Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 4/2010

01.12.2010 | Original Paper

Masculine Republicans and Feminine Democrats: Gender and Americans’ Explicit and Implicit Images of the Political Parties

verfasst von: Nicholas J. G. Winter

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 4/2010

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

During the past three decades Americans have come to view the parties increasingly in gendered terms of masculinity and femininity. Utilizing three decades of American National Election Studies data and the results of a cognitive reaction-time experiment, this paper demonstrates empirically that these connections between party images and gender stereotypes have been forged at the explicit level of the traits that Americans associate with each party, and also at the implicit level of unconscious cognitive connections between gender and party stereotypes. These connections between the parties and masculinity and femininity have important implications for citizens’ political cognition and for the study of American political behavior.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
Some scholars have explored citizen’s party images, using the ANES open-ended likes and dislikes questions, but none has focused on gender (Sanders 1988; Baumer and Gold 1995; Trilling 1976); related work on the contents of partisan stereotypes has similarly not focused on gender (e.g. Rahn 1993; Bastedo and Lodge 1980; Hamill et al. 1985). More recently, Danny Hayes has explored the traits that citizens associate with the parties’ presidential nominees, but without an explicit focus on the gendered nature of those trait attributions (2005).
 
2
For overviews of the gender gap literature, see Huddy et al. (2008) and Sapiro (2003, pp. 605–610). For an overview of the literature on female candidates, see Dolan (2008).
 
3
Interestingly, this process appears to be only partly voluntary; speakers of languages that gender nouns tend to associate a wide range of gendered characteristics with objects depending on the gender their language assigns to the noun (Phillips and Boroditsky 2003).
 
4
See, for example, Spence and Buckner (1995), Spence et al. (1978, 1979), Bem (1974, 1981, 1987), and Maccoby (1987). For a review of the vast literature on the conceptualization, measurement, and contents of ideas about masculinity and femininity, see Lippa (2005, Chap. 2). There is considerable cross-cultural consistency in gender stereotypes, amid important cultural variation, though this consistency—and debates about its social or biological bases—is tangential to the purposes of this paper (see, e.g., Ortner 1974, 1996, Chap. 7).
 
5
There is an extensive literature in social psychology showing that masculine and feminine traits and other characteristics do not, in fact, form a single bipolar dimension at the individual level (Constantinople 2005); rather, both are multidimensional constructs that vary independently (Bem 1974; Spence et al. 1978). Nevertheless, people generally believe that they form coherent and oppositional packages (Deaux 1987).
 
6
Helen Haste argues that the idea of gender difference serves as a sort of master metaphor that gives meaning to myriad dualities at the center of Western culture, including public–private, rational-intuitive, active–passive, hard-soft, thinking-feeling, and many more (1993). On the role of gender ideals in the politics of the American founding and early republic, see Kann (1998), Kerber (1986), Kang (2009), and Bloch (1987).
 
7
Huddy and Terkildsen present evidence that the gender associations of issues are not simply the product of the idea that women are more liberal than men; rather, the gender associations flow importantly from stereotyped beliefs about women’s traits and abilities.
 
8
I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for bringing this point to my attention.
 
9
The party-candidate master codes are listed in the appendix to the ANES cumulative file dataset. The mentions are in variables VCF0375A–VCF0379A (Democratic Party likes), VCF0381A–VCF0385A (Democratic dislikes), VCF0387A–VCF0391A (Republican likes), and VCF0393A–VCF0397A (Republican dislikes). In 1972 the ANES reported only the first three mentions for each target, although up to five were collected in the interview. The 1972 dataset does report how many mentions each respondent made, up to five; this indicates that about 2% of respondents mentioned more than three things in a each category. Restricting the analysis in other years to include only the first three mentions does not affect the patterns of results in those years, which suggests that the omission of the fourth and fifth mentions in 1972 probably does not substantially influence the patterns observed in that year.
 
10
Across presidential years from 1972 through 2004, 28% of respondents failed to mention anything about any party. This ranged from a low of 22% in 2004 to a high of 34% in 1980.
 
11
Of respondents with no party likes or dislikes, 25% are pure independents, compared to 6% of other respondents. Ten percent of these respondents refuse to rate the one or both parties on the feeling thermometer (compared to 2% of others), and 29% rate both parties equally at 50 degrees (compared to 5% of others). Finally, these respondents average 0.38 on the zero-to-one ANES interviewer assessment of political knowledge, compared to 0.60. All of this is consistent with Geer’s conclusion that those who fail to answer open-ended questions are, generally speaking, not interested in the question, rather than being unable to articulate a meaningful response (1988).
 
12
Of course, some issues and political groups themselves have implicit or explicit gender associations. An important area for future research is the ways that the gendered traits associated with the parties interact with gendered issue and group associations.
 
13
Of course, the party-candidate master coding introduces additional distance between the available data and respondents’ own words, and raises both reliability and validity concerns. Here it is somewhat of an advantage that the ANES coding scheme was not developed or deployed with masculinity and femininity in mind; while this probably introduces noise into the coding, hurting reliability, it means that I am not constrained to a particular definition of gender, and the ANES coders are unlikely to have been biased by their own possible gendered associations for the parties. Thus, there is little reason to think that either would bias the ANES coding in favor of my hypotheses. Nevertheless, in the absence of the original verbatim text there is no way to be certain. The ANES is in the process of revising its open-ended coding procedures, and plans to make verbatim text more readily available in future studies, so future researchers may be in a better position to address these concerns directly. See http://​www.​electionstudies.​org/​conferences/​2008Methods/​MethodsConferenc​e.​htm.
 
14
The kappa statistic for inter-rater agreement among the three raters was 0.76 for masculine traits and 0.75 for feminine traits; both in the range characterized by Landis and Koch as “substantial” (1977, p. 165; Cohen 1960). Much of the coding disagreement turned out to be over ANES master codes that do not actually appear frequently in the data, so the basic pattern of results presented below hold up when I substitute each individual coder’s initial classifications for the final consensual coding.
 
15
The distinction between positive and negative traits was collapsed for the analysis, so stereotypically masculine traits that are culturally sanctioned (e.g., independent, code 315) and those that are not (e.g., cold or aloof, code 438) were both classified simply as masculine, and normatively positive and negative feminine traits (e.g., kind, code 435 vs. indecisive, code 304) were all classified as feminine. In practice, the overwhelming majority of respondents’ party likes were normatively positive traits, and dislikes were overwhelmingly negative, although there were a few exceptions. For example, a small handful of respondents indicated in 2004 that they liked the fact that the Democratic Party lacked a definite philosophy (code 836). This example makes clear that a trait that is often considered a weakness can be a political asset in the right political context, a point to which I return in the conclusion.
 
16
Reassuringly, the pattern of results is essentially unchanged when multiple mentions by a single individual are collapsed, which reframes the analysis in terms of the proportion of respondents who mention gendered traits, rather than the proportion of mentions.
 
17
The patterns are not any different in the non-presidential years for which party likes and dislikes are available.
 
18
Overall, 45.1% of mentions related to issues, 21.2% to groups, and 5.2% to individuals. The proportions in these categories varied somewhat by party: for the Democratic Party, 76.6% of likes and 62.1% of dislikes fell in one of those three categories, as did 70.4% of Republican Party likes and 75.0% of Republican Party dislikes.
 
19
Of course, some mentions of issue positions, such as a party being “tough on crime” or “soft on communism” may reflect a respondent’s reaction to a more symbolic masculinity or femininity. As I discuss above, these sorts of issue mentions were excluded from possible coding as traits for two reasons. First, the ANES master codes simply do not provide enough detail about respondent’s actual mentions of issues to code issues in this way, and second, even with verbatim text it would be beyond the scope of this analysis to attempt to ascertain whether a particular reference “really” refers simply to an issue position, or to a possibly-gendered aspect of the approach to the issue, or some combination. As I mention in footnote 12, this is an interesting area for future research.
 
20
The slight jumps in masculine Democratic dislikes in 1992 and especially 2000 are driven mostly by references to sex scandals (code 719).
 
21
Political knowledge is based on the ANES pre-election interviewer’s assessment of the respondent’s level of political information (VCF0050A). John Zaller reports that this assessment performs very well as a general measure of political knowledge (1992, p. 338); this measure has the added advantage of being reasonably comparable across years, especially in contrast with fact-based measures. The results are somewhat attenuated, but follow essentially the same pattern, when I replace political knowledge with a motivation-based measure of political engagement, based on respondents’ self-reported interest in politics and the campaign, and when I substitute respondent education. This is consistent with Zaller’s comparisons of different strategies for measuring habitual attention to politics (1992, p. 335).
 
22
Party affiliation is drawn from the standard ANES party affiliation battery (VCF0301), with independents who lean toward a party classified as independents. The results are substantively unchanged when leaners are reclassified as partisans.
 
23
For each like and dislike type, between one-third and one-half of respondents gave no mentions at all. This means that were I to run a model among all respondents, the coefficients would pick up the tendency to mention anything at all—essentially a model of the positivity or negativity of feelings about each party—rather than distinguishing those respondents who mention a gendered trait from those who do not, from among respondents who say something about the party. In any case, the substantive results are essentially the same when each party’s models are run among all respondents who mentioned any likes or any dislikes about that party, although among this broader universe respondent partisanship captures a bit of the tendency of partisans to mention things—including gendered traits—that they like about their own party and things they dislike about the other party. The results are also the same when likes and dislikes are collapsed into a single masculine model and a single feminine model for each party. Results available from the author.
 
24
Marginal effects were calculated using the MFX command in Stata. For the dummy variables (party affiliation and gender), the marginal effect is the difference in probability between an otherwise-average respondent who has the characteristic and one who does not. For political knowledge the calculation is the instantaneous marginal impact of knowledge on the probability for an average respondent. Because political knowledge is coded to run from zero to one and because the predicted probability curve is quite linear across the entire range, this marginal effect is almost exactly the difference in predicted probabilities between otherwise-average respondents who are most informed and least informed.
 
25
Models that include more extensive sets of independent variables yield entirely consistent results, both for these Democratic Party models and for the Republican Party results I present below. In particular, the probability of gendering the party is essentially equivalent for conservatives, moderates, and independents, for residents of different regions, for white and black respondents, and for older and younger respondents. In addition, there is no evidence of an interaction between engagement and either partisanship or gender, nor between gender and partisanship. As I mention in footnote 21, respondent education acts as a weak proxy for attention to politics, although its effects are washed out when political knowledge is included with education in a single model.
 
26
There is considerable debate on the broader role of whites’ racial attitudes in contemporary American public opinion (Kinder and Sanders 1996; Sniderman and Carmines 1997; see Sears et al. 2000 for a recent set of entries in this debate) and on implicit racial priming in particular (Valentino et al. 2002; Huber and Lapinski 2006; Huber and Lapinski 2008; Mendelberg 2008a, b).
 
27
There is lively debate on the relatively importance of (unconscious) cognitive accessibility versus (conscious) evaluation of importance in the priming of political attitudes (Valentino et al. 2002; see also Winter 2008, pp. 147–151). The theoretical accounts developed by Zaller (1992) and Mendelberg (2001) both include priming and accessibility as key mechanisms, though neither measures accessibility directly (Miller and Krosnick 2000; Nelson et al. 1997).
 
28
The feminine, masculine, and filler words were matched for length and frequency of appearance in the English lexicon (Kucera and Francis 1967). The nonsense strings were created by swapping letters or phonemes in real words, and were matched with the words for length. The LDT portion of the study began with a shorter set of training trails to give participants a chance to get used to the identification task.
 
29
The LDT was implemented using PxLab, an open-source software application for psychological experiments, available from http://​www.​uni-mannheim.​de/​fakul/​psycho/​irtel/​pxlab/​index.​html. The web survey was implemented in PHPQuestionnaire (http://​www.​chumpsoft.​com), which was modified by the author to implement streaming video and to interface with PxLab.
 
30
As is typical with student samples, the participant pool is not representative of a national sample. The participants are relatively young (age averaged 20 and ranged from 17 to 32). About two-thirds (69%) of participants were women; 54% identified as Democrats, 26% as Republicans and 19% as independent. There were no substantively or statistically significant demographic differences across conditions, and there is no evidence that gender, party identification, or political knowledge moderate any of the findings reported below. The study was approved by the University of Virginia institutional review board, protocol number 2008-0408.
 
31
The ads were for the Chevy Malibu and for the Apple iPod. There was also a fourth condition, which included a pair of political advertisements in place of the product commercials. Participants in this fourth condition were omitted from the present analysis.
 
32
Because reaction time data are notoriously noisy, following standard practice I exclude trials with extreme outlier response times in calculating the averages, as well as trials in which a respondent misidentified a target word as a non-word.
 
33
Thus, I regress individual-level average reaction time to feminine words on individual-level average reaction time to neutral words and a dummy variable for the Democratic condition. Because the estimated coefficients for neutral-word reaction times are very close to one, the approach I take is almost identical to simply subtracting each respondent’s neutral-word average from that respondent’s feminine-word average. Employing this alternate approach generates estimates of the size of the priming effect that are within a few milliseconds of the estimates I present below.
 
34
Miller et al. find, for example, that people tend to explain gender differences among voters and professors—both prototypically masculine—in terms of characteristics of women, while explaining gender differences among elementary school teachers—prototypically feminine—in terms of characteristics of men (1991).
 
35
This mapping of one binary distinction onto another raises the question of how third parties are understood. Interestingly, Baker notes that during the height of the nineteenth century party era, men who were not committed to either of the major parties were seen as “political impotent” and referred to as the “third sex” of American politics (1984, p. 628). Hoganson cites references from this era to members of third parties as “‘eunuchs,’ ‘man-milliners,’ members of a ‘third sex,’ ‘political hermaphrodites,’ and ‘the neuter gender not popular either in nature or society’” (1998, p. 23). On a related note, Fausto-Sterling (1993) argues that sex is itself not as simple a binary distinction as we often assume.
 
36
Schwarzenegger deployed this phrase—drawn from a Saturday Night Live sketch that mocked Schwarzenegger himself—while campaigning for George H. W. Bush in 1988 and 1992, then again in 2004 as Governor of California in battles with the legislature, and most recently at the 2004 Republican national convention.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Adams, G. D. (1997). Abortion: Evidence of an issue evolution. American Journal of Political Science, 41(3), 718–737.CrossRef Adams, G. D. (1997). Abortion: Evidence of an issue evolution. American Journal of Political Science, 41(3), 718–737.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Alexander, D., & Andersen, K. (1993). Gender as a factor in the attribution of leadership traits. Political Research Quarterly, 46(3), 527–545. Alexander, D., & Andersen, K. (1993). Gender as a factor in the attribution of leadership traits. Political Research Quarterly, 46(3), 527–545.
Zurück zum Zitat Baker, P. (1984). The domestication of politics: Women and American Political Society, 1780–1920. The American Historical Review, 89(3), 620–647.CrossRef Baker, P. (1984). The domestication of politics: Women and American Political Society, 1780–1920. The American Historical Review, 89(3), 620–647.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bargh, J. A., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 73–79.CrossRef Bargh, J. A., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 73–79.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bastedo, R. W., & Lodge, M. (1980). The meaning of party labels. Political Behavior, 2(3), 287–308.CrossRef Bastedo, R. W., & Lodge, M. (1980). The meaning of party labels. Political Behavior, 2(3), 287–308.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baumer, D. C., & Gold, H. J. (1995). Party images and the American electorate. American Politics Quarterly, 23(1), 33–61.CrossRef Baumer, D. C., & Gold, H. J. (1995). Party images and the American electorate. American Politics Quarterly, 23(1), 33–61.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bederman, G. (1995). Manliness and civilization: A cultural history of gender and race in the United States, 1880–1917. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Bederman, G. (1995). Manliness and civilization: A cultural history of gender and race in the United States, 1880–1917. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.CrossRef Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88(4), 354–364.CrossRef Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88(4), 354–364.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bem, S. L. (1987). Masculinity and femininity exist only in the mind of the perceiver. In J. M. Reinisch, L. A. Rosenblum, & S. A. Sanders (Eds.), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives (pp. 304–311). New York: Oxford University Press. Bem, S. L. (1987). Masculinity and femininity exist only in the mind of the perceiver. In J. M. Reinisch, L. A. Rosenblum, & S. A. Sanders (Eds.), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives (pp. 304–311). New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Bloch, R. H. (1987). The gendered meanings of virtue in revolutionary America. Signs, 13(1), 37–58.CrossRef Bloch, R. H. (1987). The gendered meanings of virtue in revolutionary America. Signs, 13(1), 37–58.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carlson, J. M., & Boring, M. K. (1981). Androgyny and politics: The effects of winning and losing on candidate image. International Political Science Review, 2(4), 481–491.CrossRef Carlson, J. M., & Boring, M. K. (1981). Androgyny and politics: The effects of winning and losing on candidate image. International Political Science Review, 2(4), 481–491.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carpini, D., Michael, X., & Fuchs, E. R. (1993). The year of the woman? Candidates, voters, and the 1992 elections. Political Science Quarterly, 108(1), 29–36.CrossRef Carpini, D., Michael, X., & Fuchs, E. R. (1993). The year of the woman? Candidates, voters, and the 1992 elections. Political Science Quarterly, 108(1), 29–36.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20(1), 37–46.CrossRef Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20(1), 37–46.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Constantinople, A. (2005). Masculinity–femininity: An exception to a famous dictum? Feminism and Psychology, 15(4), 385–407.CrossRef Constantinople, A. (2005). Masculinity–femininity: An exception to a famous dictum? Feminism and Psychology, 15(4), 385–407.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cooper, F. R. (2008). Our first unisex president? Black masculinity and Obama’s feminine side. Denver University Law Review, 86, 633–661. Cooper, F. R. (2008). Our first unisex president? Black masculinity and Obama’s feminine side. Denver University Law Review, 86, 633–661.
Zurück zum Zitat Costain, A. N. (1991). After Reagan: New party attitudes toward gender. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 515, 114–125.CrossRef Costain, A. N. (1991). After Reagan: New party attitudes toward gender. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 515, 114–125.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Deaux, K. (1987). Psychological constructions of masculinity and femininity. In J. M. Reinisch, L. A. Rosenblum, & S. A. Sanders (Eds.), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives (pp. 289–303). New York: Oxford University Press. Deaux, K. (1987). Psychological constructions of masculinity and femininity. In J. M. Reinisch, L. A. Rosenblum, & S. A. Sanders (Eds.), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives (pp. 289–303). New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Deaux, K., & Emswiller, T. (1974). Explanations of successful performance on sex-linked tasks: What is skill for the male is luck for the female. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29(1), 80–85.CrossRef Deaux, K., & Emswiller, T. (1974). Explanations of successful performance on sex-linked tasks: What is skill for the male is luck for the female. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29(1), 80–85.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 95–109.CrossRef Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 95–109.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dolan, K. (1998). Voting for women in the “year of the woman”. American Journal of Political Science, 42(1), 272–293.CrossRef Dolan, K. (1998). Voting for women in the “year of the woman”. American Journal of Political Science, 42(1), 272–293.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dolan, K. (2004). The impact of candidate sex on evaluations of candidates for the U.S. House of representatives. Social Science Quarterly, 85(1), 206–217.CrossRef Dolan, K. (2004). The impact of candidate sex on evaluations of candidates for the U.S. House of representatives. Social Science Quarterly, 85(1), 206–217.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dolan, K. (2008). Women as candidates in American politics: The continuing impact of sex and gender. In W. Christina, B. Karen, & L. Baldez (Eds.), Political women and American democracy (pp. 110–127). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Dolan, K. (2008). Women as candidates in American politics: The continuing impact of sex and gender. In W. Christina, B. Karen, & L. Baldez (Eds.), Political women and American democracy (pp. 110–127). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ducat, S. (2004). The wimp factor: Gender gaps, holy wars, and the politics of anxious masculinity. Boston: Beacon Press. Ducat, S. (2004). The wimp factor: Gender gaps, holy wars, and the politics of anxious masculinity. Boston: Beacon Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Duerst-Lahti, G. (2006). Presidential elections: Gendered space and the case of 2004. In S. J. Carroll & R. L. Fox (Eds.), Gender and elections: Shaping the future of American politics (pp. 12–42). New York: Cambridge University Press. Duerst-Lahti, G. (2006). Presidential elections: Gendered space and the case of 2004. In S. J. Carroll & R. L. Fox (Eds.), Gender and elections: Shaping the future of American politics (pp. 12–42). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Duerst-Lahti, G. (2008). Seeing what has always been: Opening study of the presidency. PS: Political Science & Politics, 41(04), 733–737.CrossRef Duerst-Lahti, G. (2008). Seeing what has always been: Opening study of the presidency. PS: Political Science & Politics, 41(04), 733–737.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Duerst-Lahti, G., & Verstegen, (1995). Making something of absence: The ‘year of the woman’ and women’s political representation. In G. Duerst-Lahti & R. M. Kelly (Eds.), Gender power, leadership, and governance (pp. 211–238). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Duerst-Lahti, G., & Verstegen, (1995). Making something of absence: The ‘year of the woman’ and women’s political representation. In G. Duerst-Lahti & R. M. Kelly (Eds.), Gender power, leadership, and governance (pp. 211–238). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Elder, L. (2008). Whither republican women: The growing partisan gap among women in congress. The Forum, 6(1), 13.CrossRef Elder, L. (2008). Whither republican women: The growing partisan gap among women in congress. The Forum, 6(1), 13.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Etcheson, N. (1995). Manliness and the political culture of the old northwest, 1790–1860. Journal of the Early Republic, 15(1), 59–77.CrossRef Etcheson, N. (1995). Manliness and the political culture of the old northwest, 1790–1860. Journal of the Early Republic, 15(1), 59–77.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fahey, A. C. (2007). French and feminine: Hegemonic masculinity and the emasculation of John Kerry in the 2004 presidential race. Critical Studies in Media Communication, 24(2), 132–150.CrossRef Fahey, A. C. (2007). French and feminine: Hegemonic masculinity and the emasculation of John Kerry in the 2004 presidential race. Critical Studies in Media Communication, 24(2), 132–150.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fausto-Sterling, A. (1993). The five sexes: Why male and female are not enough. The Sciences, 33(2), 20–24. Fausto-Sterling, A. (1993). The five sexes: Why male and female are not enough. The Sciences, 33(2), 20–24.
Zurück zum Zitat Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. In H. Clyde & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Research methods in personality and social psychology (pp. 74–97). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. In H. Clyde & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Research methods in personality and social psychology (pp. 74–97). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Foushee, H. C., Helmreich, R. L., & Spence, J. T. (1979). Implicit theories of masculinity and femininity: Dualistic or bipolar? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 3(3), 259–269.CrossRef Foushee, H. C., Helmreich, R. L., & Spence, J. T. (1979). Implicit theories of masculinity and femininity: Dualistic or bipolar? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 3(3), 259–269.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Freeman, J. (1975). The politics of women’s liberation: A case study of an emerging social movement and its relation to the policy process. New York: D. McKay. Freeman, J. (1975). The politics of women’s liberation: A case study of an emerging social movement and its relation to the policy process. New York: D. McKay.
Zurück zum Zitat Freeman, J. (1987). Whom you know versus whom you represent: Feminist influence in the democratic and republican parties. In M. F. Katzenstein & C. Mueller (Eds.), The women’s movements of the United States and Western Europe: Consciousness, political opportunity, and public policy (pp. 215–244). Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Freeman, J. (1987). Whom you know versus whom you represent: Feminist influence in the democratic and republican parties. In M. F. Katzenstein & C. Mueller (Eds.), The women’s movements of the United States and Western Europe: Consciousness, political opportunity, and public policy (pp. 215–244). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Freeman, J. (1992). Feminism vs. family values: Women at the 1992 democratic and republican conventions. PS: Political Science and Politics, 26(1), 21–28.CrossRef Freeman, J. (1992). Feminism vs. family values: Women at the 1992 democratic and republican conventions. PS: Political Science and Politics, 26(1), 21–28.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Geer, J. G. (1988). What do open-ended questions measure? Public Opinion Quarterly, 52(3), 365–371.CrossRef Geer, J. G. (1988). What do open-ended questions measure? Public Opinion Quarterly, 52(3), 365–371.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Geer, J. G. (1991). The electorate’s partisan evaluations: Evidence of a continuing democratic edge. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55(2), 218–231.CrossRef Geer, J. G. (1991). The electorate’s partisan evaluations: Evidence of a continuing democratic edge. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55(2), 218–231.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gilens, M. (1988). Gender and support for Reagan: A comprehensive model of presidential approval. American Journal of Political Science, 32(1), 19–49.CrossRef Gilens, M. (1988). Gender and support for Reagan: A comprehensive model of presidential approval. American Journal of Political Science, 32(1), 19–49.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27.CrossRef Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hamill, R., Lodge, M., & Blake, F. (1985). The breadth, depth, and utility of class, partisan, and ideological schemata. American Journal of Political Science, 29(4), 850–870.CrossRef Hamill, R., Lodge, M., & Blake, F. (1985). The breadth, depth, and utility of class, partisan, and ideological schemata. American Journal of Political Science, 29(4), 850–870.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hancock, A.-M. (2007). When multiplication doesn’t equal quick addition: Examining intersectionality as a research paradigm. Perspectives on Politics, 5(1), 63–79.CrossRef Hancock, A.-M. (2007). When multiplication doesn’t equal quick addition: Examining intersectionality as a research paradigm. Perspectives on Politics, 5(1), 63–79.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Haste, H. (1993). The sexual metaphor. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Haste, H. (1993). The sexual metaphor. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Hayes, D. (2005). Candidate qualities through a partisan lens: A theory of trait ownership. American Journal of Political Science, 49(4), 908–923.CrossRef Hayes, D. (2005). Candidate qualities through a partisan lens: A theory of trait ownership. American Journal of Political Science, 49(4), 908–923.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hayes, D. (2009a). Feminine democrats, masculine republicans: Gender and party stereotyping in candidate trait attribution. Paper presented at the Midwest Political Science Association annual meeting, Chicago. Hayes, D. (2009a). Feminine democrats, masculine republicans: Gender and party stereotyping in candidate trait attribution. Paper presented at the Midwest Political Science Association annual meeting, Chicago.
Zurück zum Zitat Hayes, D. (2009b). Has television personalized voting behavior? Political Behavior, 31(2), 231–260.CrossRef Hayes, D. (2009b). Has television personalized voting behavior? Political Behavior, 31(2), 231–260.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hoganson, K. L. (1998). Fighting for American manhood: How gender politics provoked the Spanish-American and Philippine-American wars. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Hoganson, K. L. (1998). Fighting for American manhood: How gender politics provoked the Spanish-American and Philippine-American wars. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Huber, G. A., & Lapinski, J. S. (2006). The ‘race card’ revisited: Assessing racial priming in policy contests. American Journal of Political Science, 50(2), 421–440.CrossRef Huber, G. A., & Lapinski, J. S. (2006). The ‘race card’ revisited: Assessing racial priming in policy contests. American Journal of Political Science, 50(2), 421–440.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huber, G. A., & Lapinski, J. S. (2008). Testing the implicit–explicit model of racialized political communication. Perspectives on Politics, 6(01), 125–134.CrossRef Huber, G. A., & Lapinski, J. S. (2008). Testing the implicit–explicit model of racialized political communication. Perspectives on Politics, 6(01), 125–134.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huddy, L., & Capelos, T. (2002). Gender stereotyping and candidate evaluations: Good news and bad news for women politicians. In V. C. Ottati, R. S. Tindale, J. Edwards, F. B. Bryant, L. Heath, D. C. O’Connell, Y. Suarez-Balcazar, & E. J. Posavac (Eds.), The social psychology of politics (pp. 29–53). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Huddy, L., & Capelos, T. (2002). Gender stereotyping and candidate evaluations: Good news and bad news for women politicians. In V. C. Ottati, R. S. Tindale, J. Edwards, F. B. Bryant, L. Heath, D. C. O’Connell, Y. Suarez-Balcazar, & E. J. Posavac (Eds.), The social psychology of politics (pp. 29–53). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Zurück zum Zitat Huddy, L., Cassese, E., & Lizotte, M.-K. (2008). Gender, public opinion, and political reasoning. In C. Wolbrecht, K. Beckwith, & L. Baldez (Eds.), Political women and American democracy (pp. 31–49). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Huddy, L., Cassese, E., & Lizotte, M.-K. (2008). Gender, public opinion, and political reasoning. In C. Wolbrecht, K. Beckwith, & L. Baldez (Eds.), Political women and American democracy (pp. 31–49). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huddy, L., & Terkildsen, N. (1993). Gender stereotypes and the perception of male and female candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37(1), 119–147.CrossRef Huddy, L., & Terkildsen, N. (1993). Gender stereotypes and the perception of male and female candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37(1), 119–147.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hurwitz, J., & Peffley, M. (2005). Playing the race card in the post-Willie Horton era: The impact of racialized code words on support for punitive crime policy. Public Opinion Quarterly, 69(1), 99–112.CrossRef Hurwitz, J., & Peffley, M. (2005). Playing the race card in the post-Willie Horton era: The impact of racialized code words on support for punitive crime policy. Public Opinion Quarterly, 69(1), 99–112.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Iyengar, S., Valentino, N. A., Ansolabehere, S., & Simon, A. F. (1997). Running as a woman: Gender stereotyping in women’s campaigns. In P. Norris (Ed.), Women, media, and politics (pp. 77–98). New York: Oxford University Press. Iyengar, S., Valentino, N. A., Ansolabehere, S., & Simon, A. F. (1997). Running as a woman: Gender stereotyping in women’s campaigns. In P. Norris (Ed.), Women, media, and politics (pp. 77–98). New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Jeffords, S. (1994). Hard bodies: Hollywood masculinity in the Reagan era. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Jeffords, S. (1994). Hard bodies: Hollywood masculinity in the Reagan era. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahn, K. F. (1993). Gender differences in campaign messages: The political advertisements of men and women candidates for U. S. Senate. Political Research Quarterly, 46(3), 481–502. Kahn, K. F. (1993). Gender differences in campaign messages: The political advertisements of men and women candidates for U. S. Senate. Political Research Quarterly, 46(3), 481–502.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahn, K. F. (1996). The political consequences of being a woman: How stereotypes influence the conduct and consequences of political campaigns. New York: Columbia University Press. Kahn, K. F. (1996). The political consequences of being a woman: How stereotypes influence the conduct and consequences of political campaigns. New York: Columbia University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kang, J. M. (2009). Manliness and the constitution. Harvard Journal of Law and Public Policy, 32(1), 261–332. Kang, J. M. (2009). Manliness and the constitution. Harvard Journal of Law and Public Policy, 32(1), 261–332.
Zurück zum Zitat Kann, M. E. (1998). A republic of men: The American founders, gendered language, and patriarchal politics. New York: New York University Press. Kann, M. E. (1998). A republic of men: The American founders, gendered language, and patriarchal politics. New York: New York University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kerber, L. K. (1986). Women of the republic: Intellect and ideology in revolutionary America. New York: Norton. Kerber, L. K. (1986). Women of the republic: Intellect and ideology in revolutionary America. New York: Norton.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim, T. P. (1998). Clarence Thomas and the politicization of candidate gender in the 1992 senate elections. Legislative Studies Quarterly, 23(3), 399–418.CrossRef Kim, T. P. (1998). Clarence Thomas and the politicization of candidate gender in the 1992 senate elections. Legislative Studies Quarterly, 23(3), 399–418.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kimmel, M. S. (1987). The cult of masculinity: American social character and the legacy of the cowboy. In E. M. Kaufman (Ed.), Beyond patriarchy: Essays by men on pleasure, power, and change (pp. 235–249). Toronto: Oxford University Press. Kimmel, M. S. (1987). The cult of masculinity: American social character and the legacy of the cowboy. In E. M. Kaufman (Ed.), Beyond patriarchy: Essays by men on pleasure, power, and change (pp. 235–249). Toronto: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Koch, J. W. (2002). Gender stereotypes and citizens’ impressions of house candidates’ ideological orientations. American Journal of Political Science, 46(2), 453–462.CrossRef Koch, J. W. (2002). Gender stereotypes and citizens’ impressions of house candidates’ ideological orientations. American Journal of Political Science, 46(2), 453–462.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press. Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Ladd, E. C. (1997). Media framing of the gender gap. In P. Norris (Ed.), Women, media, and politics (pp. 113–128). New York: Oxford University Press. Ladd, E. C. (1997). Media framing of the gender gap. In P. Norris (Ed.), Women, media, and politics (pp. 113–128). New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Lakoff, G. (2002). Moral politics: How liberals and conservatives think (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, G. (2002). Moral politics: How liberals and conservatives think (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159–174.CrossRef Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159–174.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Leinbach, M. D., Hort, B. E., & Fagot, B. I. (1997). Bears are for boys: Metaphorical associations in young children’s gender stereotypes. Cognitive Development, 12(1), 107–130.CrossRef Leinbach, M. D., Hort, B. E., & Fagot, B. I. (1997). Bears are for boys: Metaphorical associations in young children’s gender stereotypes. Cognitive Development, 12(1), 107–130.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lippa, R. A. (2005). Gender, nature, and nurture (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Lippa, R. A. (2005). Gender, nature, and nurture (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Zurück zum Zitat Maccoby, E. E. (1987). The varied meanings of ‘masculine’ and ‘feminine’. In J. M. Reinisch, L. A. Rosenblum, & S. A. Sanders (Eds.), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives (pp. 227–239). New York: Oxford University Press. Maccoby, E. E. (1987). The varied meanings of ‘masculine’ and ‘feminine’. In J. M. Reinisch, L. A. Rosenblum, & S. A. Sanders (Eds.), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives (pp. 227–239). New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Malin, B. J. (2005). American masculinity under Clinton: Popular media and the “crisis of masculinity”. New York: Peter Lang. Malin, B. J. (2005). American masculinity under Clinton: Popular media and the “crisis of masculinity”. New York: Peter Lang.
Zurück zum Zitat Mansbridge, J. J. (1985). Myth and reality: The ERA and the gender gap in the 1980 election. Public Opinion Quarterly, 49(2), 164–178.CrossRef Mansbridge, J. J. (1985). Myth and reality: The ERA and the gender gap in the 1980 election. Public Opinion Quarterly, 49(2), 164–178.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Matland, R. E., & King, D. C. (2002). Women as candidates in congressional elections. In C. S. Rosenthal (Ed.), Women transforming congress (pp. 119–145). Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press. Matland, R. E., & King, D. C. (2002). Women as candidates in congressional elections. In C. S. Rosenthal (Ed.), Women transforming congress (pp. 119–145). Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press.
Zurück zum Zitat McDermott, M. L. (1997). Voting cues in low-information elections: Candidate gender as a social information variable in contemporary United States elections. American Journal of Political Science, 41(1), 270–283.CrossRef McDermott, M. L. (1997). Voting cues in low-information elections: Candidate gender as a social information variable in contemporary United States elections. American Journal of Political Science, 41(1), 270–283.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mendelberg, T. (2001). The race card: Campaign strategy, implicit messages, and the norm of equality. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Mendelberg, T. (2001). The race card: Campaign strategy, implicit messages, and the norm of equality. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Mendelberg, T. (2008a). Racial priming: Issues in research design and interpretation. Perspectives on Politics, 6(1), 135–140. Mendelberg, T. (2008a). Racial priming: Issues in research design and interpretation. Perspectives on Politics, 6(1), 135–140.
Zurück zum Zitat Mendelberg, T. (2008b). Racial priming revived. Perspectives on Politics, 6(1), 109–123. Mendelberg, T. (2008b). Racial priming revived. Perspectives on Politics, 6(1), 109–123.
Zurück zum Zitat Mihalec, J. (1984). Hair on the president’s chest. The Wall Street Journal, p. 30, 11 May. Mihalec, J. (1984). Hair on the president’s chest. The Wall Street Journal, p. 30, 11 May.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations: Politically knowledgeable citizens are guided by a trusted source. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 301–315.CrossRef Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations: Politically knowledgeable citizens are guided by a trusted source. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 301–315.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, D. T., Taylor, B., & Buck, M. L. (1991). Gender gaps: Who needs to be explained? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 5–12.CrossRef Miller, D. T., Taylor, B., & Buck, M. L. (1991). Gender gaps: Who needs to be explained? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 5–12.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mueller, C. M. (1988). The politics of the gender gap: The social construction of political influence. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Mueller, C. M. (1988). The politics of the gender gap: The social construction of political influence. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing of a civil liberties conflict and its effect on tolerance. American Political Science Review, 91(3), 567–583.CrossRef Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing of a civil liberties conflict and its effect on tolerance. American Political Science Review, 91(3), 567–583.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Orman, J. M. (1987). Comparing presidential behavior: Carter, Reagan, and the Macho presidential style. New York: Greenwood Press. Orman, J. M. (1987). Comparing presidential behavior: Carter, Reagan, and the Macho presidential style. New York: Greenwood Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Ortner, S. B. (1974). Is female to male as nature is to culture? In M. Z. Rosaldo & L. Lamphere (Eds.), Woman, culture, and society (pp. 67–88). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Ortner, S. B. (1974). Is female to male as nature is to culture? In M. Z. Rosaldo & L. Lamphere (Eds.), Woman, culture, and society (pp. 67–88). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Ortner, S. B. (1996). Making gender: The politics and erotics of culture. Boston: Beacon Press. Ortner, S. B. (1996). Making gender: The politics and erotics of culture. Boston: Beacon Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Petrocik, J. R. (1996). Issue ownership in presidential elections, with a 1980 case study. American Journal of Political Science, 40(3), 825–850.CrossRef Petrocik, J. R. (1996). Issue ownership in presidential elections, with a 1980 case study. American Journal of Political Science, 40(3), 825–850.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Petrocik, J. R., Benoit, W. L., & Hansen, G. J. (2003). Issue ownership and presidential campaigning, 1952–2000. Political Science Quarterly, 118(4), 599–626. Petrocik, J. R., Benoit, W. L., & Hansen, G. J. (2003). Issue ownership and presidential campaigning, 1952–2000. Political Science Quarterly, 118(4), 599–626.
Zurück zum Zitat Phillips, A. (1991). Engendering democracy. Cambridge: Polity Press. Phillips, A. (1991). Engendering democracy. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Phillips, W., & Boroditsky, L. (2003). Can quirks of grammar affect the way you think? Grammatical gender and object concepts. In Proceedings of the 25th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 928–933). Boston: Cognitive Science Society. Phillips, W., & Boroditsky, L. (2003). Can quirks of grammar affect the way you think? Grammatical gender and object concepts. In Proceedings of the 25th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 928–933). Boston: Cognitive Science Society.
Zurück zum Zitat Rahn, W. M. (1993). The role of partisan stereotypes in information processing about political candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37(2), 472–496.CrossRef Rahn, W. M. (1993). The role of partisan stereotypes in information processing about political candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37(2), 472–496.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rapoport, R. B., Metcalf, K. L., & Hartman, J. A. (1989). Candidate traits and voter inferences: An experimental study. The Journal of Politics, 51(4), 917–932.CrossRef Rapoport, R. B., Metcalf, K. L., & Hartman, J. A. (1989). Candidate traits and voter inferences: An experimental study. The Journal of Politics, 51(4), 917–932.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rich, F. (2004). How Kerry became a girlie-man. The New York Times, p. 1, 5 Sep. Rich, F. (2004). How Kerry became a girlie-man. The New York Times, p. 1, 5 Sep.
Zurück zum Zitat Ridgeway, C. L. (2001). Gender, status, and leadership. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 637–655.CrossRef Ridgeway, C. L. (2001). Gender, status, and leadership. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 637–655.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sagar, H. A., & Schofield, J. W. (1980). Racial and behavioral cues in black and white children’s perceptions of ambiguously aggressive acts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(4), 590–598.CrossRef Sagar, H. A., & Schofield, J. W. (1980). Racial and behavioral cues in black and white children’s perceptions of ambiguously aggressive acts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(4), 590–598.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sanbonmatsu, K. (2002). Democrats, republicans, and the politics of women’s place. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Sanbonmatsu, K. (2002). Democrats, republicans, and the politics of women’s place. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanbonmatsu, K., & Dolan, K. (2009). Do gender stereotypes transcend party? Political Research Quarterly, 62(3), 485–494.CrossRef Sanbonmatsu, K., & Dolan, K. (2009). Do gender stereotypes transcend party? Political Research Quarterly, 62(3), 485–494.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sanders, A. (1988). The meaning of party images. The Western Political Quarterly, 41(3), 583–599.CrossRef Sanders, A. (1988). The meaning of party images. The Western Political Quarterly, 41(3), 583–599.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sapiro, V. (2003). Theorizing gender in political psychology research. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp. 601–634). New York: Oxford University Press. Sapiro, V. (2003). Theorizing gender in political psychology research. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp. 601–634). New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Sapiro, V., & Conover, P. J. (1997). The variable gender basis of electoral politics: gender and context in the 1992 US election. British Journal of Political Science, 27(4), 497–523.CrossRef Sapiro, V., & Conover, P. J. (1997). The variable gender basis of electoral politics: gender and context in the 1992 US election. British Journal of Political Science, 27(4), 497–523.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sears, D. O., Sidanius, J., & Bobo, L. (Eds.). (2000). Racialized politics: The debate about racism in America. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Sears, D. O., Sidanius, J., & Bobo, L. (Eds.). (2000). Racialized politics: The debate about racism in America. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Sniderman, P. M., & Carmines, E. G. (1997). Reaching beyond race. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Sniderman, P. M., & Carmines, E. G. (1997). Reaching beyond race. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. (1995). Masculinity and femininity: Defining the undefinable. In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender, power, and communication in human relationships (pp. 105–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. (1995). Masculinity and femininity: Defining the undefinable. In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender, power, and communication in human relationships (pp. 105–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., & Helmreich, R. (1978). Masculinity & femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., & Helmreich, R. (1978). Masculinity & femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holahan, C. K. (1979). Negative and positive components of psychological masculinity and femininity and their relationships to self-reports of neurotic and acting out behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(10), 1673–1682.CrossRef Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holahan, C. K. (1979). Negative and positive components of psychological masculinity and femininity and their relationships to self-reports of neurotic and acting out behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(10), 1673–1682.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Spruill, M. J. (2008). Gender and America’s turn right. In B. J. Schulman & J. E. Zelizer (Eds.), Rightward bound: Making America conservative in the 1970s (pp. 71–89). Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Spruill, M. J. (2008). Gender and America’s turn right. In B. J. Schulman & J. E. Zelizer (Eds.), Rightward bound: Making America conservative in the 1970s (pp. 71–89). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Terkildsen, N., & Schnell, F. (1997). How media frames move public opinion: An analysis of the women’s movement. Political Research Quarterly, 50(4), 879–900. Terkildsen, N., & Schnell, F. (1997). How media frames move public opinion: An analysis of the women’s movement. Political Research Quarterly, 50(4), 879–900.
Zurück zum Zitat Trilling, R. J. (1976). Party image and electoral behavior. New York: Wiley. Trilling, R. J. (1976). Party image and electoral behavior. New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Valentino, N. A., Hutchings, V. L., & White, I. K. (2002). Cues that matter: How political ads prime racial attitudes during campaigns. American Political Science Review, 96(1), 75–90.CrossRef Valentino, N. A., Hutchings, V. L., & White, I. K. (2002). Cues that matter: How political ads prime racial attitudes during campaigns. American Political Science Review, 96(1), 75–90.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Winter, N. J. G. (2008). Dangerous frames: How ideas about race and gender shape public opinion. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press. Winter, N. J. G. (2008). Dangerous frames: How ideas about race and gender shape public opinion. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Wittenbrink, B. (2007). Measuring attitudes through priming. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 17–58). New York: Guilford Press. Wittenbrink, B. (2007). Measuring attitudes through priming. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 17–58). New York: Guilford Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolbrecht, C. (2000). The politics of women’s rights: Parties, positions, and change. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Wolbrecht, C. (2000). The politics of women’s rights: Parties, positions, and change. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Metadaten
Titel
Masculine Republicans and Feminine Democrats: Gender and Americans’ Explicit and Implicit Images of the Political Parties
verfasst von
Nicholas J. G. Winter
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2010
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 4/2010
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-010-9131-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2010

Political Behavior 4/2010 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner