Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 4/2015

13.12.2014 | Original Paper

More Misinformed than Myopic: Economic Retrospections and the Voter’s Time Horizon

verfasst von: Timothy Hellwig, Dani M. Marinova

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 4/2015

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Retrospective voting is often considered representative democracy’s saving grace. But just how long is the retrospective voter’s time horizon? Do voters make decisions by taking into account evidence accruing over the policy maker’s full term in office? Or do they rely on information from the recent past alone? We address these questions through a unique survey design which leverages real-world heterogeneity in economic outcomes prior to the 2012 U.S. presidential election. Contrary to conventional wisdom, our findings do not support claims that voters are myopic. Although they are able to distinguish between short- and long-term benchmarks, voters are no more accurate in assessing the former than they are the latter. The choice of time horizon also has no consistent effect on the decision to hold the incumbent to account. Our results question assumptions of voter myopia, revealing voters to be more misinformed than short-sighted.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
“October jobs report: The economy is doing better than we thought,” Washington Post, 2 November 2012.
 
2
As the Wall Street Journal (“Labor market inches forward,” 2 November 2012) pointed out, the addition of 171,000 jobs was not enough to drive down the unemployment rate to what it was before President Obama took office.
 
3
This includes Fiorina’s (1981) seminal work on retrospective voting in American elections. As the above quote implies, he theorizes that voters incorporate information on a set of performance criteria which are evaluated over the incumbent’s entire term. Fiorina’s empirical tests, however, pair electoral data with economic indicators one year previous and thus assume myopic voters.
 
4
Duch and Stevenson (2008, p. 46) indicate that some of their surveys they use in their book may have posited a retrospective evaluation of two years rather than one year. However, they give no indication of the prevalence or implications of this two-year item in their database of surveys.
 
5
Using survey data from Poland, Owen and Tucker (2010) compare the effects of a conventional 12-month item to an item evaluating economic performance since the beginning of the transition from communism. Mondak and Gearing (1998) also tie retrospections to time since transition in their study of presidential approval in Romania, though they do not compare effects across time horizons. More generally, we are aware of only one other national election study series—Denmark’s—in which the sociotropic economic voting question asks respondents to recall economic conditions for something other than 12 months previous (e.g., Stubager et al. 2013).
 
6
TAPS is an online survey of a national probability sample of 2,000 American adults. The sample is recruited using a random selection of residential addresses, stratified on age and ethnic group. Respondents who do not have access to a computer or online services were provided access by TAPS. The survey is conducted for the Weidenbaum Center at Washington University by Knowledge Networks. The cooperation rate was 78 %.
 
7
Nearly eight in ten respondents to a March 2012 CBS/New York Times poll identified Mitt Romney as the Republican nominee.
 
8
As described below, respondents are asked to evaluate economic performance across four indicators: growth, unemployment, inflation, and the size of the budget deficit. Question wording is provided in the Online Appendix.
 
9
The choice of “since December 2008” as long-term referent was intentional. Framing retrospections by using a more salient referent, such as “the height of the Great Recession,” would have conjured up strong images but would have also likely biased responses in a positive direction. And while we wished to ask respondents to think about the economy’s trajectory since the beginning of the Obama Administration, we shied away from referring to the precise historical event (e.g., “since the 2012 presidential election” or “since Obama took office”) because doing so would have confounded ‘pure’ economic evaluations with political attitudes unrelated to performance outcomes (e.g. partisan rationalizations, ideology, race, leader effects, and so on) (Palmer and Duch 2001).
 
10
Informed by Table 1, correct assessments of economic trends are as follows: Growth ST frame increased (a lot or a little); Growth LT increased (a lot or a little); Unemployment ST decreased (a lot or a little); Unemployment LT increased (a lot or a little); Consumer prices ST stayed the same or increased a little; Consumer prices LT increased (a lot or a little); Deficit ST stayed the same or decreased; Deficit LT increased (a lot or a little).
 
11
We also examined whether perceptual accuracy varies across individual attributes in terms of political sophistication and partisanship. These analyses did not yield any consistent trends across indicators (See Tables A2-A5 in the Online Appendix).
 
