Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 1/2018

25.02.2017 | Original Paper

Snap Judgment: Implicit Perceptions of a (Political) Court

verfasst von: Thomas G. Hansford, Chanita Intawan, Stephen P. Nicholson

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 1/2018

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Do people fundamentally perceive the Supreme Court as a political institution? Despite the central importance of this question to theories of public evaluations of the Court and its decisions, it remains largely unanswered. To this end, we develop a new, implicit measure of political perceptions of the Court. This new measure relies on a categorization task wherein respondents quickly associate political or non-political attributes with the Supreme Court relative to institutions that are high or low in politicization. We find that the public implicitly perceives the Court as less political than Congress (high politicization) and more political than traffic court (low politicization) and that this measure is distinct from self-reported (explicit) perceptions of politicization. Finally, we find that implicit perceptions have a distinct effect on predicting diffuse support for the court and specific support for one of two Court decisions.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
Baird and Gangl (2006) and Ramirez (2008) utilize decision-specific analogs to this approach and ask subjects whether particular Court decisions were reached in a nonpolitical manner.
 
2
We do not differentiate between “high” and “low” politics (Gibson and Caldeira 2011).
 
3
“Good” terms include “marvelous,” “superb,” “fantastic,” “glorious,” and “wonderful.” The bad terms are “horrible,” “terrible,” “awful,” “unpleasant,” and “dreadful”.
 
4
IAT studies frequently rely on online samples (e.g., Banks and Hicks 2016; Trawalter et al. 2012). Project Implicit (implicit.harvard.edu/implicit) has collected an enormous amount of data using online participants. The MTurk survey was conducted on December 9 and 10, 2014. There were no particularly salient Court cases decided or argued near these dates, which decreases any concern that these data were influenced by a short-term reaction to a Court decision. Respondents who completed our studies were paid $1.25.
 
5
For example, 27% of the sample are 40 years old or older, 24% identify as Republican (59% identify as Democrats), 50% are women, 22% are non-white (8% African American and 5% Hispanic/Latino), and 53% do not have a four-year college degree.
 
6
These data were collected from June 3 to July 2, 2014. Burwell v. Hobby Lobby Stores, Inc. (2014) was announced on June 30, 2014, which means that a small number of our student respondents could have been exposed to this decision prior to completing the survey. However, the similarity in the results across the MTurk and student samples (which are separated by six months) suggests that this decision is not affecting the results. For both the MTurk and student samples, the IAT component of the survey was conducted prior to the explicit items in order to assure that the IATs were not primed by these items.
 
7
Since only approximately 1/4 of our participants (n = 372) received both the Court-Congress politics IAT and the Court-Congress, good-bad IAT, including Implicit Court Preference in our statistical models leads to a substantial decline in sample size and statistical power. We thus exclude this variable from the models reported here. Auxiliary analyses suggest that Implicit Court Preference does not affect either diffuse or specific support. Adding Implicit Court Preference to the models in which Implicit Perception is included, if anything, increases the absolute value of the coefficient for Implicit Perception but also increases the size of the standard error, due to the dramatic reduction in sample size. See the online appendix for details.
 
8
Specifically, respondents were asked the extent to which they agree (on a five-point scale) with each of the following statements: (1) “When deciding cases, justices base their decisions on the law and the Constitution,” (2) “When deciding cases, the justices base their decisions on their own political views,” and (3) “The justices’ party affiliations have little to do with how they decide cases before the Supreme Court.” Gibson and Caldeira (2011) also ask whether justices are just “politicians in robes” and claim that this question taps perceptions of politicization, as compared to perceptions of legal realism. It is also possible, however, that this question differs primarily in its particularly negative valence and we thus excluded it from the battery.
 
9
Cronbach’s alpha for this scale is a relatively unimpressive 0.652, which is not surprising given that Gibson and Caldeira (2011, footnote 21) report that there may be more than the usual measurement error associated with the items, as they are “demanding for ordinary people.”
 
10
Court Knowledge is the total number of correct answers to the following three questions: (1) “Who is the current Chief Justice of the U.S. Supreme Court?”; (2) “Can the U.S. Supreme Court declare an act of Congress unconstitutional?”; and (3) “How are Supreme Court justices selected?” Education is measured on a six-point scale with higher values representing higher levels of educational attainment. Democrat and Republican are dummy variables that equal one if the respondent identifies with the party or is an Independent that leans toward one of the two parties. So-called “pure” Independents serve as the baseline. Ideology is measured on a seven-point scale with positive (negative) values for conservatives (liberals). Respondents who do not place themselves ideologically are coded as zero (i.e., moderate) but are also indicated by a dummy variable which is included in all the models. Income is measured on an 11-point scale, with those who decline to answer assigned to the lowest category and indicated with a dummy variable which is also included in all the models. White and Female equal one for respondents who identify as such. Age is measured in years.
 
11
As is customary in the literature, we use self-reported, explicit attitudes. There may be ways to capture implicit attitudes for these items, but we are interested in exploring whether implicit perceptions of the political nature of the Court affect diffuse and specific support, two dependent variables of long-standing importance to scholars and practitioners.
 
12
Specifically, we ask respondents whether they agree (on a five-point scale) with (1) removing the Court’s right to hear certain controversial types of case, (2) removing judges who rule at odds with what the public wants, (3) the statement that the Court is too independent and needs to be reined in, and (4) that the power of judicial review should be eliminated.
 
13
For Citizens United, respondents were told that “The U.S. Supreme Court recently decided that corporations and unions can spend as much money as they want to help political candidates win elections.” The summary for Graham is: “The U.S. Supreme Court recently decided that juveniles cannot be sentenced to life in prison without parole for any crime other than murder.”
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Albertson, B. (2011). Religious appeals and implicit attitudes. Political Psychology, 32(1), 109–130.CrossRef Albertson, B. (2011). Religious appeals and implicit attitudes. Political Psychology, 32(1), 109–130.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baird, V. A. (2001). Building institutional legitimacy: The role of procedural justice. Political Research Quarterly, 54(2), 333–354. Baird, V. A. (2001). Building institutional legitimacy: The role of procedural justice. Political Research Quarterly, 54(2), 333–354.
Zurück zum Zitat Baird, V. A., & Gangl, A. (2006). Shattering the myth of legality: The impact of the media’s framing of Supreme Court procedures on perceptions of fairness. Political Psychology, 27(4), 597–614.CrossRef Baird, V. A., & Gangl, A. (2006). Shattering the myth of legality: The impact of the media’s framing of Supreme Court procedures on perceptions of fairness. Political Psychology, 27(4), 597–614.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Banaji, M., & Greenwald, A. G. (2013). Blindspot: Hidden biases of good people. New York: Delacorte Press. Banaji, M., & Greenwald, A. G. (2013). Blindspot: Hidden biases of good people. New York: Delacorte Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Banks, A. J., & Hicks, H. M. (2016). Fear and implicit racism: Whites’ support for voter ID laws. Political Psychology, 37(5), 641–658.CrossRef Banks, A. J., & Hicks, H. M. (2016). Fear and implicit racism: Whites’ support for voter ID laws. Political Psychology, 37(5), 641–658.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels, B. L., & Johnston, C. D. (2012). Political justice? Perceptions of politicization and public preferences toward the Supreme Court appointment process. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(1), 105–116.CrossRef Bartels, B. L., & Johnston, C. D. (2012). Political justice? Perceptions of politicization and public preferences toward the Supreme Court appointment process. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(1), 105–116.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels, B. L., & Johnston, C. D. (2013). On the ideological foundations of Supreme Court legitimacy in the American public. American Journal of Political Science, 57(1), 184–199.CrossRef Bartels, B. L., & Johnston, C. D. (2013). On the ideological foundations of Supreme Court legitimacy in the American public. American Journal of Political Science, 57(1), 184–199.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels, B. L., Johnston, C. D., & Mark, A. (2015). Lawyers’ perceptions of the U.S. Supreme Court: Is the Court a “political” institution? Law and Society Review, 49(3), 761–792.CrossRef Bartels, B. L., Johnston, C. D., & Mark, A. (2015). Lawyers’ perceptions of the U.S. Supreme Court: Is the Court a “political” institution? Law and Society Review, 49(3), 761–792.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368.CrossRef Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Boddery, S. S., & Yates, J. (2014). Do policy messengers matter? Majority opinion writers as policy cues in public agreement with Supreme Court decisions. Political Research Quarterly, 67(4), 851–863.CrossRef Boddery, S. S., & Yates, J. (2014). Do policy messengers matter? Majority opinion writers as policy cues in public agreement with Supreme Court decisions. Political Research Quarterly, 67(4), 851–863.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bybee, K. J. (2010). All judges are political—except when they are not: Acceptable hypocrisies and the rule of law. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Bybee, K. J. (2010). All judges are political—except when they are not: Acceptable hypocrisies and the rule of law. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Caldeira, G. A., & Gibson, J. L. (1992). The etiology of public support for the Supreme Court. American Journal of Political Science, 36(3), 635–664.CrossRef Caldeira, G. A., & Gibson, J. L. (1992). The etiology of public support for the Supreme Court. American Journal of Political Science, 36(3), 635–664.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carmines, E. G., & Stimson, J. A. (1980). The two faces of issue voting. American Political Science Review, 74(1), 78–91.CrossRef Carmines, E. G., & Stimson, J. A. (1980). The two faces of issue voting. American Political Science Review, 74(1), 78–91.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Clark, T. S., & Kastellec, J. P. (2015). Source cues and public support for the Supreme Court. American Politics Research, 43(3), 504–535.CrossRef Clark, T. S., & Kastellec, J. P. (2015). Source cues and public support for the Supreme Court. American Politics Research, 43(3), 504–535.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Evans, J. S. B. T. (2010). Thinking twice: Two minds in one Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Evans, J. S. B. T. (2010). Thinking twice: Two minds in one Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Fitzgerald, J. (2013). What does “political” mean to you? Political Behavior, 35(3), 453–479.CrossRef Fitzgerald, J. (2013). What does “political” mean to you? Political Behavior, 35(3), 453–479.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2009). Citizens, courts, and confirmations: Positivity theory and the judgments of the American people. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRef Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2009). Citizens, courts, and confirmations: Positivity theory and the judgments of the American people. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2011). Has legal realism damaged the legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court? Law & Society Review, 45(1), 195–219.CrossRef Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2011). Has legal realism damaged the legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court? Law & Society Review, 45(1), 195–219.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J. L., Caldeira, G. A., & Spence, L. K. (2003). The Supreme Court and the U.S. Presidential Election of 2000: Wounds, self-inflicted or otherwise? British Journal of Political Science, 33(4), 535–556.CrossRef Gibson, J. L., Caldeira, G. A., & Spence, L. K. (2003). The Supreme Court and the U.S. Presidential Election of 2000: Wounds, self-inflicted or otherwise? British Journal of Political Science, 33(4), 535–556.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27.CrossRef Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.CrossRef Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216.CrossRef Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington, M. J., & Smith, J. L. (2007). Issue preferences and evaluations of the U.S. Supreme Court. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71(1), 40–66.CrossRef Hetherington, M. J., & Smith, J. L. (2007). Issue preferences and evaluations of the U.S. Supreme Court. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71(1), 40–66.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hibbing, J. R., & Theiss-Morse, E. (1995). Congress as public enemy: Public attitudes toward American political institutions. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Hibbing, J. R., & Theiss-Morse, E. (1995). Congress as public enemy: Public attitudes toward American political institutions. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hoekstra, V. J. (1995). The Supreme Court and opinion change: An experimental study of the Court’s ability to change opinion. American Politics Quarterly, 23(1), 109–129.CrossRef Hoekstra, V. J. (1995). The Supreme Court and opinion change: An experimental study of the Court’s ability to change opinion. American Politics Quarterly, 23(1), 109–129.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(10), 1369–1385.CrossRef Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(10), 1369–1385.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Intawan, C. & Nicholson, S.P. (2016). My Trust in Government is Implicit: Automatic Trust in Government and System Support. Paper presented at Exploring New Frontiers, Forging New Synergies: Bolstering the Links Between Bio-Politics and Political Psychology conference, University of California, Merced. Intawan, C. & Nicholson, S.P. (2016). My Trust in Government is Implicit: Automatic Trust in Government and System Support. Paper presented at Exploring New Frontiers, Forging New Synergies: Bolstering the Links Between Bio-Politics and Political Psychology conference, University of California, Merced.
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.CrossRef Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.CrossRef Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux. Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.
Zurück zum Zitat Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2013). The rationalizing voter. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2013). The rationalizing voter. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mo, C. H. (2015). The consequences of explicit and implicit gender attitudes and candidate quality in the calculations of voters. Political Behavior, 37(2), 357–395.CrossRef Mo, C. H. (2015). The consequences of explicit and implicit gender attitudes and candidate quality in the calculations of voters. Political Behavior, 37(2), 357–395.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. J. (1990). Perceived legitimacy of Supreme Court decisions: Three functions of source credibility. Political Behavior, 12(4), 363–384.CrossRef Mondak, J. J. (1990). Perceived legitimacy of Supreme Court decisions: Three functions of source credibility. Political Behavior, 12(4), 363–384.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nicholson, S. P., & Hansford, T. G. (2014). Partisans in robes: Party cues and public acceptance of Supreme Court decisions. American Journal of Political Science, 58(3), 620–636.CrossRef Nicholson, S. P., & Hansford, T. G. (2014). Partisans in robes: Party cues and public acceptance of Supreme Court decisions. American Journal of Political Science, 58(3), 620–636.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nicholson, S. P., & Howard, R. M. (2003). Framing support for the Supreme Court in the aftermath of Bush v. Gore. Journal of Politics, 65(3), 676–695.CrossRef Nicholson, S. P., & Howard, R. M. (2003). Framing support for the Supreme Court in the aftermath of Bush v. Gore. Journal of Politics, 65(3), 676–695.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pérez, E. O. (2016). Unspoken politics: Implicit attitudes and political thinking. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Pérez, E. O. (2016). Unspoken politics: Implicit attitudes and political thinking. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Posner, R. A. (2008). How judges think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Posner, R. A. (2008). How judges think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Ramirez, M. D. (2008). Procedural perceptions and support for the U.S. Supreme Court. Political Psychology, 29(5), 675–698.CrossRef Ramirez, M. D. (2008). Procedural perceptions and support for the U.S. Supreme Court. Political Psychology, 29(5), 675–698.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ranganath, K. A., Smith, C. T., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Distinguishing automatic and controlled components of attitudes from direct and indirect measurement methods. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(2), 386–396.CrossRef Ranganath, K. A., Smith, C. T., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Distinguishing automatic and controlled components of attitudes from direct and indirect measurement methods. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(2), 386–396.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Scheb, J. M., & Lyons, W. (2000). The myth of legality and public evaluation of the Supreme Court. Social Science Quarterly, 81(4), 928–940. Scheb, J. M., & Lyons, W. (2000). The myth of legality and public evaluation of the Supreme Court. Social Science Quarterly, 81(4), 928–940.
Zurück zum Zitat Scheb, J. M., & Lyons, W. (2001). Judicial behavior and public opinion: Popular expectations regarding the factors that influence Supreme Court decisions. Political Behavior, 23(2), 181–194.CrossRef Scheb, J. M., & Lyons, W. (2001). Judicial behavior and public opinion: Popular expectations regarding the factors that influence Supreme Court decisions. Political Behavior, 23(2), 181–194.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sinclair, S., Dunn, E., & Lowery, B. S. (2005). The relationship between parental racial attitudes and children’s implicit prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(3), 283–289.CrossRef Sinclair, S., Dunn, E., & Lowery, B. S. (2005). The relationship between parental racial attitudes and children’s implicit prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(3), 283–289.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stoutenborough, J. W., Haider-Markel, D. P., & Allen, M. D. (2006). Re-assessing the impact of Supreme Court decisions on public opinion: Gay civil rights cases. Political Research Quarterly, 59(3), 419–433.CrossRef Stoutenborough, J. W., Haider-Markel, D. P., & Allen, M. D. (2006). Re-assessing the impact of Supreme Court decisions on public opinion: Gay civil rights cases. Political Research Quarterly, 59(3), 419–433.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Theodoridis, A. G. (forthcoming). Me, myself, and (I), (D) or (R)? Partisan intensity through the lens of implicit identity. Journal of Politics. Theodoridis, A. G. (forthcoming). Me, myself, and (I), (D) or (R)? Partisan intensity through the lens of implicit identity. Journal of Politics.
Zurück zum Zitat Trawalter, S., Hoffman, K. M., & Waytz, A. (2012). Racial bias in perceptions of others’ pain. PLoS ONE, 7(11), e48546.CrossRef Trawalter, S., Hoffman, K. M., & Waytz, A. (2012). Racial bias in perceptions of others’ pain. PLoS ONE, 7(11), e48546.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zink, J. R., Spriggs, J. F., II, & Scott, J. T. (2009). Courting the public: The influence of decision attributes on individuals’ views of Court opinions. Journal of Politics, 71(3), 909–925.CrossRef Zink, J. R., Spriggs, J. F., II, & Scott, J. T. (2009). Courting the public: The influence of decision attributes on individuals’ views of Court opinions. Journal of Politics, 71(3), 909–925.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Snap Judgment: Implicit Perceptions of a (Political) Court
verfasst von
Thomas G. Hansford
Chanita Intawan
Stephen P. Nicholson
Publikationsdatum
25.02.2017
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 1/2018
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9398-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

Political Behavior 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe