Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Theory and Decision 3/2018

20.06.2017

The Impact of Health-Related Emotions on Belief Formation and Behavior

verfasst von: Elyès Jouini, Clotilde Napp

Erschienen in: Theory and Decision | Ausgabe 3/2018

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

We present a theoretical model of health beliefs and behaviors that explicitly takes into account the emotional impact of possible bad news (i.e., illness), ex-ante in the form of anxiety and ex-post in the form of disappointment. Our model makes it possible to explain (simultaneously) a number of anomalies such as ’low’ testing rates, heterogeneous perceptions of risk levels, underestimation of health risk, ostriches and hypochondriacs, over-use and under-use of health services, patient preference for information when relatively certain of not being ill, yet avoiding information when relatively certain of being ill, etc. Our model matches observed patterns both in health beliefs and health behaviors and irrational health beliefs and behaviors can be characterized as the optimal response under a given structure of emotions and preferences.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
For breast cancer risk, the analyses of Katapodi et al. (2004, 2009a, b) show that “women do not have accurate perceptions of their risk”, some women having a “pessimistic bias” and others having an “optimistic bias”; more precisely, 40% are optimists, underestimating their breast cancer risk, \(10\%\) are pessimists, overestimating their breast cancer risk, \(12\%\) are “ostriches” or extremely optimistic, believing that they “definitely will not get breast cancer”, and \(4\%\) are hypochondriacs or extremely pessimistic believing that they “will get cancer”.
 
2
Woody Allen provides a good illustration of such an hypochondriac: “at the appearance of the mildest symptom, let’s say chapped lips, I instantly leap to the conclusion that the chapped lips indicate a brain tumor. Or maybe lung cancer.[...] The point is, I am always certain I have come down with something life threatening. It matters little that few people are ever found dead of chapped lips”. W. Allen (Hypochondria: an Inside Look. NYTimes, 01.12.2013).
 
3
Lerman et al. (1996b) find that 40% of high-risk patients who are offered a test for genetic susceptibility to breast and ovarian cancer declined the test. In a similar study on a type of colon cancer, 57% of high-risk individuals declined communication of the test results (Lerman et al. 1999). See Köszegi (2003) for a review.
 
4
“Every minor ache or pain sends me to a doctor’s office in need of reassurance that my latest allergy will not require a heart transplant.[...] I get all available vaccines and inoculations, making me immune to everything from Whipple’s disease to the Andromeda strain.[...] It is also true that when I leave the house for a stroll in Central Park or to Starbucks for a latte, I might just pick up a quick cardiogram or CT prophylactically.” W. Allen (Hypochondria: an Inside Look. NY Times, 01.12.2013).
 
5
Delayed diagnosis of melanoma directly impacts patient survival; however, Richard et al. (2000) find that the median melanoma patient delays seeing a doctor for two months after realizing that she has a suspicious lesion and Betti et al. (2003) observe a mean delay of more than 6 months.
 
6
As the director of a genetic counseling program told the New York Times: “There are basically two types of people. There are “want to knowers” and there are “avoiders”. There are some people who, even in the absence of being able to alter outcomes, find information beneficial. The more they know, the more their level of anxiety goes down. But there are others who cope by avoiding, who would rather stay hopeful and optimistic and not have the unanswered questions answered.” New York Times Magazine, Sept 17., 1995, quoted in Grant et al. (1996).
 
7
Note that y also represents the subjectively anticipated health level.
 
8
“Worry [...] is concerned with future events, the future being thought about is a negative one, and this is accompanied by feelings of anxiety”, McLeod et al. (1991), quoted in Caplin and Leahy (2001).
 
9
See Bell (1985), Loomes and Sugden (1986), Gul (1991) for models of disappointment, and Köszegi and Rabin (2006) for reference-dependent models.
 
10
More precisely, we show that an increase in risk is associated with an increase in anxiety at date 0 in the case of immediate resolution of uncertainty, given by \(v\left( 1\right) -v\left( 0\right) \) and an increase in disappointment at date 1 in the case of delayed resolution of uncertainty, given by \(-D\left( 0,y^{*}\right) \) and we show that for an individual for whom the value of information is zero, the first quantity is larger than the second quantity.
 
11
Unlike Köszegi (2003), we assume that treatment is possible only when an individual tests positive.
 
12
Note that these specifications are consistent with Bell (1985) or Loomes and Sugden (1986) models of disappointment, but also with Köszegi and Rabin (2006) model, in which disappointment derives from comparing the realized outcome to all possible outcomes.
 
13
The main result of Köszegi (2003, Theorem 1) shows that avoidance cannot take any form, and more precisely that for a non serious disease, the individual chooses immediate resolution of uncertainty; this result is obtained under the assumption that the set of treatments available is independent of whether the individual learns his true health state or not.
 
14
Even though testing rates for HIV are much higher than testing rates for Huntington’s disease, the prediction on testing rates cannot be empirically verified since the benefits from treatment interfere.
 
15
We have not considered doctor-patient interaction, and the strategic problems that arise (Caplin and Leahy 2004) but have instead focused on individuals’ decision making.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Akerlof, G., & Dickens, W. T. (1982). The economic consequences of cognitive dissonance. American Economic Review, 72, 307–319. Akerlof, G., & Dickens, W. T. (1982). The economic consequences of cognitive dissonance. American Economic Review, 72, 307–319.
Zurück zum Zitat Barigozzi, F., & Levaggi, R. (2010). Emotional decision-makers and anomalous attitudes towards information. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 40, 255–280.CrossRef Barigozzi, F., & Levaggi, R. (2010). Emotional decision-makers and anomalous attitudes towards information. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 40, 255–280.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory. American Psychologist, 55, 1247–63.CrossRef Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory. American Psychologist, 55, 1247–63.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bell, D. E. (1985). Disappointment in decision making. Operations Research, 33, 1–27.CrossRef Bell, D. E. (1985). Disappointment in decision making. Operations Research, 33, 1–27.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ben-Horin, D. (1979). Dying to work: Occupational cynicism plagues chemical workers. In These Times, 3, 24. Ben-Horin, D. (1979). Dying to work: Occupational cynicism plagues chemical workers. In These Times, 3, 24.
Zurück zum Zitat Benabou, R., & Tirole, J. (2004). Willpower and personal rules. Journal of Political Economy, 112, 848–886.CrossRef Benabou, R., & Tirole, J. (2004). Willpower and personal rules. Journal of Political Economy, 112, 848–886.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Betti, R., Vergani, R., Tolomio, E., Santambrogio, R., & Crosti, C. (2003). Factors of delay in the diagnosis of melanoma. European Journal of Dermatology, 13(2), 183–8. Betti, R., Vergani, R., Tolomio, E., Santambrogio, R., & Crosti, C. (2003). Factors of delay in the diagnosis of melanoma. European Journal of Dermatology, 13(2), 183–8.
Zurück zum Zitat Brewer, N. T., Weinstein, N. D., Cuite, C. L., & Herrington, J. E. (2004). Risk perceptions and their relation to risk behavior. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 27, 125–130.CrossRef Brewer, N. T., Weinstein, N. D., Cuite, C. L., & Herrington, J. E. (2004). Risk perceptions and their relation to risk behavior. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 27, 125–130.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brunnermeier, M., & Parker, J. (2005). Optimal expectations. American Economic Review, 95, 1092–1118.CrossRef Brunnermeier, M., & Parker, J. (2005). Optimal expectations. American Economic Review, 95, 1092–1118.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cameron, L. D., Sherman, K. A., Marteau, T. M., & Brown, P. M. (2009). Impact of genetic risk information and type of disease on perceived risk, anticipated affect and expected consequences of genetic test. Health Psychology, 28, 307–316.CrossRef Cameron, L. D., Sherman, K. A., Marteau, T. M., & Brown, P. M. (2009). Impact of genetic risk information and type of disease on perceived risk, anticipated affect and expected consequences of genetic test. Health Psychology, 28, 307–316.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Caplan, L. S. (1995). Patient delay in seeking help for potential breast cancer. Public Health Reviews, 23, 263–274. Caplan, L. S. (1995). Patient delay in seeking help for potential breast cancer. Public Health Reviews, 23, 263–274.
Zurück zum Zitat Caplin, A., & Leahy, J. (2001). Psychological expected utility theory and anticipatory feelings. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116, 55–79.CrossRef Caplin, A., & Leahy, J. (2001). Psychological expected utility theory and anticipatory feelings. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116, 55–79.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Caplin, A., & Leahy, J. (2004). The supply of information by a concerned expert. Economic Journal, 114, 487–505.CrossRef Caplin, A., & Leahy, J. (2004). The supply of information by a concerned expert. Economic Journal, 114, 487–505.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carman, K., & Kooreman, P. (2014). Probability perceptions and preventive health care. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 49, 43–71.CrossRef Carman, K., & Kooreman, P. (2014). Probability perceptions and preventive health care. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 49, 43–71.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carrillo, J. D., & Mariotti, T. (2000). Strategic ignorance as a self-disciplining device. Review of Economic Studies, 67, 529–544.CrossRef Carrillo, J. D., & Mariotti, T. (2000). Strategic ignorance as a self-disciplining device. Review of Economic Studies, 67, 529–544.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dent, O. F., Bartrop, R., Goulston, K. J., & Chapuis, P. H. (1983). Participation in fecal occult blood screening for colorectal cancer. Social Science and Medicine, 17, 17–23.CrossRef Dent, O. F., Bartrop, R., Goulston, K. J., & Chapuis, P. H. (1983). Participation in fecal occult blood screening for colorectal cancer. Social Science and Medicine, 17, 17–23.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Feather, N. T. (1967). Valence of outcomes and expectations of success in relation to task difficulty and perceived locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 372–386.CrossRef Feather, N. T. (1967). Valence of outcomes and expectations of success in relation to task difficulty and perceived locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 372–386.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fels, M. (2015). On the value of information. Why people reject medical tests. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 56, 1–12.CrossRef Fels, M. (2015). On the value of information. Why people reject medical tests. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 56, 1–12.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gollier, C., & Muermann, A. (2010). Optimal choice and beliefs with ex ante savoring and ex post disappointment. Management Science, 56, 1272–1284.CrossRef Gollier, C., & Muermann, A. (2010). Optimal choice and beliefs with ex ante savoring and ex post disappointment. Management Science, 56, 1272–1284.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Golman, R., Hagmann, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2017). Information avoidance. Journal of Economic Literature, 55(1), 96–135.CrossRef Golman, R., Hagmann, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2017). Information avoidance. Journal of Economic Literature, 55(1), 96–135.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grant, S., Kajii, A., Polak, B. (1996). Preference for information. Cowles Foundation Discussion Paper, 1114. Grant, S., Kajii, A., Polak, B. (1996). Preference for information. Cowles Foundation Discussion Paper, 1114.
Zurück zum Zitat Gul, F. (1991). A theory of disappointment aversion. Econometrica, 59, 667–686.CrossRef Gul, F. (1991). A theory of disappointment aversion. Econometrica, 59, 667–686.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Helmes, A. W. (2002). Application of the protection theory to genetic testing for breast cancer risk. Preventive Medicine, 35, 453–462.CrossRef Helmes, A. W. (2002). Application of the protection theory to genetic testing for breast cancer risk. Preventive Medicine, 35, 453–462.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Inman, J. J., Dyer, J. S., & Jia, J. (1997). A generalized utility model of disappointment and regret effects on post choice valuation. Marketing Science, 16, 97–111.CrossRef Inman, J. J., Dyer, J. S., & Jia, J. (1997). A generalized utility model of disappointment and regret effects on post choice valuation. Marketing Science, 16, 97–111.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jouini, E., Karehnke, P., & Napp, C. (2014). On portfolio choice with savoring and disappointment. Management Science, 60, 796–804.CrossRef Jouini, E., Karehnke, P., & Napp, C. (2014). On portfolio choice with savoring and disappointment. Management Science, 60, 796–804.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Karlsson, N., Loewenstein, G., & Patty, J. W. (2004). A Dynamic Model of Optimism. Pittsburgh: Mimeo, Carnegie Mellon University. Karlsson, N., Loewenstein, G., & Patty, J. W. (2004). A Dynamic Model of Optimism. Pittsburgh: Mimeo, Carnegie Mellon University.
Zurück zum Zitat Karlsson, N., Loewenstein, G., & Seppi, D. (2009). The Ostrich effect: Selective attention to information. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 38, 95–115.CrossRef Karlsson, N., Loewenstein, G., & Seppi, D. (2009). The Ostrich effect: Selective attention to information. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 38, 95–115.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kash, K. M., Holland, J. C., Halper, M. S., & Miller, D. G. (1992). Psychological distress and surveillance behaviors of women with a family history of breast cancer. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 84, 24–30.CrossRef Kash, K. M., Holland, J. C., Halper, M. S., & Miller, D. G. (1992). Psychological distress and surveillance behaviors of women with a family history of breast cancer. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 84, 24–30.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Katapodi, M. C., Lee, K. A., Faccione, N. C., & Dodd, M. J. (2004). Predictors of perceived breast cancer risk and the relation between perceived risk and breast cancer screening: A meta analytic review. Preventive Medicine, 38, 388–402.CrossRef Katapodi, M. C., Lee, K. A., Faccione, N. C., & Dodd, M. J. (2004). Predictors of perceived breast cancer risk and the relation between perceived risk and breast cancer screening: A meta analytic review. Preventive Medicine, 38, 388–402.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Katapodi, M. C., Dodd, M. J., Lee, K. A., & Faccione, N. C. (2009a). Underestimation of breast cancer risk: Influence on screening behavior. Oncology Nursing Forum, 36, 306–314.CrossRef Katapodi, M. C., Dodd, M. J., Lee, K. A., & Faccione, N. C. (2009a). Underestimation of breast cancer risk: Influence on screening behavior. Oncology Nursing Forum, 36, 306–314.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Katapodi, M. C., Dodd, M. J., Faccione, N. C., Humphrey, J. C., & Lee, K. A. (2009b). Why some women have an optimistic or a pessimistic bias about their breast cancer risk: Experiences, heuristics, and knowledge of risk factors. Cancer Nursing, 33, 64–73.CrossRef Katapodi, M. C., Dodd, M. J., Faccione, N. C., Humphrey, J. C., & Lee, K. A. (2009b). Why some women have an optimistic or a pessimistic bias about their breast cancer risk: Experiences, heuristics, and knowledge of risk factors. Cancer Nursing, 33, 64–73.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Köszegi, B. (2003). Health anxiety and patient behavior. Journal of Health Economics, 22, 1073–1084.CrossRef Köszegi, B. (2003). Health anxiety and patient behavior. Journal of Health Economics, 22, 1073–1084.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Köszegi, B., & Rabin, M. (2006). A model of reference-dependent preferences. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121, 1133–1165.CrossRef Köszegi, B., & Rabin, M. (2006). A model of reference-dependent preferences. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121, 1133–1165.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Köszegi, B., & Rabin, M. (2009). Reference-Dependent Consumption Plans. American Economic Review, 99, 909–936.CrossRef Köszegi, B., & Rabin, M. (2009). Reference-Dependent Consumption Plans. American Economic Review, 99, 909–936.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunreuther, H., et al. (1978). Disaster Insurance Protection: Public Policy Lessons. New York: Wiley. Kunreuther, H., et al. (1978). Disaster Insurance Protection: Public Policy Lessons. New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Laciana, C. E., & Weber, E. U. (2008). Correcting expected utility for comparisons between alternative outcomes: A unified parameterization of regret and disappointment. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 36, 1–17.CrossRef Laciana, C. E., & Weber, E. U. (2008). Correcting expected utility for comparisons between alternative outcomes: A unified parameterization of regret and disappointment. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 36, 1–17.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lerman, C., Croyle, R. T., Tercyak, K. P., & Hamann, H. (2002). Genetic testing: Psychological aspects and implications. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 784–797.CrossRef Lerman, C., Croyle, R. T., Tercyak, K. P., & Hamann, H. (2002). Genetic testing: Psychological aspects and implications. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 784–797.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lerman, C., Hughes, C., Trock, B. J., et al. (1999). Genetic testing in families with hereditary nonpolyposis colon cancer. Journal of the American Medical Association, 281, 1618–22.CrossRef Lerman, C., Hughes, C., Trock, B. J., et al. (1999). Genetic testing in families with hereditary nonpolyposis colon cancer. Journal of the American Medical Association, 281, 1618–22.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lerman, C. S., Marshall, J., Audrain, J., & Gomez-Caminero, A. (1996a). Genetic testing for colon cancer susceptibility: Anticipated reactions of patients and challenges to providers. International Journal of Cancer, 69, 58–61.CrossRef Lerman, C. S., Marshall, J., Audrain, J., & Gomez-Caminero, A. (1996a). Genetic testing for colon cancer susceptibility: Anticipated reactions of patients and challenges to providers. International Journal of Cancer, 69, 58–61.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lerman, C. S., Narod, S., Schulman, K., et al. (1996b). BRCA1 testing in families with hereditary breast-ovarian Cancer. A prospective study of patient decision making and outcomes. Journal of the American Medical Association, 275, 1885–1892.CrossRef Lerman, C. S., Narod, S., Schulman, K., et al. (1996b). BRCA1 testing in families with hereditary breast-ovarian Cancer. A prospective study of patient decision making and outcomes. Journal of the American Medical Association, 275, 1885–1892.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Loewenstein, G., Linville, P. (1986). Expectations formation and the timing of outcomes: A cognitive strategy for balancing the conflicting incentives for savoring success and avoiding disappointment. Unpublished Paper. Loewenstein, G., Linville, P. (1986). Expectations formation and the timing of outcomes: A cognitive strategy for balancing the conflicting incentives for savoring success and avoiding disappointment. Unpublished Paper.
Zurück zum Zitat Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1986). Disappointment and dynamic consistency in choice under uncertainty. Review of Economic Studies, 53, 271–282.CrossRef Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1986). Disappointment and dynamic consistency in choice under uncertainty. Review of Economic Studies, 53, 271–282.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McCaul, K. D., Brandstetter, A. D., Schroeder, D. M., & Glasgow, R. E. (1996). What is the relationship between breast cancer risk and mammography screening? A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 15, 423–9.CrossRef McCaul, K. D., Brandstetter, A. D., Schroeder, D. M., & Glasgow, R. E. (1996). What is the relationship between breast cancer risk and mammography screening? A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 15, 423–9.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McGraw, A. P., Mellers, B., & Ritov, I. (2004). The affective costs of overconfidence. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17, 281–295.CrossRef McGraw, A. P., Mellers, B., & Ritov, I. (2004). The affective costs of overconfidence. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17, 281–295.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McLeod, A. J., Williams, J., & Bekerian, D. (1991). Worry is reasonable: The role of explanations in pessimism about future personal events. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 478–486.CrossRef McLeod, A. J., Williams, J., & Bekerian, D. (1991). Worry is reasonable: The role of explanations in pessimism about future personal events. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 478–486.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Meechan, G., Collins, J., & Petrie, K. (2002). Delay in seeking medical care for self-detected breast symptoms in New Zealand women. New Zealand Medical Journal, 115, U257. Meechan, G., Collins, J., & Petrie, K. (2002). Delay in seeking medical care for self-detected breast symptoms in New Zealand women. New Zealand Medical Journal, 115, U257.
Zurück zum Zitat Mellers, B., & McGraw, A. P. (2001). Anticipated emotions as guides to choice. Current Directions, 6, 210–214.CrossRef Mellers, B., & McGraw, A. P. (2001). Anticipated emotions as guides to choice. Current Directions, 6, 210–214.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mellers, B., Schwartz, A., Ho, K., & Ritov, I. (1997). Decision affect theory: Emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky operations. Psychological Science, 8, 423–429.CrossRef Mellers, B., Schwartz, A., Ho, K., & Ritov, I. (1997). Decision affect theory: Emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky operations. Psychological Science, 8, 423–429.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mellers, B., Schwartz, A., & Ritov, I. (1999). Emotion based choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 128, 332–345.CrossRef Mellers, B., Schwartz, A., & Ritov, I. (1999). Emotion based choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 128, 332–345.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mellers, B. A. (2000). Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 910–924.CrossRef Mellers, B. A. (2000). Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 910–924.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ortony, A., Clore, G., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New york: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Ortony, A., Clore, G., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New york: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Oster, E., Shoulson, I., & Dorsey, E. R. (2013). Optimal expectations and limited medical testing: Evidence from huntington disease. American Economic Review, 103(2), 804–30.CrossRef Oster, E., Shoulson, I., & Dorsey, E. R. (2013). Optimal expectations and limited medical testing: Evidence from huntington disease. American Economic Review, 103(2), 804–30.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat PHAROS. (2006). Huntington study group PHAROS investigators, 2006. At risk for Huntington disease: The PHAROS (prospective huntington at risk observational study) cohort enrolled. Archives of Neurology, 63, 991–996.CrossRef PHAROS. (2006). Huntington study group PHAROS investigators, 2006. At risk for Huntington disease: The PHAROS (prospective huntington at risk observational study) cohort enrolled. Archives of Neurology, 63, 991–996.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Parente, S. T., Salveker, D., & DaVanzo, J. (2004). The role of consumer knowledge of insurance benefits in the deand for preventive health care among the elderly. Health Economics, 13, 1–14.CrossRef Parente, S. T., Salveker, D., & DaVanzo, J. (2004). The role of consumer knowledge of insurance benefits in the deand for preventive health care among the elderly. Health Economics, 13, 1–14.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Picone, G., Sloan, F., & Taylor, D. (2004). Effects of risk and time preference and expected longevity on demand for medical tests. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 28, 39–53.CrossRef Picone, G., Sloan, F., & Taylor, D. (2004). Effects of risk and time preference and expected longevity on demand for medical tests. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 28, 39–53.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Richard, M. A., Grob, J. J., Avril, M. F., Delaunay, M., Gouvernet, J., Wolkenstein, P., et al. (2000). Delays in diagnosis and melanoma prognosis (II): The role of doctors. International Journal of Cancer, 89, 280–285.CrossRef Richard, M. A., Grob, J. J., Avril, M. F., Delaunay, M., Gouvernet, J., Wolkenstein, P., et al. (2000). Delays in diagnosis and melanoma prognosis (II): The role of doctors. International Journal of Cancer, 89, 280–285.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sackett, A.M., Armor, D.A. (2006). Manipulating the reasons for optimism: reversing bias by shifting consequences. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University. Sackett, A.M., Armor, D.A. (2006). Manipulating the reasons for optimism: reversing bias by shifting consequences. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheppers, J. A., & Mc Nulty, J. (2002). The affective consequences of expected and unexpected outcomes. Psychological Science, 13, 85–88.CrossRef Sheppers, J. A., & Mc Nulty, J. (2002). The affective consequences of expected and unexpected outcomes. Psychological Science, 13, 85–88.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shepperd, J. A., Carroll, P., & Sweeny, K. (2006). Forsaking optimism. Review of General Psychology, 10, 56–73.CrossRef Shepperd, J. A., Carroll, P., & Sweeny, K. (2006). Forsaking optimism. Review of General Psychology, 10, 56–73.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shepperd, J. A., & Sweeny, K. (2007). Do people brace sensibly? Risk judgments and event likelihood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1064–1075.CrossRef Shepperd, J. A., & Sweeny, K. (2007). Do people brace sensibly? Risk judgments and event likelihood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1064–1075.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shepperd, J. A., & Sweeny, K. (2010). The costs of optimism and benefits of pessimism. Emotion, 10, 750–753.CrossRef Shepperd, J. A., & Sweeny, K. (2010). The costs of optimism and benefits of pessimism. Emotion, 10, 750–753.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor, S. E., & Shepperd, J. A. (1998). Bracing for the worst: Severity testing and feedback timing as moderators of the optimistic bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 915–926.CrossRef Taylor, S. E., & Shepperd, J. A. (1998). Bracing for the worst: Severity testing and feedback timing as moderators of the optimistic bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 915–926.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sicherman, N., Loewenstein, G., Seppi, D., & Utkus, S. (2016). Financial attention. Review of Financial Studies, 29, 863–897.CrossRef Sicherman, N., Loewenstein, G., Seppi, D., & Utkus, S. (2016). Financial attention. Review of Financial Studies, 29, 863–897.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Thornton, R. (2008). The demand for, and impact of, learning HIV status. American Economic Review, 98, 1829–1863.CrossRef Thornton, R. (2008). The demand for, and impact of, learning HIV status. American Economic Review, 98, 1829–1863.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat van Dijk, W., & van der Pligt, W. J. (1997). The impact of probability and magnitude of outcomes on disappointment and elation. Organisational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69, 277–284.CrossRef van Dijk, W., & van der Pligt, W. J. (1997). The impact of probability and magnitude of outcomes on disappointment and elation. Organisational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69, 277–284.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat van Dijk, W., Zeelenberg, M., & van der Pligt, W. J. (2003). Blessed are those who expect nothing: Lowergin expectations as a way of avoiding disappointment. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 505–516.CrossRef van Dijk, W., Zeelenberg, M., & van der Pligt, W. J. (2003). Blessed are those who expect nothing: Lowergin expectations as a way of avoiding disappointment. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 505–516.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (1999). Comparing service delivery to what might have been: Behavioral responses to regret and disappointment. Journal of Service Research, 2, 86–97.CrossRef Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (1999). Comparing service delivery to what might have been: Behavioral responses to regret and disappointment. Journal of Service Research, 2, 86–97.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W., Manstead, A., & van der Pligt, W. J. (2000). On bad decisions and disconfirmed expectancies: The psychology of regret and disappointment. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 521–541.CrossRef Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W., Manstead, A., & van der Pligt, W. J. (2000). On bad decisions and disconfirmed expectancies: The psychology of regret and disappointment. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 521–541.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The Impact of Health-Related Emotions on Belief Formation and Behavior
verfasst von
Elyès Jouini
Clotilde Napp
Publikationsdatum
20.06.2017
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Theory and Decision / Ausgabe 3/2018
Print ISSN: 0040-5833
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-7187
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11238-017-9610-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2018

Theory and Decision 3/2018 Zur Ausgabe

OriginalPaper

The curse of hope

Premium Partner