12
Overreporting in studies of voter turnout is a case in point. Keeter et al. (2002, p. 2) point out that “research that both validates voting claims and asks the same question at different points in time has found over reporting increases the farther away the question is asked from the target election.”.
 
13
We also examined the effect of respondents’ objective political and economic knowledge levels on the probability they accurately estimate the change in economic conditions. Not surprisingly, those with higher levels of knowledge are more likely to make the correct assessments of change in economic conditions. However, there is no systematic conditioning effect of temporal frame on the relationship between objective knowledge and accuracy of economic assessments.
 
14
The economic perceptions items are coded such that “increased a lot” = 2, “increased a little” = 1, “stayed the same” = 0, “decreased a little” = −1, and “decreased a lot” = −2.
 
15
According to an April 2012 CBS News/New York Times National Poll, 54 % of Americans viewed the economy, jobs, or the deficit as being the most important problem facing the country. No respondents listed inflation or prices as the most important problem.
 
16
This conclusion parts ways with Healy and Lenz (2014). The difference may be attributable to research design. Healy and Lenz supply voters with hypothetical annual figures of growth; thus their experimental setup takes for granted that voters dispose of such economic information when evaluating performance. Our design makes no such assumption but rather compares voter knowledge of macroeconomic performance across the short and long spans.
 
17
Recall that our survey was in the field in March 2012, less than five months before the November 6 vote. Thus, our dependent variable is vote intention. Duch and Stevenson (2008) show that reliance on vote intention or vote choice yields similar survey responses.
 
18
In our sample, 53.8 percent intended to vote for Obama and 43.4 for Romney. We omit from analysis the 2.8 % who did not signal their vote intentions.
 
19
To check the robustness of the interaction effect, we estimated its coefficient and standard error with the Inteff package in Stata (Ai and Norton 2003). The results are consistent with those reported here.
 
20
We assign values that best approximate the direction and magnitude of macroeconomic conditions: Growth SR +1, Growth LR +2, Unemployment SR +1, Unemployment LR −1, Consumer Prices SR +1, Consumer Prices LR +2, Deficit SR −1, Deficit LR +2.
 
21
We are mainly interested in the magnitude rather than the direction (+/−) of this effect.
 
22
Exceptions are found with respect to sophistication and growth: For high sophisticates 12-month growth evaluations had a stronger effect on their vote intent, a conditional result displayed in Fig. A1 in the Online Appendix. We also find a myopic effect on vote intent for evaluations of unemployment, in this case shaped by partisanship. Those voters who identify with the Democratic Party gave greater weight to 12-month assessments of unemployment (appendix Fig. A2). Despite this partial evidence for voter myopia, we observed mixed evidence in the case of the budget deficit. In evaluating the rate at which the national budget deficit had changed, Democrats did not consistently weigh their assessments in the expected direction; instead they gave differing weights to each reference frame as a function of the direction of change of their evaluations of the deficit (Appendix Fig. A3). All told, no consistent pattern is revealed with respect to the conditioning factors of sophistication or partisanship.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Achen, C. H., & Bartels, L. M. (2004). Musical chairs: Pocketbook voting and the limits of democratic accountability. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association, Chicago, IL. Achen, C. H., & Bartels, L. M. (2004). Musical chairs: Pocketbook voting and the limits of democratic accountability. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association, Chicago, IL.
Zurück zum Zitat Ai, C., & Norton, E. C. (2003). Interaction terms in logit and probit models. Economics Letters, 80(1), 123–129.CrossRef Ai, C., & Norton, E. C. (2003). Interaction terms in logit and probit models. Economics Letters, 80(1), 123–129.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Andersen, R., Tilley, J., & Heath, A. F. (2005). Political knowledge and enlightened preferences: Party choice through the electoral cycle. British Journal of Political Science, 35(2), 285–302.CrossRef Andersen, R., Tilley, J., & Heath, A. F. (2005). Political knowledge and enlightened preferences: Party choice through the electoral cycle. British Journal of Political Science, 35(2), 285–302.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Arnold, J. R., & Samuels, D. (2011). Evidence from public opinion. In S. Levitsky & K. Roberts (Eds.), Latin america’s left turn: A conceptual and theoretical overview (pp. 31–51). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Arnold, J. R., & Samuels, D. (2011). Evidence from public opinion. In S. Levitsky & K. Roberts (Eds.), Latin america’s left turn: A conceptual and theoretical overview (pp. 31–51). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels, L. M. (2008). Unequal democracy: The political economy of the new gilded age. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Bartels, L. M. (2008). Unequal democracy: The political economy of the new gilded age. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels, L. M. (2014). Ideology and retrospection in electoral responses to the Great Recession. In N. Bermeo & L. M. Bartels (Eds.), Mass politics in tough times: Opinion, votes, and protest in the great recession (pp. 185–223). New York: Oxford University Press. Bartels, L. M. (2014). Ideology and retrospection in electoral responses to the Great Recession. In N. Bermeo & L. M. Bartels (Eds.), Mass politics in tough times: Opinion, votes, and protest in the great recession (pp. 185–223). New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Blendon, R. J., Benson, J. M., Brodie, M., Morin, R., Altman, D. E., Gitterman, D., et al. (1997). Bridging the gap between the public’s and economists’ views of the economy. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 11(3), 105–118.CrossRef Blendon, R. J., Benson, J. M., Brodie, M., Morin, R., Altman, D. E., Gitterman, D., et al. (1997). Bridging the gap between the public’s and economists’ views of the economy. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 11(3), 105–118.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bryce, J. (1921). Modern Democracies. New York: Macmillan. Bryce, J. (1921). Modern Democracies. New York: Macmillan.
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The american voter. New York: Wiley. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The american voter. New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Clarke, H. D., Stewart, M. C., & Whiteley, P. F. (1998). New models for new labour: The political economy of labour party support, January 1992–April 1997. American Political Science Review, 92, 559–575.CrossRef Clarke, H. D., Stewart, M. C., & Whiteley, P. F. (1998). New models for new labour: The political economy of labour party support, January 1992–April 1997. American Political Science Review, 92, 559–575.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Conover, P. J., Feldman, S., & Knight, K. (1986). Judging inflation and unemployment: The origins of retrospective evaluations. Journal of Politics, 48(3), 565–588.CrossRef Conover, P. J., Feldman, S., & Knight, K. (1986). Judging inflation and unemployment: The origins of retrospective evaluations. Journal of Politics, 48(3), 565–588.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Duch, R. M., & Stevenson, R. T. (2008). The economic vote: How political and economic institutions condition election results. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Duch, R. M., & Stevenson, R. T. (2008). The economic vote: How political and economic institutions condition election results. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Duch, R. M., & Stevenson, R. T. (2011). Context and economic expectations: When do voters get it right? British Journal of Political Science, 41(01), 1–31.CrossRef Duch, R. M., & Stevenson, R. T. (2011). Context and economic expectations: When do voters get it right? British Journal of Political Science, 41(01), 1–31.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fair, R. C. (1996). The effect of economic events on votes for president: 1992 update. Political Behavior, 18(2), 119–139.CrossRef Fair, R. C. (1996). The effect of economic events on votes for president: 1992 update. Political Behavior, 18(2), 119–139.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fauvelle-Aymar, C., & Stegmaier, M. (2013). The stock market and U.S. presidential approval. Electoral Studies, 32(3), 411–417.CrossRef Fauvelle-Aymar, C., & Stegmaier, M. (2013). The stock market and U.S. presidential approval. Electoral Studies, 32(3), 411–417.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fiorina, M. (1981). Retrospective voting in American national elections. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Fiorina, M. (1981). Retrospective voting in American national elections. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Gaines, B. J., Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2007). The logic of the survey experiment reexamined. Political Analysis, 15(1), 1–20.CrossRef Gaines, B. J., Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2007). The logic of the survey experiment reexamined. Political Analysis, 15(1), 1–20.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goidel, R. K., & Langley, R. E. (1995). Media coverage of the economy and aggregate economic evaluations: Uncovering evidence of indirect media effects. Political Research Quarterly, 48(2), 313–328.CrossRef Goidel, R. K., & Langley, R. E. (1995). Media coverage of the economy and aggregate economic evaluations: Uncovering evidence of indirect media effects. Political Research Quarterly, 48(2), 313–328.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Harrington, D. E. (1989). Economic news on television: The determinants of coverage. Public Opinion Quarterly, 53(1), 17–40.CrossRef Harrington, D. E. (1989). Economic news on television: The determinants of coverage. Public Opinion Quarterly, 53(1), 17–40.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Healy, A., & Lenz, G. S. (2014). Substituting the end for the whole: Why voters respond primarily to the election-year economy. American Journal of Political Science, 58(1), 31–47.CrossRef Healy, A., & Lenz, G. S. (2014). Substituting the end for the whole: Why voters respond primarily to the election-year economy. American Journal of Political Science, 58(1), 31–47.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huber, G. A., Hill, S. J., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Sources of bias in retrospective decision-making: Experimental evidence on voters’ limitations in controlling incumbents. American Political Science Review, 106(4), 720–741.CrossRef Huber, G. A., Hill, S. J., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Sources of bias in retrospective decision-making: Experimental evidence on voters’ limitations in controlling incumbents. American Political Science Review, 106(4), 720–741.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kayser, M. A., & Peress, M. (2012). Benchmarking across borders: Electoral accountability and the necessity of comparison. American Political Science Review, 106(3), 661–684.CrossRef Kayser, M. A., & Peress, M. (2012). Benchmarking across borders: Electoral accountability and the necessity of comparison. American Political Science Review, 106(3), 661–684.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Key, V. O. (1966). The responsible electorate: Rationality in presidential voting, 1936–1960. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.CrossRef Key, V. O. (1966). The responsible electorate: Rationality in presidential voting, 1936–1960. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat King, G., Tomz, M., & Wittenberg, J. (2000). Making the most of statistical analyses: Improving interpretation and presentation. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 347–361.CrossRef King, G., Tomz, M., & Wittenberg, J. (2000). Making the most of statistical analyses: Improving interpretation and presentation. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 347–361.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kramer, G. H. (1971). Short-term fluctuations in US voting behavior, 1896–1964. American Political Science Review, 65(1), 131–143.CrossRef Kramer, G. H. (1971). Short-term fluctuations in US voting behavior, 1896–1964. American Political Science Review, 65(1), 131–143.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lewis-Beck, M. S. (1991). French national elections: Political economic forecasts. European Journal of Political Economy, 7(4), 487–496.CrossRef Lewis-Beck, M. S. (1991). French national elections: Political economic forecasts. European Journal of Political Economy, 7(4), 487–496.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lewis-Beck, M. S., & Stegmaier, M. (2013). The VP-function revisited: A survey of the literature on vote and popularity functions after over 40 Years. Public Choice, 157, 367–385. Lewis-Beck, M. S., & Stegmaier, M. (2013). The VP-function revisited: A survey of the literature on vote and popularity functions after over 40 Years. Public Choice, 157, 367–385.
Zurück zum Zitat Lippman, W. (1925). The phantom public. New York: Harcourt, Brace. Lippman, W. (1925). The phantom public. New York: Harcourt, Brace.
Zurück zum Zitat MacKuen, M. B., Erikson, R. S., & Stimson, J. A. (1992). Peasants or bankers? The American electorate and the U.S. economy. American Political Science Review, 86(3), 597–611.CrossRef MacKuen, M. B., Erikson, R. S., & Stimson, J. A. (1992). Peasants or bankers? The American electorate and the U.S. economy. American Political Science Review, 86(3), 597–611.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Malhotra, N., & Margalit, Y. (2010). Short-term communication effects or longstanding dispositions? The public’s response to the financial crisis of 2008. Journal of Politics, 72(3), 852–867.CrossRef Malhotra, N., & Margalit, Y. (2010). Short-term communication effects or longstanding dispositions? The public’s response to the financial crisis of 2008. Journal of Politics, 72(3), 852–867.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. J., & Gearing, A. F. (1998). Civic engagement in a post-communist state. Political Psychology, 19(3), 615–637.CrossRef Mondak, J. J., & Gearing, A. F. (1998). Civic engagement in a post-communist state. Political Psychology, 19(3), 615–637.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nadeau, R., Lewis-Beck, M. S., & Bélanger, É. (2013). Economics and elections revisited. Comparative Political Studies, 46(5), 551–573.CrossRef Nadeau, R., Lewis-Beck, M. S., & Bélanger, É. (2013). Economics and elections revisited. Comparative Political Studies, 46(5), 551–573.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nannestad, P., & Paldam, M. (2000). Into Pandora’s box of economic evaluations: A study of the Danish macro VP-function, 1986–1997. Electoral Studies, 19(2), 123–140.CrossRef Nannestad, P., & Paldam, M. (2000). Into Pandora’s box of economic evaluations: A study of the Danish macro VP-function, 1986–1997. Electoral Studies, 19(2), 123–140.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Owen, A., & Tucker, J. A. (2010). Past is still present: Micro-level comparisons of conventional vs. transitional economic voting in three Polish elections. Electoral Studies, 29(1), 25–39.CrossRef Owen, A., & Tucker, J. A. (2010). Past is still present: Micro-level comparisons of conventional vs. transitional economic voting in three Polish elections. Electoral Studies, 29(1), 25–39.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Palmer, H. D., & Duch, R. M. (2001). Do surveys provide representative or whimsical assessments of the economy? Political Analysis, 9(1), 58–77.CrossRef Palmer, H. D., & Duch, R. M. (2001). Do surveys provide representative or whimsical assessments of the economy? Political Analysis, 9(1), 58–77.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Peltzman, S. (1990). How efficient is the voting market? Journal of Law and Economics, 33(1), 27–63.CrossRef Peltzman, S. (1990). How efficient is the voting market? Journal of Law and Economics, 33(1), 27–63.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schwarz, N., & Sudman, S. (1994). Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports. New York: Springer.CrossRef Schwarz, N., & Sudman, S. (1994). Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports. New York: Springer.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Soroka, S. N. (2006). Good news and bad news: Asymmetric responses to economic information. Journal of Politics, 68(2), 372–385.MathSciNetCrossRef Soroka, S. N. (2006). Good news and bad news: Asymmetric responses to economic information. Journal of Politics, 68(2), 372–385.MathSciNetCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stubager, R., Lewis-Beck, M. S., & Nadeau, R. (2013). Reaching for profit in the welfare state: Patrimonial economic voting in Denmark. Electoral Studies, 32(3), 438–444.CrossRef Stubager, R., Lewis-Beck, M. S., & Nadeau, R. (2013). Reaching for profit in the welfare state: Patrimonial economic voting in Denmark. Electoral Studies, 32(3), 438–444.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Brug, W., van der EijK, C., & Franklin, M. N. (2007). The economy and the vote: Economic conditions and elections in fifteen countries. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Van der Brug, W., van der EijK, C., & Franklin, M. N. (2007). The economy and the vote: Economic conditions and elections in fifteen countries. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J. (2004). Floating voters in U.S. presidential elections. In W. E. Saris & P. M. Sniderman (Eds.), Studies in public opinion: Attitudes, nonattitudes, measurement error, and change (pp. 1948–2000). Princenton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Zaller, J. (2004). Floating voters in U.S. presidential elections. In W. E. Saris & P. M. Sniderman (Eds.), Studies in public opinion: Attitudes, nonattitudes, measurement error, and change (pp. 1948–2000). Princenton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Metadaten
Titel
More Misinformed than Myopic: Economic Retrospections and the Voter’s Time Horizon
verfasst von
Timothy Hellwig
Dani M. Marinova
Publikationsdatum
13.12.2014
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 4/2015
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-014-9295-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2015

Political Behavior 4/2015 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner