.CrossRef Aldrich, J. H. (1993). Rational choice & turnout. American Journal of Political Science, 37(1), 246–278.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Aroopala, C. (2010). Mobilizing collective identity: A survey-experiment. Paper presented at the Midwest Political Science Association, Chicago. Aroopala, C. (2010). Mobilizing collective identity: A survey-experiment. Paper presented at the Midwest Political Science Association, Chicago.
Zurück zum Zitat Aroopala, C. (2011). Are group sources always credible?: An experimental study of sources, stakes, and participation. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 23(1) (forthcoming). Aroopala, C. (2011). Are group sources always credible?: An experimental study of sources, stakes, and participation. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 23(1) (forthcoming).
Zurück zum Zitat Barreto, M. (2007). Si Se Puede! Latino candidates and the mobilization of Latino voters. American Political Science Review, 101, 425–441.CrossRef Barreto, M. (2007). Si Se Puede! Latino candidates and the mobilization of Latino voters. American Political Science Review, 101, 425–441.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brass, P. (2003). The production of Hindu–Muslim violence in contemporary India. London: University of Washington Press. Brass, P. (2003). The production of Hindu–Muslim violence in contemporary India. London: University of Washington Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Canon, D. (1999). Race, redistricting, and representation: The unintended consequences of Black majority districts. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Canon, D. (1999). Race, redistricting, and representation: The unintended consequences of Black majority districts. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Cardy, E. A. (2005). An experimental field study of the GOTV and persuasion effects of partisan direct mail and phone calls. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 28–40.CrossRef Cardy, E. A. (2005). An experimental field study of the GOTV and persuasion effects of partisan direct mail and phone calls. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 28–40.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cason, T. N., & Mui, V.-L. (2005). Uncertainty and resistance to reform in laboratory participation games. European Journal of Political Economy, 21(3), 708–737.CrossRef Cason, T. N., & Mui, V.-L. (2005). Uncertainty and resistance to reform in laboratory participation games. European Journal of Political Economy, 21(3), 708–737.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chai, S.-K. (2001). Choosing an identity: A general model of preference and belief formation. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press. Chai, S.-K. (2001). Choosing an identity: A general model of preference and belief formation. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Chong, D. (2000). Rational lives: Norms and values in politics and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Chong, D. (2000). Rational lives: Norms and values in politics and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007a). Framing public opinion in competitive democracies. American Political Science Review, 101(4), 637–655.CrossRef Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007a). Framing public opinion in competitive democracies. American Political Science Review, 101(4), 637–655.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007b). Framing theory. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103–126.CrossRef Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007b). Framing theory. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103–126.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007c). A theory of framing and opinion formation in competitive elite environments. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 99–118. Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007c). A theory of framing and opinion formation in competitive elite environments. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 99–118.
Zurück zum Zitat Coate, S., & Conlin, M. (2004). A group rule: Utilitarian approach to voter turnout: Theory and evidence. American Economic Review, 94, 1476–1504.CrossRef Coate, S., & Conlin, M. (2004). A group rule: Utilitarian approach to voter turnout: Theory and evidence. American Economic Review, 94, 1476–1504.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Diani, M. (1996). Linking mobilization frames and political opportunities: Insights from regional populism in Italy. American Sociological Review, 61, 1053–1069.CrossRef Diani, M. (1996). Linking mobilization frames and political opportunities: Insights from regional populism in Italy. American Sociological Review, 61, 1053–1069.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J. N. (2001a). The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Political Behavior, 23(3), 225–256.CrossRef Druckman, J. N. (2001a). The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Political Behavior, 23(3), 225–256.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J. N. (2001b). On the limits of framing effects: Who can frame? Journal of Politics, 63(4), 1041–1066. Druckman, J. N. (2001b). On the limits of framing effects: Who can frame? Journal of Politics, 63(4), 1041–1066.
Zurück zum Zitat Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (1998). Are women less selfish than men?: Evidence from dictator experiments. The Economic Journal, 108, 726–735.CrossRef Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (1998). Are women less selfish than men?: Evidence from dictator experiments. The Economic Journal, 108, 726–735.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Edlin, A., et al. (2007). Voting as a rational choice: Why and how people vote to improve the well-being of others. Rationality & Society, 19, 293.CrossRef Edlin, A., et al. (2007). Voting as a rational choice: Why and how people vote to improve the well-being of others. Rationality & Society, 19, 293.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Erbring, L., Goldenberg, E. N., & Miller, A. H. (1980). Front-page news and real-world cues: Another look at agenda-setting by the media. American Journal of Political Science, 24, 16–49.CrossRef Erbring, L., Goldenberg, E. N., & Miller, A. H. (1980). Front-page news and real-world cues: Another look at agenda-setting by the media. American Journal of Political Science, 24, 16–49.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fowler, J., & Kam, C. (2007). Beyond the self: Social identity, altruism, and political participation. Journal of Politics, 69, 813–827. Fowler, J., & Kam, C. (2007). Beyond the self: Social identity, altruism, and political participation. Journal of Politics, 69, 813–827.
Zurück zum Zitat Gamson, W. A. (1992). Talking politics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Gamson, W. A. (1992). Talking politics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Gerber, A. S., & Green, D. P. (2000). The effects of canvassing, telephone calls, and direct mail on voter turnout: A field experiment. American Political Science Review, 94(September), 653–663. Gerber, A. S., & Green, D. P. (2000). The effects of canvassing, telephone calls, and direct mail on voter turnout: A field experiment. American Political Science Review, 94(September), 653–663.
Zurück zum Zitat Green, D., & Seher, R. (2003). What role does prejudice play in ethnic conflict? Annual Review of Political Science, 6, 509–531.CrossRef Green, D., & Seher, R. (2003). What role does prejudice play in ethnic conflict? Annual Review of Political Science, 6, 509–531.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hajnal, Z. (2007). Changing White attitudes toward Black political leadership. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hajnal, Z. (2007). Changing White attitudes toward Black political leadership. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (1992). Political parties and electoral mobilization: Political structure, social structure, and the party canvas. American Political Science Review, 86, 70–86.CrossRef Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (1992). Political parties and electoral mobilization: Political structure, social structure, and the party canvas. American Political Science Review, 86, 70–86.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huddy, L. (2003). Group identity and political cohesion. In D. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Huddy, L. (2003). Group identity and political cohesion. In D. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible: How television frames political issues. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible: How television frames political issues. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacoby, W. G. (2000). Issue framing & public opinion on government spending. American Journal of Political Science, 44(4), 750–767.CrossRef Jacoby, W. G. (2000). Issue framing & public opinion on government spending. American Journal of Political Science, 44(4), 750–767.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Karp, D., et al. (1993). Raising the minimum in the minimum group paradigm. The Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32(3), 231–240.CrossRef Karp, D., et al. (1993). Raising the minimum in the minimum group paradigm. The Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32(3), 231–240.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kinder, D., & Sanders, L. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kinder, D., & Sanders, L. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Klandermans, B. (2003). Collective political action. In L. H. D. Sears & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Klandermans, B. (2003). Collective political action. In L. H. D. Sears & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kravitz, D. A., & Platonia, J. (1993). Attitudes and beliefs about affirmative action: Effects of target and of respondent sex and ethnicity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 928.CrossRef Kravitz, D. A., & Platonia, J. (1993). Attitudes and beliefs about affirmative action: Effects of target and of respondent sex and ethnicity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 928.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ledyard, J. O. (1995). Public goods: A survey of experimental research. In J. Kagel & A. Roth (Eds.), Handbook of experimental economics (pp. 111–194). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Ledyard, J. O. (1995). Public goods: A survey of experimental research. In J. Kagel & A. Roth (Eds.), Handbook of experimental economics (pp. 111–194). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Leonardelli, G. L., & Brewer, M. (2001). Minority and majority discrimination: When and why. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 468–485.CrossRef Leonardelli, G. L., & Brewer, M. (2001). Minority and majority discrimination: When and why. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 468–485.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Levine, D., & Palfrey, T. R. (2007). The paradox of voter participation? A laboratory study. American Political Science Review, 101, 143.CrossRef Levine, D., & Palfrey, T. R. (2007). The paradox of voter participation? A laboratory study. American Political Science Review, 101, 143.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lublin, D. V. (1997). The paradox of representation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Lublin, D. V. (1997). The paradox of representation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat McCarthy, J. D., & Zald, M. N. (1996). Introduction: Opportunities, mobilizing structures, and framing processes—toward a synthetic, comparative perspective on social movements. In D. McAdam (Ed.), Comparative perspectives on social movements: Political opportunities, mobilizing structures, and cultural framings (pp. 1–22). New York: Cambridge University Press. McCarthy, J. D., & Zald, M. N. (1996). Introduction: Opportunities, mobilizing structures, and framing processes—toward a synthetic, comparative perspective on social movements. In D. McAdam (Ed.), Comparative perspectives on social movements: Political opportunities, mobilizing structures, and cultural framings (pp. 1–22). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat McNulty, J. E. (2005). Phone-based GOTV—what’s on the line? field experiments with varied partisan components, (2002)–(2003). Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 41–65.CrossRef McNulty, J. E. (2005). Phone-based GOTV—what’s on the line? field experiments with varied partisan components, (2002)–(2003). Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 41–65.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Michelson, M. R. (2005). Meeting the challenge of Latino voter mobilization. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 85–101.CrossRef Michelson, M. R. (2005). Meeting the challenge of Latino voter mobilization. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 85–101.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Morton, R. (1987). A group majority voting model of public good provision. Social Choice and Welfare, 4, 117–131.CrossRef Morton, R. (1987). A group majority voting model of public good provision. Social Choice and Welfare, 4, 117–131.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson, T., & Kinder, D. (1996). Issue frames & group-centrism. Journal of Politics, 58(4), 1055–1078.CrossRef Nelson, T., & Kinder, D. (1996). Issue frames & group-centrism. Journal of Politics, 58(4), 1055–1078.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nickerson, D. W. (2005). Partisan mobilization using volunteer phone banks and door hangers. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601, 10–27.CrossRef Nickerson, D. W. (2005). Partisan mobilization using volunteer phone banks and door hangers. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601, 10–27.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Olson, M. (1965). The logic of collective action: Public goods and the theory of groups. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Olson, M. (1965). The logic of collective action: Public goods and the theory of groups. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Palfrey, T. R., & Rosenthal, H. (1983). A strategic calculus of voting. Public Choice, 41, 7–43.CrossRef Palfrey, T. R., & Rosenthal, H. (1983). A strategic calculus of voting. Public Choice, 41, 7–43.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Palfrey, T. R., & Rosenthal, H. (1985). Voter participation and strategic uncertainty. American Political Science Review, 79(1), 62–78.CrossRef Palfrey, T. R., & Rosenthal, H. (1985). Voter participation and strategic uncertainty. American Political Science Review, 79(1), 62–78.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Perreault, S., & Bourhis, R. Y. (1999). Ethnocentrism, social identification, and discrimination. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 92–103.CrossRef Perreault, S., & Bourhis, R. Y. (1999). Ethnocentrism, social identification, and discrimination. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 92–103.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Polletta, F., & Kai Ho, M. (2006). Frames and their consequences. In C. T. R. E. Gooden (Ed.), The oxford handbook of contextual political analysis (pp. 187–209). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Polletta, F., & Kai Ho, M. (2006). Frames and their consequences. In C. T. R. E. Gooden (Ed.), The oxford handbook of contextual political analysis (pp. 187–209). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rabbie, J. M., et al. (1998). Social identity theory: A conceptual and empirical critique from the perspective of a behavioural interaction model. European Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 171–202.CrossRef Rabbie, J. M., et al. (1998). Social identity theory: A conceptual and empirical critique from the perspective of a behavioural interaction model. European Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 171–202.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ramirez, R. (2005). Giving voice to Latino voters: A field experiment on the effectiveness of a national nonpartisan mobilization effort. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 66–84.CrossRef Ramirez, R. (2005). Giving voice to Latino voters: A field experiment on the effectiveness of a national nonpartisan mobilization effort. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 66–84.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenstone, S., & Hansen, J. (1993). Mobilization, participation, and democracy in America. New York: Macmillan Press. Rosenstone, S., & Hansen, J. (1993). Mobilization, participation, and democracy in America. New York: Macmillan Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Schram, A., & Sonnemans, J. (1996). Voter turnout as a participation game: An experimental investigation. International Journal of Game Theory, 25, 385–406.CrossRef Schram, A., & Sonnemans, J. (1996). Voter turnout as a participation game: An experimental investigation. International Journal of Game Theory, 25, 385–406.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shachar, R., & Nalebuff, B. (1999). Follow the leader: Theory and evidence on political participation. American Economic Review, 89, 525–547.CrossRef Shachar, R., & Nalebuff, B. (1999). Follow the leader: Theory and evidence on political participation. American Economic Review, 89, 525–547.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Snow, D. A., & Benford, R. D. (1988). Ideology, frame resonance, and participant mobilization. In B. Klandermans, H. Kriesi, & S. Tarrow (Eds.), From structure to action: Comparing movement participation across cultures, international social movement research (pp. 197–217). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Snow, D. A., & Benford, R. D. (1988). Ideology, frame resonance, and participant mobilization. In B. Klandermans, H. Kriesi, & S. Tarrow (Eds.), From structure to action: Comparing movement participation across cultures, international social movement research (pp. 197–217). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Suleiman, R., & Rapoport, A. (1992). Provision of step-level public goods with continuous contribution. Journal of Behavioral Decision-Making, 5, 133–153.CrossRef Suleiman, R., & Rapoport, A. (1992). Provision of step-level public goods with continuous contribution. Journal of Behavioral Decision-Making, 5, 133–153.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1(2), 149–178.CrossRef Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1(2), 149–178.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Zurück zum Zitat Terkildsen, N. (1993). When white voters evaluate black candidates: The processing implications of candidate skin color, prejudice and self-monitoring. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 1032–1053.CrossRef Terkildsen, N. (1993). When white voters evaluate black candidates: The processing implications of candidate skin color, prejudice and self-monitoring. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 1032–1053.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Transue, J. (2007). Identity salience, identity acceptance, and racial policy attitudes: American national identity as a uniting force. American Journal of Political Science, 51(1), 78–91.CrossRef Transue, J. (2007). Identity salience, identity acceptance, and racial policy attitudes: American national identity as a uniting force. American Journal of Political Science, 51(1), 78–91.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Trivedi, N. (2005). The effect of identify-based GOTV direct mail appeals on the turnout of Indian Americans. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601, 115–122.CrossRef Trivedi, N. (2005). The effect of identify-based GOTV direct mail appeals on the turnout of Indian Americans. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601, 115–122.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Uhlaner, C. (1989). Rational turnout: The neglected role of groups. American Journal of Political Science, 33, 390–422.CrossRef Uhlaner, C. (1989). Rational turnout: The neglected role of groups. American Journal of Political Science, 33, 390–422.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wong, J. S. (2005). Mobilizing Asian American voters: A field experiment. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 102–114.CrossRef Wong, J. S. (2005). Mobilizing Asian American voters: A field experiment. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 601(September), 102–114.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Interpersonal trust and attitudes toward human nature. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of social psychological attitudes. San Diego: Academic Press. Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Interpersonal trust and attitudes toward human nature. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of social psychological attitudes. San Diego: Academic Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Metadaten
Titel
Mobilizing Collective Identity: Frames & Rational Individuals
verfasst von
Christy Aroopala
Publikationsdatum
01.06.2012
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 2/2012
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-010-9155-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2012

Political Behavior 2/2012 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner