Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 1/2014

01.03.2014 | Original Paper

The Displaying of Yard Signs as a Form of Political Participation

verfasst von: Todd Makse, Anand E. Sokhey

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 1/2014

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Despite the ubiquity of yard signs, little is known about how and why individuals display them. Using two original studies of the 2008 presidential race, along with American National Election Study data, we address three points pertaining to this understudied form of political participation. First, what are the correlates of the individuals and households that display signs? Second, what motivates such displays, and to what extent do those motives reflect expressive and communicative desires? Finally, how do individuals obtain signs, and do individuals—rather than parties or candidates—play a role in spreading signs throughout neighborhoods? Our findings suggest that the dissemination of yard signs is not merely a top–down process driven by campaign professionals, but a genuine participatory act that is fueled by individual initiative and social networking.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
Although we focus on motives for participation, our general approach also draws upon the social influence literature, particularly Huckfeldt and Sprague (1992) work on parties and mobilization, and McClurg (2006a) examination of the relationship between neighborhood disagreement, interpersonal networks, and political participation.
 
2
The 2008 Franklin County Presidential Election Panel Study consisted of a three-wave panel survey of registered voters, and focused on tracking political network composition and across the 2008 presidential election cycle. 4,548 registered voters were randomly sampled from the Franklin County voter file and sent a questionnaire by mail. Across two waves (1 initial, and 1 reminder mailing), 817 respondents returned usable surveys, yielding an AAPOR response rate of 22.5 % for the March sample (930 sampled voters were invalid). Follow-up surveys were then sent to these respondents—496 provided usable responses to the post-convention questionnaire for a response rate of 62 %, and 493 the post-election survey (yielding an almost identical response rate of about 62 %). Not all initial respondents responded to all waves (i.e., some skipped wave 2, and re-entered the panel at wave 3; some did the opposite)—427 of the original respondents completed all three questionnaires, for an overall panel retention rate of 53.5 %. In this paper, we utilize only the general election wave. The 2000 American National Election Study—part of the broader ANES time series—consisted of 1,807 pre-election interviews, and 1,555 post-election interviews; we utilize it as it includes standard questions on political characteristics and demographics, but also items on participation (including a question on yard sign displaying), and an interpersonal political network battery. Complete information on the study and design can be found at: http://​www.​electionstudies.​org.
 
3
Such “expressive” motivations are likely preceded by emotions such as enthusiasm, as these have been found to predict political action (e.g., Marcus and MacKuen 1993).
 
4
Our work is also situated in a long-line of research looking at the distributions of behaviors and/or opinions in contexts, whether defined in terms of neighborhoods or other units (e.g., Huckfeldt 1986; Leighley 1990; Putnam 1966). In distinguishing—and observing the relationships—between levels of context, in particular we draw upon the work of Huckfeldt and Sprague (1992, 1995) and McClurg (2006a), though our work also speaks to efforts by Djupe and Gilbert (2006) and Huckfeldt et al. (2004), among others.
 
5
Other than traditional yard signs, we observed home-made signs, stickers and posters attached to doors and windows. We also witnessed a pro-Obama statue, a pro-Obama lawn chair, a demagogical sign posted on a tree (accompanied by a video camera and an threat toward anyone who vandalized the sign), and even a sign mocking the practice of putting up yard signs. We did not include bumper stickers because of the measurement error associated with the presence/absence of such symbols at the time of observation.
 
6
Among the metropolitan areas of the 30 largest U.S. cities, Columbus is at or near the mean in most important demographic categories including Black percentage, median income, poverty rate, home ownership, educational attainment, percent married, and median age. The only category in which the Columbus area differs significantly is its lack of a sizable Hispanic population. According to an index of the previous eight categories, Columbus’ average deviation from the mean ranks second.
 
7
There are at least three other reasons why surveying non-participants would have been problematic in the context of this study. First, asking people why they chose not to display a yard sign would have required different survey questions in some cases, and the total exclusion of questions in other cases, leading to concerns over comparability. Second, it is not clear that asking people about motives for inaction is straightforward, especially in the case of displaying a yard sign. With a socially prescribed act such as voting, people who choose not to vote may have considered reasons for not doing so, but in the case of displaying a yard sign, the answer may be, in essence, that the act never occurred to them. Third, concerns about the accuracy of self-reported motivations may be multiplied when it comes to non-participation: Nesbitt and Wilson (1977, p. 252) note that when it comes to such introspective reports, “non-behavior will generally be less available than behavior.”
 
8
Sampling of precincts was based on the 2006 Ohio Colleges exit poll, which used stratified systematic probability-proportional-to-size (PPS) sampling to generate a representative sample of voters. Polling places were divided into five strata based on percentage of 2004 presidential vote for John Kerry. Systematic PPS sampling was used to identify polling places within strata, and after matching these polling locations with the precincts from which they draw, we sampled precincts within polling locations.
 
9
Upper Arlington is a close-in suburb of Columbus with a population of approximately 34,000, and was chosen as a subject for a case study not due to its representativeness (it is upper income and lacks any racial diversity), but due to its self-apparently high level of democratic participation. Upper Arlington has long had an active local Republican party, but is politically competitive, and Democratic participation has been bolstered in the last four years by the creation of Upper Arlington Progressive Action, a grassroots organization that has been active in the last two presidential campaigns. At more than half of the Upper Arlington homes that posted a sign supporting Barack Obama, at least one of these signs was a sign distributed by UAPA. In addition to its turnout rate of well over 80 %, Upper Arlington was inundated with political yard signs (with over 2,700 out of 11,500 housing units having at least one).
 
10
For the analyses that follow, we exclude the eight respondents who voted for Ralph Nader or Bob Barr in the general election, and the one voter who claimed to have voted for Ron Paul (presumably by write-in since he was not on the Ohio general election ballot).
 
11
Of the 15 voters who gave their preferred candidate a rating of ‘5’ or lower, all were Republican voters, and 14 voted for a candidate other than McCain in the primary. All but one of these individuals gave Sarah Palin scores of ‘8’ or higher, suggesting their support for McCain was primarily based on enthusiasm toward Palin. In fact, a number of these individuals posted yard signs with messages such as “Sarah!”, which quite unusually did not contain the presidential nominee’s name at all.
 
12
Because of the small number of respondents choosing the “not at all important” option, all models were re-estimated with the third and fourth response options combined. Results were nearly identical in all cases, and all conclusions are robust to this change.
 
13
Specifically, the elections chosen were races for President (Obama–McCain 2008, Bush–Kerry 2004 and Bush–Gore 2000), governor (Taft–Hagan 2002), Senator (Brown–DeWine 2006, Voinovich–Fingerhut 2004, and DeWine–Celeste 2000) and Attorney General (Dann–Montgomery 2006). Other races were excluded from the index because they produced very little variance across precincts.
 
14
While there is some debate over the measurement of interpersonal disagreement, and by extension, the meaning of homogeneity in social circles (e.g., Klofstad et al. 2013), measures based on shared partisanship have been widely used (e.g., Mutz 2002, 2006).
 
15
The measure is based on the authors’ judgments regarding the amount of visibility a sign might have on a given property. Streets are coded between ‘1’ (for a ‘dead end’) and ‘6’ (for a ‘major artery’), and the score is multiplied by a weight of 1.5 if the property is located at an intersection with a busier street. Inter-coder agreement was more than 94 %, with discrepancies resolved by the authors through discussion. Full details regarding this measure are available upon request, and in a companion paper (Makse et al. 2010).
 
16
Given the unusually polarizing vice-presidential candidacy of Sarah Palin, we also created a measure of difference in affect toward tickets, which also included feeling thermometer scores toward the vice presidential candidates. Although this distinction proved valuable in predicting some behaviors (e.g. number of additional participatory acts), it had no predictive value regarding motivations.
 
17
This variable was excluded from the Upper Arlington models since only 4 out of 705 respondents were African American.
 
18
One might argue that it is the relative propensity to select these prompts that is important—after all, individuals were unconstrained in agreeing with as many of these prompts as they wished, and it seems that Obama supporters and strong partisans were more likely to agree with the prompts generally. For this reason, we also conducted pairwise analyses of the propensity to agree with the prompts: (1) “show pride” versus “let neighbors know”; (2) “show pride” versus “catch the eye of traffic”; and, (3) “let neighbors know” versus “catch the eye of traffic.” We found that individuals whose neighbors posted signs were significantly more likely to cite both of the communicative motives, relative to the “showing pride” motive. Sign placement, however, did not affect the relative likelihood of choosing the two communicative motives. We also found that individuals with homogeneous networks were relatively more likely to choose ‘show pride’ compared to either of the other prompts, but there was no difference between the propensity to choose between the two communicative prompts. Finally, individuals who live on busy roads and individuals in homogeneous neighborhoods are relatively more likely to choose ‘catch the eye of traffic’ compared to either of the other two prompts, but there was no difference in the propensity to choose between the other two.
 
19
Indeed, among our observations, we saw a dozen individuals with 15 or more signs on their property.
 
20
Throughout this section, unless otherwise indicated, changes in predicted probabilities for continuous variables are based on changes from one standard deviation below the mean to one standard deviation above the mean. The changes in predicted probabilities for the ordered scale questions (folded party identification, folded ideology, political knowledge, education, religious importance, religious service attendance, “taking politics personally,” and “received encouragement”) are based on changes from the “minimum” response to the “maximum” response.
 
21
We also find two sets of strangely contradictory results. Turnout in general elections is negatively associated with distributing signs, while turnout in primary elections is positively associated with it. Meanwhile, political knowledge is positively associated with distributing signs, while education is negatively associated with the act. Although we find no particularly notable levels of multicollinearity in the models, we suspect that these unusual results stem from difficulty in parsing these similar effects apart.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Baybeck, B., & McClurg, S. D. (2005). What do they know and how do they know it? An examination of citizen awareness of context. American Politics Research, 33, 492–520.CrossRef Baybeck, B., & McClurg, S. D. (2005). What do they know and how do they know it? An examination of citizen awareness of context. American Politics Research, 33, 492–520.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Berelson, B. R., Lazarsfeld, P. F., & McPhee, W. N. (1954). Voting: A study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Berelson, B. R., Lazarsfeld, P. F., & McPhee, W. N. (1954). Voting: A study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Burns, N., Schlozman, K. L., & Verba, S. (2001). The private roots of public action: Gender, equality, and political participation. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Burns, N., Schlozman, K. L., & Verba, S. (2001). The private roots of public action: Gender, equality, and political participation. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Burt, R. S. (1987). Social contagion and innovation: Cohesion versus structural equivalence. American Journal of Sociology, 92(6), 1287–1335.CrossRef Burt, R. S. (1987). Social contagion and innovation: Cohesion versus structural equivalence. American Journal of Sociology, 92(6), 1287–1335.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell, D. E. (2006). Why we vote: How schools and communities shape our civic life. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Campbell, D. E. (2006). Why we vote: How schools and communities shape our civic life. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Champion, D. J., & Sear, A. M. (1969). Questionnaire response rate: A methodological analysis. Social Forces, 47(3), 335–339.CrossRef Champion, D. J., & Sear, A. M. (1969). Questionnaire response rate: A methodological analysis. Social Forces, 47(3), 335–339.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Claassen, R. L. (2008). Testing the reciprocal effects of campaign participation. Political Behavior, 30, 277–296.CrossRef Claassen, R. L. (2008). Testing the reciprocal effects of campaign participation. Political Behavior, 30, 277–296.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Djupe, P. A., & Gilbert, C. P. (2006). The resourceful believer: Generating civic skills in church. The Journal of Politics, 68(1), 116–127. Djupe, P. A., & Gilbert, C. P. (2006). The resourceful believer: Generating civic skills in church. The Journal of Politics, 68(1), 116–127.
Zurück zum Zitat Djupe, P. A., & Gilbert, C. P. (2009). The political influence of churches. New York: Cambridge University Press. Djupe, P. A., & Gilbert, C. P. (2009). The political influence of churches. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Eveland, W. P., Jr., & Hively, M. H. (2009). Political discussion frequency, network size, and ‘heterogeneity’ of discussion as predictors of political knowledge and participation. Journal of Communication, 59, 205–224.CrossRef Eveland, W. P., Jr., & Hively, M. H. (2009). Political discussion frequency, network size, and ‘heterogeneity’ of discussion as predictors of political knowledge and participation. Journal of Communication, 59, 205–224.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Faucheux, R. A. (2003). Winning elections: Political campaign management, strategy and tactics. New York: M. Evans and Company. Faucheux, R. A. (2003). Winning elections: Political campaign management, strategy and tactics. New York: M. Evans and Company.
Zurück zum Zitat Geddes, B. (1990). How the cases you choose affect the answers you get: Selection bias in comparative politics. Political Analysis, 2, 131–150.CrossRef Geddes, B. (1990). How the cases you choose affect the answers you get: Selection bias in comparative politics. Political Analysis, 2, 131–150.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gerber, A., Green, D., & Larimer, C. (2008). Social pressure and voter turnout: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment. American Political Science Review, 102, 33–48.CrossRef Gerber, A., Green, D., & Larimer, C. (2008). Social pressure and voter turnout: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment. American Political Science Review, 102, 33–48.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Greene, S. (2002). The social–psychological measurement of partisanship. Political Behavior, 24, 171–197.CrossRef Greene, S. (2002). The social–psychological measurement of partisanship. Political Behavior, 24, 171–197.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hawkings, D., & Nutting, B. (Eds.). (2003). Congressional quarterly’s politics in America: 2004, the 108rd congress. Washington DC: CQ Press. Hawkings, D., & Nutting, B. (Eds.). (2003). Congressional quarterly’s politics in America: 2004, the 108rd congress. Washington DC: CQ Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Huckfeldt, R. (1986). Politics in context: Assimilation and conflict in urban neighborhoods. New York: Agathon Press. Huckfeldt, R. (1986). Politics in context: Assimilation and conflict in urban neighborhoods. New York: Agathon Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (1992). Political parties and electoral mobilization: Political structure, social structure, and the party canvass. American Political Science Review, 86, 70–86.CrossRef Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (1992). Political parties and electoral mobilization: Political structure, social structure, and the party canvass. American Political Science Review, 86, 70–86.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (1993). Citizens, contexts, and politics. In A. Finifter (Ed.), Political science: The state of the discipline II (pp. 281–303). Washington, D.C.: American Political Science Association. Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (1993). Citizens, contexts, and politics. In A. Finifter (Ed.), Political science: The state of the discipline II (pp. 281–303). Washington, D.C.: American Political Science Association.
Zurück zum Zitat Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (1995). Citizens, politics, and social communications: Information and influence in an election campaign. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (1995). Citizens, politics, and social communications: Information and influence in an election campaign. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huckfeldt, R., Beck, P. A., Dalton, R. J., & Levine, J. (1995). Political environments, cohesive social groups, and the communication of public opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 39, 1025–1054.CrossRef Huckfeldt, R., Beck, P. A., Dalton, R. J., & Levine, J. (1995). Political environments, cohesive social groups, and the communication of public opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 39, 1025–1054.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huckfeldt, R., Mendez, J. M., & Osborn, T. (2004a). Disagreement, ambivalence, and engagement: The political consequences of heterogeneous networks. Political Psychology, 25(1), 65–95. Huckfeldt, R., Mendez, J. M., & Osborn, T. (2004a). Disagreement, ambivalence, and engagement: The political consequences of heterogeneous networks. Political Psychology, 25(1), 65–95.
Zurück zum Zitat Huckfeldt, R., Johnson, P. E., & Sprague, J. (2004b). The survival of diverse opinions within communication networks. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Huckfeldt, R., Johnson, P. E., & Sprague, J. (2004b). The survival of diverse opinions within communication networks. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jang, S.-J. (2009). Are diverse networks always bad for participatory democracy? Indifference, alienation, and political disagreements. American Politics Research, 37(5), 879–898.CrossRef Jang, S.-J. (2009). Are diverse networks always bad for participatory democracy? Indifference, alienation, and political disagreements. American Politics Research, 37(5), 879–898.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kenny, C. B. (1998). The behavioral consequences of political discussion: Another look at discussant effects on vote choice. Journal of Politics, 60, 231–244.CrossRef Kenny, C. B. (1998). The behavioral consequences of political discussion: Another look at discussant effects on vote choice. Journal of Politics, 60, 231–244.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Klofstad, C. (2009). Civic talk and civic participation: The moderating effect of individual predispositions. American Politics Research, 37, 856–878.CrossRef Klofstad, C. (2009). Civic talk and civic participation: The moderating effect of individual predispositions. American Politics Research, 37, 856–878.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Klofstad, C. A., Sokhey, A. E., & McClurg, S. D. (2013). Disagreeing about disagreement: How conflict in social networks affects political behavior. American Journal of Political Science, 57(1), 120–134. Klofstad, C. A., Sokhey, A. E., & McClurg, S. D. (2013). Disagreeing about disagreement: How conflict in social networks affects political behavior. American Journal of Political Science, 57(1), 120–134.
Zurück zum Zitat Laband, D., Pandit, R., Sophocleus, J., & Laband, A. (2009). Patriotism, pigskins, and politics: An empirical examination of expressive behavior and voting. Public Choice, 138, 97–108.CrossRef Laband, D., Pandit, R., Sophocleus, J., & Laband, A. (2009). Patriotism, pigskins, and politics: An empirical examination of expressive behavior and voting. Public Choice, 138, 97–108.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lacy, D. (2001). A theory of non-separable preferences in survey responses. American Journal of Political Science, 45, 239–258.CrossRef Lacy, D. (2001). A theory of non-separable preferences in survey responses. American Journal of Political Science, 45, 239–258.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Leighley, J. E. (1990). Social interaction and contextual influences on political participation. American Politics Quarterly, 18(4), 459–475.CrossRef Leighley, J. E. (1990). Social interaction and contextual influences on political participation. American Politics Quarterly, 18(4), 459–475.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Makse, T., & Sokhey, A. (2010). Revisiting the divisive primary hypothesis: 2008 and the Clinton–Obama nomination battle. American Politics Research, 38, 233–265.CrossRef Makse, T., & Sokhey, A. (2010). Revisiting the divisive primary hypothesis: 2008 and the Clinton–Obama nomination battle. American Politics Research, 38, 233–265.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Makse, T., Sokhey, A., & Minkoff, S. (2010). Understanding visible political participation: The diffusion of yard signs in neighborhoods. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association. Makse, T., Sokhey, A., & Minkoff, S. (2010). Understanding visible political participation: The diffusion of yard signs in neighborhoods. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association.
Zurück zum Zitat Marcus, G. E., & MacKuen, M. B. (1993). Anxiety, enthusiasm, and the vote: The emotional underpinnings of learning and involvement during presidential campaigns. American Political Science Review, 87, 688–701.CrossRef Marcus, G. E., & MacKuen, M. B. (1993). Anxiety, enthusiasm, and the vote: The emotional underpinnings of learning and involvement during presidential campaigns. American Political Science Review, 87, 688–701.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McClurg, S. D. (2006a). Political disagreement in context: The conditional effect of neighborhood context, disagreement and political talk on electoral participation. Political Behavior, 28, 349–366.CrossRef McClurg, S. D. (2006a). Political disagreement in context: The conditional effect of neighborhood context, disagreement and political talk on electoral participation. Political Behavior, 28, 349–366.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McClurg, S. D. (2006b). The electoral relevance of political talk: Examining disagreement and expertise effects in social networks on political participation. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 737–754.CrossRef McClurg, S. D. (2006b). The electoral relevance of political talk: Examining disagreement and expertise effects in social networks on political participation. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 737–754.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mutz, D. C. (2002). The consequences of cross-cutting networks for political participation. American Journal of Political Science, 46(4), 838–855.CrossRef Mutz, D. C. (2002). The consequences of cross-cutting networks for political participation. American Journal of Political Science, 46(4), 838–855.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mutz, D. C. (2006). Hearing the other side: Deliberative versus participatory democracy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Mutz, D. C. (2006). Hearing the other side: Deliberative versus participatory democracy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nesbitt, R., & Wilson, T. (1977). Telling more than we know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.CrossRef Nesbitt, R., & Wilson, T. (1977). Telling more than we know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Panagopoulos, C. (2009). Street fight: The impact of a street sign campaign on voter turnout. Electoral Studies, 28, 309–313.CrossRef Panagopoulos, C. (2009). Street fight: The impact of a street sign campaign on voter turnout. Electoral Studies, 28, 309–313.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Putnam, R. D. (1966). Political attitudes and the local community. American Political Science Review, 60, 640–654.CrossRef Putnam, R. D. (1966). Political attitudes and the local community. American Political Science Review, 60, 640–654.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenstone, S. J., & Hansen, J. M. (1993). Mobilization, participation, and democracy in America. New York: MacMillan. Rosenstone, S. J., & Hansen, J. M. (1993). Mobilization, participation, and democracy in America. New York: MacMillan.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanders, K. R., & Kaid, L. L. (1981). Political rallies: Their uses and effects. Communication Studies, 32, 1–11. Sanders, K. R., & Kaid, L. L. (1981). Political rallies: Their uses and effects. Communication Studies, 32, 1–11.
Zurück zum Zitat Schlozman, K. L., Verba, S., & Brady, H. (1995). Participation’s not a paradox: The view from American activists. British Journal of Political Science, 25, 1–36.CrossRef Schlozman, K. L., Verba, S., & Brady, H. (1995). Participation’s not a paradox: The view from American activists. British Journal of Political Science, 25, 1–36.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schuessler, A. A. (2000). A logic of expressive choice. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Schuessler, A. A. (2000). A logic of expressive choice. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaw, C. (2004). The campaign manager. Cambridge, MA: Perseus. Shaw, C. (2004). The campaign manager. Cambridge, MA: Perseus.
Zurück zum Zitat Shea, D., & Burton, M. (2006). Campaign craft: The strategies, tactics, and art of campaign management. Westport, CT: Greenwood. Shea, D., & Burton, M. (2006). Campaign craft: The strategies, tactics, and art of campaign management. Westport, CT: Greenwood.
Zurück zum Zitat Verba, S., Schlozman, K. L., & Brady, H. E. (1995). Voice and equality: Civic volunteerism in American politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Verba, S., Schlozman, K. L., & Brady, H. E. (1995). Voice and equality: Civic volunteerism in American politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Verhulst, J., & Walgrave, S. (2009). The first time is the hardest: A cross-national and cross-issue comparison of first-time protest participants. Political Behavior, 31, 455–484.CrossRef Verhulst, J., & Walgrave, S. (2009). The first time is the hardest: A cross-national and cross-issue comparison of first-time protest participants. Political Behavior, 31, 455–484.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wald, K. D., Owen, D., & Hill, S. (1988). Churches as political communities. American Political Science Review, 82, 531–548.CrossRef Wald, K. D., Owen, D., & Hill, S. (1988). Churches as political communities. American Political Science Review, 82, 531–548.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Weisberg, H. F., & Hasecke, E. B. (1999). The psychological underpinnings of party identification. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association. Weisberg, H. F., & Hasecke, E. B. (1999). The psychological underpinnings of party identification. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association.
Metadaten
Titel
The Displaying of Yard Signs as a Form of Political Participation
verfasst von
Todd Makse
Anand E. Sokhey
Publikationsdatum
01.03.2014
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 1/2014
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-013-9224-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2014

Political Behavior 1/2014 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner