Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 4/2018

17.10.2017 | Original Paper

Seeing Spots: Partisanship, Negativity and the Conditional Receipt of Campaign Advertisements

verfasst von: John A. Henderson, Alexander G. Theodoridis

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 4/2018

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Changes in the media landscape increasingly put voters in control of the amount and type of political content they consume. We develop a novel experiment to assess the factors that drive this conditional receipt of information. We focus on how party source and tone interact with partisanship to influence the campaign messages voters seek out or avoid, as discretion over self-exposure varies. We randomly expose subjects to comparable positive or negative television ads aired by Democratic or Republican candidates from the 2012 Presidential election, and measure subjects’ propensities to skip, re-watch and share the spots. Partisans avoid out-party ads, albeit asymmetrically: Republicans are more consistent partisan screeners than Democrats. We find more such selectivity as discretion increases, but little evidence that negativity influences self-exposure. Our findings provide greater insight into the forces behind information selectivity, and have important implications for elections in the post-broadcast era.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
Data and code to reproduce analysis are available on the Political Behavior Dataverse at: https://​dataverse.​harvard.​edu/​dataset.​xhtml?​persistentId=​doi:​10.​7910/​DVN/​WP7ETR.
 
2
Indeed by 2016, more voters now say they primarily use online sources (30%) to learn about elections, than they do either cable (24%) or broadcast (10%) television (Gottfried et al. 2016). This ongoing trend has prompted presidential campaigns to increase online ad spending from $78 million in 2012, to over $1 billion by 2016 (Kurtzleben 2016).
 
3
We also replicated this study through a virtually-identical experiment for the 2013 Gubernatorial election in Virginia (fielded through YouGov). See Online Appendix Section F.
 
4
While attack ads may be more interesting or engaging on average, it is not clear that such characteristics should be treated as inherent to tone rather than as distinct dimensions. If separable, at least some the negativity effect could be attributable to how interesting the information is, rather than the tone itself.
 
5
Voters observe party cues from media they passively or actively consume, and then decide to tune in or out given these features, in order to avoid cognitive dissonance from information that conflicts with core aspects of their political identity (Jerit and Barabas 2012; Theodoridis 2012a).
 
6
We surveyed leading scholars (as determined by publications on the topic) to gauge their baseline expectations about voter ad consumption. (Details of this survey are available in the Online Appendix.) Among the 62 scholars surveyed, 57% expect rates of skipping to be higher for positive ads, while 15% expect rates to be higher for negative ads and 29% thought there will be no difference. 71% of experts believe ads from the opposing party will elicit more skipping, while 16% expect voters will skip more ads aired by their own party, with 13% stating no difference. The scholars in our survey expect negative ads to elicit greater self-exposure than positive ads; they expect more ad-seeking behavior from respondents presented with in-party ads; and while they expect both tone and source to influence behavior, they expect the impact of party source to be more pronounced.
 
7
We also fielded a similar study through YouGov on a sample (\(N=1200\)) of Virginians in the days prior to that Commonwealth’s 2013 Gubernatorial election. Details of that study can be found in the Online Appendix. In the interest of space, we do not highlight those results here, as they largely corroborate the findings in our national sample.
 
8
Following Mutz (2011) and Miratrix et al. (2017), we do not use survey weights provided by YouGov in our analysis. Not all subjects could watch video in the CCES, and weights are generated for the entire sample, not just this subset. Online recruitment by YouGov potentially over-samples the politically engaged, which could somewhat limit the generalizability of our results. For instance, we might uncover lower skipping rates than observed in the broader electorate, or might understate tone effects, if negativity impacts the least politically interested the most. The sample is diverse and broadly representative without weights, and our findings do not differ when using weights.
 
9
Though the selected ads are very similar, they are not identical on all non-experimental dimensions. Yet, we use actual, as opposed to hypothetical ads, to maximize the realism and external validity of our experimental findings, which we think outweighs concerns about the slight heterogeneity across treatments.
 
10
For robustness, we ran our analyses iteratively excluding each ad, and find no evidence of ad-specific effects. We also ran analyses excluding the outside-group ads “Briefcase” and “It’s OK”, and including an indicator for candidate or outside-group sponsored ad, again recovering identical results.
 
11
Dial testing data in Iyengar et al. (2010) suggests that subjects generally are able to discern the source and tone of ads within the first few seconds. In our sample, Barack Obama or Mitt Romney appeared in voice, image or name within the first second or two of every ad. In every negative ad, a clear line of attack is made within the first 5 or 6 s, accompanied by negative music, voiceovers and images. Four of the six positive ads open with the candidates speaking either to the camera or to supporters in rallies, alongside applause and uplifting music. The remaining Obama ad opens with photos of the President as a child with his mother going to school and growing up, and the final Romney ad opens with him talking about his character and leadership with workers on a factory floor. The videos used can be found at https://​sites.​google.​com/​site/​ccesvideos/​.
 
12
We also recorded the time spent on the screen following the video page to make sure respondents did not leave their computers while completing the survey.
 
13
We descriptively assess effect heterogeneity in downstream behaviors, stratifying on skippers and non-skippers, in Tables 6 and 7 in the Online Appendix.
 
14
We also examine skipping using a 25-s cut-off, and recover similar findings indicating our results are not driven by individuals skipping at the final seconds.
 
15
The 5-s ad skipping feature is extensively used by YouTube, allowing viewers to reach media content after only brief exposure to ads. Many online sources similarly allow viewers to skip ad content, and online television providers (e.g., Hulu) give viewers discretion to choose the amount and type of ads they watch.
 
16
Viewing times are shown in density plots in Figs. 1 and 3. A few subjects watched the video for longer than their duration, due to slight variations in connection speed for streaming video. The rarity of these longer views results from features of YouGov’s survey system designed to optimize video streaming. Since treatment assignment is random, there is no reason to expect differences in connection speed across conditions.
 
17
Due to non-linearity in modeling binomial outcomes, we replicate results using logit model, and recover virtually identical results. See Tables 2 and 3 in the Online Appendix.
 
18
We recover virtually identical findings using ks-tests when comparing how long participants watched positive and negative ads.
 
19
Our findings clearly contrast with Berger and Milkman (2011), who examine whether particular types of positivity or negativity influence the information people share online. A possible reason for the divergent results is that our study uses a large-N survey experiment to measure how people respond to policy or character attacks made in partisan presidential advertisements, while Berger and Milkman (2011) analyze responses to an amusing Jimmy Dean sausage commercial (\(N=49\)), an angry story about bad customer service by United Airlines (\(N=45\)), and a sad story about 9/11 victims (\(N=47\)). None of these latter vignettes prime partisanship, and they focus on particular emotional dimensions of tone, rather than on attack or promotion politics.
 
20
We also assess whether tone influences ad-seeking behavior differently for certain respondents, in particular the most (or least) interested in current events (Baum 2002). In our auxiliary, multivariate models, we interact interest with (randomized) ad tone, to account for any conditional effects. We recover null interaction effects for all of ad-seeking outcomes, suggesting tone does not influence exposure depending on political interest. See Tables 2 and 3 in the online Appendix.
 
21
Auxiliary OLS results presented in Table 4 in the Appendix show that an ad’s interestingness does not increase exposure to it overall, or conditional on tone. Between 20 and 40% of respondents rated ads in our sample as interesting. For comparison, the Vanderbilt Ads Project rated a larger sample of 2012 Presidential ads using an identical survey instrument, and found virtually all of these range between 34 and 41% (Geer 2012a). Both findings suggest that our results are unlikely to be idiosyncratic to the particular ads we studied.
 
22
We also assess whether prior (reported) exposure to ads outside the experiment is attenuating ad-seeking behaviors, especially by tone. We ask respondents whether they previously had seen the ad shown to them, and aggregate this to measure overall exposure to each ad. Of course, this measure could be influenced by factors that vary across the ads, rather than outside exposure, though again we sought to make ads as similar (and as similarly interesting) as possible to minimize such concerns. Nevertheless, we find that respondents were equally likely to report being exposed to positive (43%) and negative (42%) ads in our sample. We present additional auxiliary regression results in Table 5 in the Online Appendix, using this measure to assess what impact prior exposure has on ad-seeking in our experimental frame. Previous viewing dampens self-exposure to ads in our study, but this effect is statistically similar for both positive and negative ads. In combination, these two results strongly suggest that outside exposure to the sample ads is unlikely to have any mediating effect on how ad tone impacts self-exposure.
 
23
Pure independents appear to behave somewhat more like Republicans, perhaps hinting at the composition of that group in this particular electoral context. See Section E.1 in the Appendix for more details.
 
24
Figure 12 in the Online Appendix show ad-seeking choices by ad type (tone and source) and respondent PID. Interestingly, a fair number of Republicans (49%) and Democrats (55%) completely watch out-party ads. We do not find that these partisan identifiers are more moderate or cross-pressured on the issues, but do find they are more interested in politics and consume more news media.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Ansolabehere, S., & Iyengar, S. (1995). Going negative: How political advertisements shrink and polarize the electorate. New York: Free Press. Ansolabehere, S., & Iyengar, S. (1995). Going negative: How political advertisements shrink and polarize the electorate. New York: Free Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Arceneaux, K., & Nickerson, D. W. (2010). Comparing negative and positive campaign messages: Evidence from two field experiments. American Politics Research, 38, 1–30.CrossRef Arceneaux, K., & Nickerson, D. W. (2010). Comparing negative and positive campaign messages: Evidence from two field experiments. American Politics Research, 38, 1–30.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Arceneaux, K., & Johnson, M. (2013). Changing minds or changing channels: Partisan news in an age of choice. Chicago: Chicago University Press.CrossRef Arceneaux, K., & Johnson, M. (2013). Changing minds or changing channels: Partisan news in an age of choice. Chicago: Chicago University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Arceneaux, K., Johnson, M., & Murphy, C. (2012). Polarized political communication, oppositional media hostility, and selective exposure. Journal of Politics, 74(1), 174–186.CrossRef Arceneaux, K., Johnson, M., & Murphy, C. (2012). Polarized political communication, oppositional media hostility, and selective exposure. Journal of Politics, 74(1), 174–186.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117–150.CrossRef Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117–150.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baum, M. (2002). Sex, lies and war: How soft news brings foreign policy to the inattentive public. American Political Science Review, 39(1), 176–187. Baum, M. (2002). Sex, lies and war: How soft news brings foreign policy to the inattentive public. American Political Science Review, 39(1), 176–187.
Zurück zum Zitat Berelson, B., Lazarsfeld, P., & McPhee, W. (1954). Voting: A study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Berelson, B., Lazarsfeld, P., & McPhee, W. (1954). Voting: A study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2011). What makes online content viral? Journal of Marketing Research, 49(2), 192–205.CrossRef Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2011). What makes online content viral? Journal of Marketing Research, 49(2), 192–205.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bizer, G. Y., Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Christian Wheeler, S., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). The impact of personality on cognitive, behavioral, and affective political processes: The effects of need to evaluate. Journal of Personality, 72(5), 995–1028.CrossRef Bizer, G. Y., Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Christian Wheeler, S., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). The impact of personality on cognitive, behavioral, and affective political processes: The effects of need to evaluate. Journal of Personality, 72(5), 995–1028.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brader, T. (2006). Campaigning for hearts and minds: How emotional appeals in political ads work. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Brader, T. (2006). Campaigning for hearts and minds: How emotional appeals in political ads work. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1960). The American voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1960). The American voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Clinton, J. D., & Lapinski, J. S. (2004). Targeted advertising and voter turnout: An experimental study of the 2000 presidential election. Journal of Politics, 66(1), 69–96.CrossRef Clinton, J. D., & Lapinski, J. S. (2004). Targeted advertising and voter turnout: An experimental study of the 2000 presidential election. Journal of Politics, 66(1), 69–96.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Djupe, P. A., & Peterson, D. A. M. (2002). The impact of negative campaigning: Evidence from the 1998 Senatorial primaries. Political Research Quarterly, 55(4), 845–860.CrossRef Djupe, P. A., & Peterson, D. A. M. (2002). The impact of negative campaigning: Evidence from the 1998 Senatorial primaries. Political Research Quarterly, 55(4), 845–860.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J. N., Fein, J., & Leeper, T. J. (2012). A source of bias in public opinion stability. American Political Science Review, 106(2), 430–454.CrossRef Druckman, J. N., Fein, J., & Leeper, T. J. (2012). A source of bias in public opinion stability. American Political Science Review, 106(2), 430–454.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Freedman, P., Franz, M., & Goldstein, K. (2004). Campaign advertising and democratic citizenship. American Journal of Political Science, 48(4), 723–741.CrossRef Freedman, P., Franz, M., & Goldstein, K. (2004). Campaign advertising and democratic citizenship. American Journal of Political Science, 48(4), 723–741.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gaines, B. J., & Kuklinski, J. H. (2011). Experimental estimation of heterogeneous treatment effects related to self-selection. American Journal of Political Science, 55(3), 724–736.CrossRef Gaines, B. J., & Kuklinski, J. H. (2011). Experimental estimation of heterogeneous treatment effects related to self-selection. American Journal of Political Science, 55(3), 724–736.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Garramone, G. M., Atkin, C. T., Pinkleton, B. E., & Cole, R. T. (1990). Effects of negative political advertising on the political process. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 34(3), 299–311.CrossRef Garramone, G. M., Atkin, C. T., Pinkleton, B. E., & Cole, R. T. (1990). Effects of negative political advertising on the political process. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 34(3), 299–311.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Garrett, R. K. (2009). Politically motivated reinforcement seeking: Reframing the selective exposure debate. Journal of Communication, 59(3), 676–699.CrossRef Garrett, R. K. (2009). Politically motivated reinforcement seeking: Reframing the selective exposure debate. Journal of Communication, 59(3), 676–699.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Garrett, R. K., Carnahan, D., & Lynch, E. K. (2013). A turn toward avoidance? Selective exposure to online political information, 2004–2008. Political Behavior, 35(1), 113–135.CrossRef Garrett, R. K., Carnahan, D., & Lynch, E. K. (2013). A turn toward avoidance? Selective exposure to online political information, 2004–2008. Political Behavior, 35(1), 113–135.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Geer, J. G. (2006). In defense of negativity: Attack ads in presidential campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Geer, J. G. (2006). In defense of negativity: Attack ads in presidential campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Geer, J. G. (2012b). The news media and the rise of negativity in presidential campaigns. PS: Political Science and Politics, 45(3), 422–427. Geer, J. G. (2012b). The news media and the rise of negativity in presidential campaigns. PS: Political Science and Politics, 45(3), 422–427.
Zurück zum Zitat Goggin, S. N., & Theodoridis, A. G. (2017). Disputed ownership: Parties, issues, and traits in the minds of voters. Political Behavior, 39(3), 675–702.CrossRef Goggin, S. N., & Theodoridis, A. G. (2017). Disputed ownership: Parties, issues, and traits in the minds of voters. Political Behavior, 39(3), 675–702.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Graber, D. A. (1984). Processing the news: How people tame the information tide. New York: Longman. Graber, D. A. (1984). Processing the news: How people tame the information tide. New York: Longman.
Zurück zum Zitat Green, D. P., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds: Political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Green, D. P., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds: Political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Grossmann, M., & Hopkins, D. A. (2015). Ideological Republicans and group interest Democrats: The asymmetry of American party politics. Perspectives on Politics, 13(1), 119–139.CrossRef Grossmann, M., & Hopkins, D. A. (2015). Ideological Republicans and group interest Democrats: The asymmetry of American party politics. Perspectives on Politics, 13(1), 119–139.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grossmann, M., & Hopkins, D. A. (2016). Asymmetric politics: Ideological Republicans and group interest Democrats. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Grossmann, M., & Hopkins, D. A. (2016). Asymmetric politics: Ideological Republicans and group interest Democrats. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hill, R. P. (1989). An exploration of voter responses to political advertisements. Journal of Advertising, 18(4), 14–22.CrossRef Hill, R. P. (1989). An exploration of voter responses to political advertisements. Journal of Advertising, 18(4), 14–22.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hillygus, D. S., & Shields, T. G. (2009). The persuadable voter: Wedge issues in presidential campaigns. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRef Hillygus, D. S., & Shields, T. G. (2009). The persuadable voter: Wedge issues in presidential campaigns. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Kyle Smith, N., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 887.CrossRef Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Kyle Smith, N., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 887.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S., Sood, G., & Lelkes, Y. (2012). Affect, not ideology: A social identity perspective on polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(3), 405–431.CrossRef Iyengar, S., Sood, G., & Lelkes, Y. (2012). Affect, not ideology: A social identity perspective on polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(3), 405–431.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S., & Hahn, K. S. (2009). Red media, blue media: Evidence of ideological selectivity in media use. Journal of Communication, 59(2), 19–39.CrossRef Iyengar, S., & Hahn, K. S. (2009). Red media, blue media: Evidence of ideological selectivity in media use. Journal of Communication, 59(2), 19–39.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S., Hahn, K. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Walker, J. (2008). Selective exposure to campaign communication: The role of anticipated agreement and issue public membership. Journal of Politics, 70(1), 186–200.CrossRef Iyengar, S., Hahn, K. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Walker, J. (2008). Selective exposure to campaign communication: The role of anticipated agreement and issue public membership. Journal of Politics, 70(1), 186–200.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.CrossRef Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jerit, J., & Barabas, J. (2012). Partisan perceptual bias and the information environment. Journal of Politics, 74(3), 672–684.CrossRef Jerit, J., & Barabas, J. (2012). Partisan perceptual bias and the information environment. Journal of Politics, 74(3), 672–684.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kahn, K., & Kenney, P. J. (2004). No holds barred: Negativity in U.S. Senate campaigns. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall. Kahn, K., & Kenney, P. J. (2004). No holds barred: Negativity in U.S. Senate campaigns. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Zurück zum Zitat Kam, C. D., & Trussler, M. J. (2017). At the nexus of experimental and observational research: Theory, specification, and analysis of experiments with heterogeneous treatment effects. Political Behavior. doi:10.1007/s11109-016-9379-z.CrossRef Kam, C. D., & Trussler, M. J. (2017). At the nexus of experimental and observational research: Theory, specification, and analysis of experiments with heterogeneous treatment effects. Political Behavior. doi:10.​1007/​s11109-016-9379-z.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Krupnikov, Y. (2011). When does negativity demobilize? Tracing the conditional effect of negative campaigning on voter turnout. American Journal of Political Science, 55(4), 797–813.CrossRef Krupnikov, Y. (2011). When does negativity demobilize? Tracing the conditional effect of negative campaigning on voter turnout. American Journal of Political Science, 55(4), 797–813.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lau, R. R., Sigelman, L., & Rovner, I. (2007). The effects of negative political campaigns: A meta-analytic reassessment. Journal of Politics, 69(4), 1176–1209.CrossRef Lau, R. R., Sigelman, L., & Rovner, I. (2007). The effects of negative political campaigns: A meta-analytic reassessment. Journal of Politics, 69(4), 1176–1209.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lawrence, E., Sides, J., & Farrell, H. (2010). Self-segregation or deliberation? Blog readership, participation, and polarization in American politics. Perspectives on Politics, 8, 141–157.CrossRef Lawrence, E., Sides, J., & Farrell, H. (2010). Self-segregation or deliberation? Blog readership, participation, and polarization in American politics. Perspectives on Politics, 8, 141–157.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lewis-Beck, M. S., Jacoby, W. G., Norpoth, H., & Weisberg, H. F. (2008). The American voter revisited. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.CrossRef Lewis-Beck, M. S., Jacoby, W. G., Norpoth, H., & Weisberg, H. F. (2008). The American voter revisited. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mattes, K., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2015). The positive case for negative campaigning. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Mattes, K., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2015). The positive case for negative campaigning. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miratrix, L. W., Sekhon, J. S., Theodoridis, A. G., & Campos, L. F. (2017). Worth weighting? How to think about and use weights in survey experiments. arXiv preprint arXiv:1703.06808. Miratrix, L. W., Sekhon, J. S., Theodoridis, A. G., & Campos, L. F. (2017). Worth weighting? How to think about and use weights in survey experiments. arXiv preprint arXiv:​1703.​06808.
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. J., Hibbing, M. V., Canache, D., Seligson, M. A., & Anderson, M. R. (2010). Personality and civic engagement: An integrative framework for the study of trait effects on political behavior. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 85–110.CrossRef Mondak, J. J., Hibbing, M. V., Canache, D., Seligson, M. A., & Anderson, M. R. (2010). Personality and civic engagement: An integrative framework for the study of trait effects on political behavior. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 85–110.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mummolo, J. (2016). News from the other side: How topic relevance limits the prevalence of partisan selective exposure. Journal of Politics, 78(3), 763–773.CrossRef Mummolo, J. (2016). News from the other side: How topic relevance limits the prevalence of partisan selective exposure. Journal of Politics, 78(3), 763–773.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mutz, D. C. (2011). Population-based survey experiments. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Mutz, D. C. (2011). Population-based survey experiments. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicholson, S. P. (2012). Polarizing cues. American Journal of Political Science, 56(1), 52–66.CrossRef Nicholson, S. P. (2012). Polarizing cues. American Journal of Political Science, 56(1), 52–66.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nicholson, S. P., Coe, C. M., Emory, J., & Song, A. V. (2016). The politics of beauty: The effects of partisan bias on physical attractiveness. Political Behavior, 38(4), 883–898.CrossRef Nicholson, S. P., Coe, C. M., Emory, J., & Song, A. V. (2016). The politics of beauty: The effects of partisan bias on physical attractiveness. Political Behavior, 38(4), 883–898.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Peterson, D. A. M., & Djupe, P. A. (2005). When primary campaigns go negative: The determinants of campaign negativity. Political Research Quarterly, 58(1), 45–54.CrossRef Peterson, D. A. M., & Djupe, P. A. (2005). When primary campaigns go negative: The determinants of campaign negativity. Political Research Quarterly, 58(1), 45–54.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391.CrossRef Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Prior, M. (2007). Post broadcast democracy: How media choices increases inequality in political involvement and polarizes elections. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Prior, M. (2007). Post broadcast democracy: How media choices increases inequality in political involvement and polarizes elections. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sears, D., & Freedman, J. (1967). Selective exposure to information: A critical review. Public Opinion Quarterly, 31(2), 194–213.CrossRef Sears, D., & Freedman, J. (1967). Selective exposure to information: A critical review. Public Opinion Quarterly, 31(2), 194–213.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sides, J., Lipsitz, K., & Grossmann, M. (2010). Do voters perceive negative campaigns as informative campaigns? American Politics Research, 33(3), 502–520.CrossRef Sides, J., Lipsitz, K., & Grossmann, M. (2010). Do voters perceive negative campaigns as informative campaigns? American Politics Research, 33(3), 502–520.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sigelman, L., & Kugler, M. (2003). Why is research on the effects of negative campaigning so inconclusive? Understanding citizens’ perceptions of negativity. Journal of Politics, 65(1), 142–160.CrossRef Sigelman, L., & Kugler, M. (2003). Why is research on the effects of negative campaigning so inconclusive? Understanding citizens’ perceptions of negativity. Journal of Politics, 65(1), 142–160.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). May I have your attention, please: Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 171–183.CrossRef Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). May I have your attention, please: Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 171–183.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stroud, N. J. (2011). Niche news: The politics of news choice. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Stroud, N. J. (2011). Niche news: The politics of news choice. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Theodoridis, A. (2012a). Rooting interest measuring and manipulating partisan bias. Midwest Political Science Association, April 2012. Theodoridis, A. (2012a). Rooting interest measuring and manipulating partisan bias. Midwest Political Science Association, April 2012.
Zurück zum Zitat Theodoridis, A. G. (2017). Me, myself, and (I), (D) or (R)? Partisan intensity through the lens of implicit identity. Journal of Politics, 79(4). Theodoridis, A. G. (2017). Me, myself, and (I), (D) or (R)? Partisan intensity through the lens of implicit identity. Journal of Politics, 79(4).
Zurück zum Zitat Theodoridis, A. G. (2012b). Party identity in political cognition. PhD Thesis, University of California, Berkeley. Theodoridis, A. G. (2012b). Party identity in political cognition. PhD Thesis, University of California, Berkeley.
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origin of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origin of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zavala, D., Golec, A., Cislak, A., & Wesolowska, E. (2010). Political conservatism, need for cognitive closure, and intergroup hostility. Political Psychology, 31(4), 521–541.CrossRef Zavala, D., Golec, A., Cislak, A., & Wesolowska, E. (2010). Political conservatism, need for cognitive closure, and intergroup hostility. Political Psychology, 31(4), 521–541.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Seeing Spots: Partisanship, Negativity and the Conditional Receipt of Campaign Advertisements
verfasst von
John A. Henderson
Alexander G. Theodoridis
Publikationsdatum
17.10.2017
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 4/2018
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9432-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2018

Political Behavior 4/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner