Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Happiness Studies 2/2023

Open Access 07.12.2022 | Research Paper

Should We Cheer Together? Gender Differences in Instantaneous Well-being: An Application to COVID-19 Lockdowns

verfasst von: José Ignacio Giménez-Nadal, José Alberto Molina, Jorge Velilla

Erschienen in: Journal of Happiness Studies | Ausgabe 2/2023

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

The COVID-19 pandemic has confined millions in their homes, an unprecedented opportunity to spend more time together with family members. This paper explores subjective well-being in the uses of time for US and UK workers, differentiating between solo activities and activities done with family members, at home and outside the home. Using American and British time use surveys, we compute the instant utility associated with paid work, unpaid work, leisure, and childcare activities. OLS regressions on both men and women show that workers prefer joint leisure to solo leisure, and that significant differences exist for solo and joint market work and housework, between the sexes. Despite that, the effect magnitudes are relatively low. Furthermore, we simulate a strict lockdown situation by replacing where and with whom worker episodes would be, based on mid-2020 strict confinements. Results suggest diverging effects, since more time with the spouse/partner and children, and less time with others, seems to increase the experienced wellbeing of women, compared to that of men. The simulation exercise also reveals asymmetric effects in the US and in the UK. The conclusions of this paper may help in assessing the psychological consequences of COVID-19 lockdowns, beyond the negative economic and labour market consequences.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

1 Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic spread across the world, with serious consequences for daily life, including the confinement of individuals in their homes. This is not to trivialize the devastating death toll, nor the unprecedented damage to the global economy, but this confinement has clear implications for the time-allocation decisions of families, as many parents are forced to telework, and to take care of their children, with no in-person school classes (Boca et al., 2020; Sevilla & Smith, 2020). Thus, the time spent with spouses/partners, children, and other family members is certain to increase as a consequence of the confinement. This, in turn, has significant implications for individual well-being, as time spent with others is generally preferable to solitude (Sullivan, 1996a, b; Hallberg, 2003; Kahneman et al., 2004; Jenkins & Osberg, 2004; Sevilla et al., 2012; Cosaert et al., 2022), and loneliness may lead to lower well-being (Hamermesh, 2020).
Prior research has analyzed the link between time-allocation decisions and affective well-being (Kahneman et al., 2004; Kahneman & Krueger, 2006; Krueger, 2007; Sevilla et al., 2012; Gimenez-Nadal & Molina, 2015; Gimenez-Nadal et al., 2020).1 For instance, Kahneman et al., (2004) find that leisure activities are superior in terms of instant enjoyment to activities such as commuting, market work, or housework. Gimenez-Nadal & Molina (2015) find that voluntary activities produce enjoyment for the participants in those activities, with a spillover effect on other daily activities. The research in this field has also shown that spending time with others is preferable to solitude (Sullivan, 1996a; Kahneman et al., 2004; Helliwell & Putnam, 2005; Sevilla et al., 2012). For instance, Sullivan (1996a) finds that time spent in different activities is more enjoyable when spent in company, in comparison to being alone. Kahneman et al., (2004) find that activities done in the presence of friends, relatives, and the spouse and children are superior in terms of utility, compared to doing something alone. Sevilla et al., (2012) reach the same conclusions, although restricting their analysis to leisure activities.
If the COVID19 pandemic and its associated confinements and lockdowns are related to changes in the time devoted to activities and with whom those activities are done, it is important to examine the consequences of these confinements and lockdowns in terms of time-allocation decisions, togetherness, and well-being, to understand the impact of confinement on individual daily life, and on gender differences in well-being. While some studies have found that confinements and lockdowns have led many couples to regress towards more traditional gender roles (Boca et al., 2020; Sevilla & Smith, 2020), others have reported a more equal division of labor between men and women (Boll et al., 2021; Sevilla & Smith, 2020). Thus, gender differences in the changes in affective well-being may arise from COVID19 measures, since more leisure may represent increased affective well-being, more housework may do the opposite. Gender differences in the instant enjoyment obtained from similar activities have been reported, with women reporting higher affective well-being during leisure and housework activities (Krueger, 2007; Sevilla et al., 2012; Gershuny, 2013; Qian & Fan, 2019).
Within this framework, this paper analyzes the experienced utility (or instantaneous well-being, as in Kahneman et al., 2004) of workers in the United Kingdom and the United States, using the UK Time Use Survey (UKTUS) 2014–2015, and the American Time Use Survey (ATUS) 2010–2013, and focusing on the difference between activities done alone and those in the presence of other household members.2 We find that women benefit more from the presence of others in their daily activities, as the increase in experienced utility when the activity is done in the presence of others, in comparison to being alone, is greater for women than for men. This gender difference, in line with the findings of Qian & Fan (2019), is limited to market work and housework activities, since men and women both experience similar increases in their experienced utility when leisure happens in the presence of others.3
We also simulate a lockdown, by assuming changes in individual time allocations (e.g., less paid work time, and more leisure time), where such activities take place (at home), and with whom they are done (alone, with the spouse, with children, and/or other relatives). The simulation is based on a strict lockdown in which all activities are done at home, alone for individuals who live alone, or together with the respective cohabitants (e.g., a partner, children, and/or others) for individuals who cohabit with others. The changes in the experienced utility of individuals under the simulation, compared to the general setting, reveal asymmetric effects for the US and the UK, as the lockdown has different impacts in these countries, in addition to different effects for women and men. Overall, confinement has negative effects on experienced enjoyment among UK individuals, but positive effects for US individuals.
The gender and country analysis of experienced utility may help in understanding the possible consequences of confinement caused by the COVID-19 pandemic, beyond the negative consequences on both the economy and the labour market. Women appear more likely to do things alone than men, and to the extent that confinement may mean more time with family members, the confinement itself may imply greater increases in well-being for women. However, the gains from more time with family members may be outweighed by the fact that the COVID-19 outbreak has amplified the need for care work within the home, not only due to school closures, but also due to the large number of individuals contracting the virus and being quarantined. In a world where women do relatively more unpaid care work than do men (Eaton, 2005; Carmichael et al., 2008), this pandemic may have increased the demands on women’s time and thus increased the gender imbalance of housework (including care work) time (Aguiar & Hurst, 2007; Gimenez-Nadal & Sevilla, 2012).
Furthermore, when individuals are at home as a consequence of job losses, there is a gender asymmetry in how time allocations are redistributed; while women increase the time they devote to housework, men increase the time devoted to personal care and leisure (Beblo & Robledo, 2008; Aguiar et al., 2013; Berik and Kongar, 2013; Gimenez-Nadal & Molina 2014; Boca et al., 2020; Sevilla & Smith, 2020). The gender asymmetry in the gains in utility from more time with family members, favoring women, may compensate for the negative consequences of the extra workload for women.
Our contribution to the literature is twofold. We first contribute to the analysis of gender differences in experienced utility, with a focus on how the presence of others during the different activities is related to changes in the experienced utility of men and women. Prior research has analyzed this for a range of activities, finding a positive relationship (Sullivan, 1996a; Kahneman et al., 2004; Sevilla et al., 2012). However, this prior research has not explored gender differences in the positive relationship. We find that, in general terms, the increase in experienced utility when the activity is done in the presence of others, in comparison to being alone, is greater for women than for men. Second, we contribute to the literature by analyzing the effects of COVID19 confinements and lockdowns on individual well-being (Brand et al., 2020; Foa et al., 2020; Recchi et al., 2020; Brindal et al., 2021; Fujiwara et al., 2020; Long, 2021; Möhring et al., 2021; Ruiz et al., 2021; Zacher & Rudolph, 2021). The evidence presented in these studies is far from conclusive, and is based on cognitive, subjective well-being measures. We use an alternative approach, with a simulation and analysis of affective experienced utility while doing activities, as an instrument to evaluate possible gender differences in such effects.

2 Background

The analysis of the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic - and the confinements and lockdowns that followed - on individual subjective well-being has received special attention in recent months, generating a flourishing field of research. Some authors have conducted specific research based on pilot analyses or specific surveys to study how lockdowns have affected daily behaviors and well-being, although there is no consensus on the effects of confinements and lockdowns on subjective well-being.4 Recchi et al., (2020) reported increased well-being derived from lockdowns in France, and Foa et al., (2020) found that lockdowns reduced individual negative effects in the UK, thus increasing subjective well-being. Similarly, Long (2021) found increased subjective well-being of US individuals during the US lockdown, and Brand et al., (2020) found that physical exercise during lockdowns had a positive impact.
Other authors have reported decreased well-being during the COVID-19 lockdowns. Möhring et al., (2021) reported declines in family and work satisfaction in Germany during strict lockdowns, while Zacher & Rudolph (2021) found no differences in subjective well-being in the same country. Ruiz et al., (2021) found different channels through which COVID-19 affected well-being negatively, such as physical health in the UK, and emotional well-being in Latin American countries. In Australia, Brindal et al., (2021) found that lockdowns generated increased isolation, leading to decreases in well-being (although using a non-representative online survey). A recent report on the negative implications of COVID-19 on well-being, with a focus on the UK, is provided by Fujiwara et al., (2020). Hamermesh (2020) studied how loneliness and togetherness correlate with overall cognitive well-being (i.e., overall life satisfaction), finding that loneliness correlates with decreased satisfaction for both singles and couples, in general terms.
Prior research is based on respondents’ subjective well-being by asking for general satisfaction with life, or general happiness with life, which aims to capture the subjective cognitive evaluations (subjective well-being measures often referred to as Satisfaction With Life Scale, SWLS).5 Alternative approaches to measure the well-being of individuals exist, including measures aimed at capturing the affective component of subjective well-being (e.g., affective well-being); that is to say, the subjective evaluation of emotions experienced during one’s daily life (Watson et al., 1988). A popular tool to measure the affective component of well-being is the Positive Affect Negative Affect Scale (PANAS), which measures individuals’ experience of positive and negative affect. The measurement of affective well-being linked to time-allocation decisions is known as instantaneous well-being, or experienced utility (Kahneman et al., 2004).
There are several methodologies to assess the link between activities and instant feelings, including the use of Activity Enjoyment Ratings (Juster & Stafford, 1985; Gershuny & Halpin, 1996) and the Experience Sampling Method. Alternative methods of collecting affective data on hedonic experience, such as the conventional yesterday diary used in time-budget surveys (Szalai, 1972) and the Day Reconstruction Method (Kahneman et al., 2004), are less costly to implement. Both methods collect information on how the respondent experienced all or some of the activities he/she engaged in during the previous day, as described in a time-use diary. This approach has been used to measure national well-being (Krueger, 2009; Stiglitz, Sen and Fittousi, 2009; Gershuny 2013), and individual daily happiness (Kahneman et al., 2004; Kahneman & Krueger, 2006; Krueger, 2007; Knabe et al., 2010; Sevilla et al., 2012; Gimenez-Nadal & Molina, 2015; Hoang & Knabe, 2021).
Several authors have shown that daily activities are linked to the well-being of individuals, defined in terms of instant enjoyment, experienced utility, or positive/negative affect (Kahneman et al., 2004; Kahneman & Krueger, 2006; Krueger, 2007; Sevilla et al., 2012; Gimenez-Nadal & Molina, 2015; Gimenez-Nadal et al., 2020).6 For instance, Kahneman et al., (2004) find that leisure activities are superior in terms of instant enjoyment to activities such as commuting, market work, or housework. Gimenez-Nadal & Molina (2015) find that voluntary activities produce enjoyment for the participants, with a spillover effect on the rest of daily activities. Research in this field has also shown that spending time with others is preferable to solitude, as there is evidence from instant enjoyment data suggesting that individuals report higher levels of instant enjoyment from activities done in the company of others than those done alone (Helliwell & Putnam, 2005; Kahneman et al., 2004). For instance, Sullivan (1996a) analyzes time use in couples in the UK, finding that time spent in different activities is more enjoyable when it is spent in company. Kahneman et al., (2004), using data on experienced utility for a sample of 909 working women in the US, found that activities done in the presence of friends, relatives, and the spouse and children are superior in terms of utility, compared to acting alone. Sevilla et al., (2012) find that, for both the United Kingdom and the United States, the presence of young children is associated with greater happiness.
Given that confinements and lockdowns derived from the COVID19 pandemic may lead to changes in the allocation of time, and the presence of others during time use activities may have implications for individual well-being. Two contrasting hypotheses about the impact of the pandemic on gender divisions of unpaid work have emerged. The “re-traditionalization hypothesis” claims that the pandemic is a ‘patriarchal pandemic’ (Kreyenfeld & Zinn, 2021). More concretely, it is assumed that the pandemic and accompanying measures has led many couples to regress towards more traditional gender roles, whereby women, and in particular mothers, are more likely to shoulder additional housework and childcare responsibilities, almost exclusively (Boca et al., 2020; Sevilla & Smith, 2020). In contrast, the “equalizing hypothesis” claims that the COVID-19 crisis may have promoted a more equal division of labor between men and women in some families (Boll et al., 2021; Sevilla & Smith, 2020). This latter hypothesis is based on women’s over-representation in essential occupations, and hence their crucial role in society’s pandemic response. On the other hand, as the pandemic has forced many workers into short-time work and working from home, men and fathers may have had more time at their disposal or may have spent more time at home. In combination with a greater exposure to family life and the possible inability of the woman to take on the additional workload, men’s and in particular fathers’ involvement in childcare and household tasks may have increased (Yerkes et al., 2020; Sevilla & Smith, 2020; Carlson et al., 2021).
Despite that an increasing body of research has found changes in time allocation derived from confinements, the results are somewhat inconclusive. Some evidence points to increased engagement of mothers in unpaid work (Boca et al., 2020; Farré et al., 2020), although other studies show that men, and particularly fathers, have become more involved in unpaid work, thus narrowing the gender gap (Craig and Churchill, 2021; Carlson et al., 2021; Yaish et al., 2021). These diverse and sometimes contradictory results are related to a range of factors, such as the exact population under study, the country’s cultural context, and its pandemic response, as well as the type of data used. Thus, the possible effects of COVID-19 confinements on affective well-being, measured through instant enjoyment of activities, are not clear, nor are the differential effects depending on gender.

3 Data and Variables

We use diary data from the UK Time Use Survey (UKTUS), for the years 2014–2015, and the Subjective Well-being (SWB) module of the American Time Use Survey (ATUS) for the years 2010, 2012 and 2013.7 Apart from providing information on the socio-demographic characteristics of respondents, these surveys include time use diaries, with information on respondents’ activities during the 24h of the day, from 4 am to 4 am of the following day. Time use diaries have become a common tool in analyzing individual time allocation and daily behaviors, as they produce more reliable estimates than surveys based on stylized questionnaires. The UKTUS is the official time use survey of the UK and is sponsored by the Centre for Time Use Research, while the ATUS is the official time use survey of the US, and is conducted by the US Bureau of Labor Statistics. The ATUS Well-being modules were fielded from January through December in each year, and the 2015 UKTUS was fielded from April 2014 through March 2015.
The time use categories analyzed are based on Aguiar & Hurst (2007) and Gimenez-Nadal & Sevilla (2012), and we define paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure. Paid work includes those activities related to employment, excluding commuting. Unpaid work time is defined as those activities related to household chores and domestic activities (cooking, setting the table, washing, cleaning, adult care).8 Childcare time includes all activities related to the care of children, and includes basic, educational, and supervisory childcare. For leisure time, we consider activities such as watching TV, sports, out-of-home leisure, gardening, pet care, and socializing.
The UKTUS and the SWB module of the ATUS include information at the diary level on the feelings experienced by individuals during their daily episodes. Both surveys use the Day Reconstruction Method, in which respondents are asked to fill out a diary summarizing episodes of activities for the selected day, and then are asked about their feelings during the activities (Kahneman et al., 2004).9 In the ATUS SWB module, respondents are asked to rank three randomly selected episodes, lasting at least five minutes, describing the extent to which they were happy, stressed, sad, tired, or felt pain. Values are recorded on a 7-point scale, with “0” indicating that the respondent “did not experience the feeling at all”, and “6” indicating that a “feeling was extremely strong”. In the UKTUS, respondents answered the question “How much did you enjoy this time?”, with possible answers going from 1 “not at all” to 7 “very much”. Compared to the ATUS SWB module, the UKTUS collects instantaneous well-being information for all episodes in the diary. Although the type of questions used to elicit a respondent’s instantaneous well-being differs between the UK (enjoyment) and the US (happiness) surveys, research suggests that the two types of measure are highly correlated (Knabe et al., 2010).10 The type of well-being that can be measured with the ATUS Well-Being Module and the UK Time Use Survey refers to the instantaneous affective well-being, experienced utility, or instant feelings experienced by individuals throughout the day.
We have computed the coefficients for the happiness and enjoyment scales, to determine whether they are reliable and consistent concerning the respondents. We have estimated the \(\alpha\) and \(\omega\) reliability coefficients, which are about 0.746 and 0.738 for the UK, and 0.753 and 0.777 for the US. These measures are above the standard threshold for research purposes, often fixed at 0.7 (Cortina, 1993), and rank as “high” or “fairly high” (Taber, 2018), suggesting that they are consistent and the analysis is appropriate. We have also computed the average inter-item covariances, which are found to be 0.511 for the UK enjoyment scale, and 1.198 for the US happiness scale, along with the inter-class correlation between averages on the same individuals, which are 0.575 in the UK sample, and 0.774 in the US sample. In both cases, the F test-retest rejects that inter-item correlations are null at statistically significant levels (p < 0.001 for both samples).
To minimize the role of time-allocation decisions over the life cycle (Gimenez-Nadal & Sevilla, 2012), we restrict the samples to individuals between 21 and 65 years old, and we omit individuals who filled-in their diaries on holidays, and individuals not in paid work (as we are interested only in market work time). Given that we analyze information at the episode level, we restrict the sample to time-use episodes of respondents with non-missing information on instantaneous well-being, which leaves us with a sample of 8,612 episodes from 444 women, and 6,930 episodes from 392 men in the UK, and 13,744 episodes from 9,818 women and 12,473 episodes from 9,501 men in the US. We have additionally checked for outliers using the blocked adaptive computationally efficient outlier nominators (BACON) algorithm, to detect outliers in multivariate data on the main variables (Billor et al., 2000). The BACON algorithm detects no outliers in the UK and US samples, at statistically significant levels (p < 0.01).
Table 1 shows the average enjoyment and episode duration for the UK, and Table 2 shows the same for the US. Among men in the UK, the average enjoyment levels during paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure activities are 4.58, 4.88, 5.71, and 5.78, out of 7, respectively. For women, the equivalent enjoyment levels are 5.00, 4.77, 5.71, and 5.96. The differences between men and women are significant at standard levels in paid work episodes (p < 0.001), unpaid work (p = 0.017), and leisure (p < 0.001), with women reporting greater enjoyment while doing paid work and leisure, and lower enjoyment while doing unpaid work. Differences between women and men in terms of enjoyment during childcare are not significant. In the case of the US, the average happiness scores for men (women) in paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure activities are 3.92 (3.96), 4.10 (4.11), 4.94 (4.68), and 4.56 (4.74), on a scale of 6, respectively. Differences between men and women are not significant for the happiness experienced while doing both paid and unpaid work (p = 0.335 and p = 0.788, respectively), but men seem to be happier while doing childcare than are women, and women report greater experienced happiness during leisure episodes (p < 0.001 in both cases).
Table 1
Summary statistics – United Kingdom
 
WOMEN
MEN
DIFF.
Mean
S. Dev.
Mean
S. Dev.
P-value
PAID WORK
     
Episode duration
56.835
76.349
51.847
70.658
(0.054)
Enjoyment scale
4.999
1.223
4.584
1.453
(< 0.001)
Proportion of episodes with:
     
Spouse
1.532
12.288
4.258
20.196
(< 0.001)
Children
0.666
8.138
1.496
12.143
(0.024)
Other family members
4.930
21.657
6.789
25.164
(0.025)
Non-family members
66.089
47.356
58.343
49.313
(< 0.001)
Proportion of episodes at home
7.795
26.818
11.047
31.357
(0.002)
N. periods
1,501
1,738
 
UNPAID WORK
     
Episode duration
20.289
19.955
22.047
25.459
(0.009)
Enjoyment scale
4.767
1.583
4.879
1.461
(0.017)
Proportion of episodes with:
     
Spouse
28.651
45.221
35.740
47.938
(< 0.001)
Children
12.570
33.156
12.709
33.317
(0.891)
Other family members
17.657
38.136
15.453
36.157
(0.053)
Non-family members
9.288
29.031
7.757
26.757
(0.075)
Proportion of episodes at home
86.938
33.704
81.802
38.594
(< 0.001)
N. periods
3,446
3,191
 
CHILDCARE
     
Episode duration
18.964
14.523
21.754
19.571
(0.013)
Enjoyment scale
5.706
1.323
5.705
1.283
(0.992)
Proportion of episodes with:
     
Spouse
33.495
47.236
54.154
49.904
(< 0.001)
Children
71.197
45.321
79.385
40.517
(0.006)
Other family members
26.861
44.359
16.615
37.279
(< 0.001)
Non-family members
11.165
31.519
4.615
21.014
(< 0.001)
Proportion of episodes at home
88.835
31.519
86.769
33.935
(0.352)
N. periods
3,047
1,676
 
LEISURE
     
Episode duration
30.235
31.997
36.606
40.023
(< 0.001)
Enjoyment scale
5.955
1.298
5.781
1.270
(< 0.001)
Proportion of episodes with:
     
Spouse
38.218
48.599
45.534
49.808
(< 0.001)
Children
8.996
28.617
12.661
33.258
(< 0.001)
Other family members
17.092
37.650
16.233
36.881
(0.348)
Non-family members
19.037
39.265
16.797
37.390
(0.018)
Proportion of episodes at home
75.392
43.079
75.682
42.907
(0.784)
N. periods
618
325
 
Note: The sample (UKTUS 2014–2015) is restricted to paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. Time uses are measured in minutes per day. Enjoyment is measured on a 7-point scale, from 1 (“not at all”) to 7 (“very much”). T-test p-values for the differences between women and men in parentheses
Table 2
Summary statistics – United States
 
WOMEN
MEN
DIFF.
Mean
S. Dev.
Mean
S. Dev.
P-value
PAID WORK
     
Episode duration
213.657
148.561
224.903
163.069
(0.008)
Happiness scale
3.964
1.573
3.923
1.571
(0.335)
Proportion of episodes with:
     
Spouse
3.331
17.947
2.988
17.030
(0.469)
Children
3.331
17.947
1.832
13.411
(< 0.001)
Other family members
0.740
8.573
0.161
4.006
(0.001)
Non-family members
69.161
46.192
63.850
48.051
(< 0.001)
Proportion of episodes at home
16.530
37.152
16.131
36.788
(0.690)
N. periods
2,432
3,112
 
UNPAID WORK
     
Episode duration
51.149
64.063
54.030
70.795
(0.096)
Happiness scale
4.106
1.608
4.095
1.562
(0.788)
Proportion of episodes with:
     
Spouse
18.581
38.900
27.169
44.493
(< 0.001)
Children
23.860
42.628
16.865
37.452
(< 0.001)
Other family members
3.129
17.411
2.158
14.533
(0.024)
Non-family members
8.479
27.860
8.542
27.957
(0.930)
Proportion of episodes at home
94.698
22.410
93.307
24.996
(0.022)
N. periods
4,187
2,271
 
CHILDCARE
     
Episode duration
35.938
49.765
44.819
60.237
(< 0.001)
Happiness scale
4.676
1.408
4.937
1.227
(< 0.001)
Proportion of episodes with:
     
Spouse
19.235
39.426
39.035
48.804
(< 0.001)
Children
90.720
29.023
93.246
25.107
(0.016)
Other family members
3.881
19.319
2.105
14.362
(0.008)
Non-family members
9.168
28.865
5.877
23.530
(0.001)
Proportion of episodes at home
77.334
41.879
83.684
36.967
(< 0.001)
N. periods
1,778
1,140
 
LEISURE
     
Episode duration
53.103
68.181
53.937
69.566
(0.521)
Happiness scale
4.739
1.450
4.560
1.471
(< 0.001)
Proportion of episodes with:
     
Spouse
30.353
45.983
34.504
47.542
(< 0.001)
Children
28.951
45.358
24.084
42.763
(< 0.001)
Other family members
5.779
23.337
4.235
20.141
(< 0.001)
Non-family members
38.208
48.594
34.723
47.613
(< 0.001)
Proportion of episodes at home
57.079
49.501
58.555
49.267
(0.113)
N. periods
5,347
5,950
 
Note: The sample (ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) is restricted to paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. Time uses are measured in minutes per day. Happiness is measured on a 7-point scale, from 0 (“not at all”) to 6 (“very much”). T-test p-values for the differences between women and men in parentheses
The mean duration of episodes of paid work is about 57min for UK women, vs. 52min for UK men, with these being significant at the 90% level (p = 0.054). The average duration of unpaid work episodes is 20min for women, and 22min for men, with the difference being significant (p = 0.017). For childcare episodes, the average duration is 19min for women, and 22min for men, and the gender difference is significant at standard levels (p = 0.013). For leisure episodes, the average duration is 30 minutes for women, and 37 minutes for men, with the difference being highly significant (p < 0.001).
In the US, the average episode duration is 214 (225) minutes for women’s (men’s) paid work, 51 (54) minutes for unpaid work, 36 (45) minutes for childcare, and 53 (54) minutes for leisure. Differences between women and men in the duration of these episodes are statistically significant for the periods of paid work (p = 0.008), unpaid work (p = 0.096), and childcare (p < 0.001), but the average duration of leisure episodes is not statistically different for women and men (p = 0.521).
The UKTUS and ATUS surveys include information about who was present for all activities, distinguishing between solo activities, activities with the partner/spouse present, activities with children, activities with other family members, and activities with non-family individuals. We use this information to identify joint and solo time uses. Tables 1 and 2 show the percentage of episodes in the presence of someone else, for the 4 activities defined. In the UK, for women’s paid work episodes, 1.5% are done with the spouse, 0.7% with a child, 4.9% with other relatives, and 66.1% with others. For men, 4.3% of the episodes are done with the spouse, 1.5% with a child, 6.8% with other relatives, and 58.3% with others. In the case of unpaid work episodes of women (men), 28.7% (35.7%) are done with the spouse, 12.6% (12.7%) with a child, 17.7% (15.5%) with other relatives, and 9.3% (7.8%) with others. For childcare episodes, 33.5% (54.2%) are done with the spouse, 71.2% (79.4%) with the child, 26.9% (16.6%) with other relatives, and 11.2% (4.6%) with others. For episodes of leisure of women (men), 38.2% (45.5%) are done with the spouse, 9.0% (12.7%) with a child, 17.1% (16.2%) with other relatives, and 19.0% (16.8%) with others. For episodes of women (men) in the US, we observe that 3.3, 18.6, 19.2, and 30.4 (3.0, 27.2, 39.0 and 34.5) percent of episodes of paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure is done with the spouse, respectively. Similarly, 3.3, 23.9, 90.7, and 29.0 (1.8, 16.9, 93.2, and 24.1) percent are done with a child; 0.7, 3.1, 3.9, and 5.8 (0.2, 2.2, 2.1, and 4.2) percent with other relatives, and 69.2, 8.5, 9.2, and 38.2 (63.9, 8.5, 5.9 and 34.7) percent with others. T-type test p-values for the differences between women and men in these percentages are shown in Tables 1 and 2.
Both the UKTUS and the ATUS surveys allow us to identify where the reported activities take place, differentiating among several locations (e.g., respondent’s home, workplace, restaurant, someone else’s home, stores, school, or outdoors away from home, among others). We use this information to compute a dummy variable at the episode level, which takes value 1 if activities are done at the respondent’s home, and 0 otherwise. Therefore, the variable identifies those activities done at home, versus all the activities done elsewhere. Tables 1 and 2 show the proportion of paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure activities done at home, for male and female workers in the UK and the US, respectively.
In the UK, women (men) spend about 7.8 (11.0) percent of their paid work episodes at home, with the difference being statistically significant at standard levels. Conversely, women do more unpaid work at home than men, with 86.9% of women’s unpaid work episodes taking place at home, vs. 81.8% for men. The difference is statistically significant at standard levels. Regarding childcare, 88.8% of women’s episodes, and 86.8% of the men’s episodes are at home, with the gender difference being not significant at standard levels (p = 0.352). Similarly, about 75% of the leisure episodes are home-leisure, for both men and women, with a non-significant difference between them (p = 0.784). The distribution of home and not-at-home episodes in the US is different than in the UK. For instance, about 16% of the paid work episodes are at home for both men and women, with this difference not being statistically significant. Women do relatively more unpaid work at home than do men, as 94.7% of women’s unpaid work are at home, vs. 93.3% of men’s episodes, with this difference being significant (p = 0.022). Regarding childcare, 77.3 (83.7) percent of the women’s (men’s) episodes are at home, with the difference being highly significant. Finally, there are no gender differences in terms of the percent of leisure episodes done at home (57.1% for women, and 58.5% for men).
The UKTUS and ATUS allow us to examine additional control variables at the individual level, defined analogously for the UK and the US. These variables include: age, formal education, native status/being Hispanic, marital status (married or cohabiting or single), household composition (the number of family unit members, and the number of children), and employment status (identifying self-employed workers, and full-time employees). For education, we define three dummies in terms of the maximum level of formal education completed by individuals: primary, secondary, and university education. The surveys allow us to define dummies identifying geographical regions. UK regions include: “North East”, “North West & Merseyside”, “Yorkshire & Humberside”, “East midlands”, “West midlands”, “East of England”, “London”, “South East”, “South West”, “Wales”, “Scotland”, and “Northern Ireland”. US regions include: “Northeast”, “Midwest”, “South”, and “West”.
Summary statistics of the socio-demographic characteristics, and of the total time devoted to the various activities in the presence of others, are shown in Tables A1 and A2 in the Appendix. For the UK (US), men devote 230, 94, 18, and 298 (286, 52, 27, and 143) minutes to paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure, respectively, while women devote 192, 139, 26, and 235 (233, 103, 39, and 136) minutes to these activities. While men devote more time to paid work (p = 0.024 in the UK, p < 0.001 in the US), women devote more time to unpaid work (p < 0.001 in both cases) and childcare (p = 0.042 and p < 0.001).11 Regarding the time devoted to these activities in the presence of others, we observe that most of the time spent in paid work corresponds to paid work with others (i.e., coworkers). For instance, the average time per day working with others for women in the UK (US) is about 147 (181) minutes, vs. 154 (2001) minutes among men. Differences between women and men are not significant in the UK, but highly significant (p < 0.001) in the US. For unpaid work, in the UK (US) the average time spent by women doing unpaid work is 42 (19) minutes with the spouse, 15 (26) minutes with children, 25 (3) minutes with other relatives, and 14 (8) minutes with others. Among men, the corresponding averages are 35 (16) minutes, 13 (10) minutes, 15 (1) minutes, and 8 (5) minutes.
For childcare time, women spend about 9 (9) minutes per day doing childcare with the spouse, 7 (2) minutes with other relatives, and 3 (5) minutes with others, vs. 10 (12), 2 (1) and 1 (3) minutes per day among men. In both the UK and the US, a small proportion of the childcare time (about 8 and 3min for UK women and men, and 3 and 2min for their US counterparts) is not spent in the presence of children, as some childcare activities, such as transport related to childcare, are not necessarily done with children. Finally, for leisure time in the UK (US), women spend about 91 (45) minutes per day doing leisure activities with the spouse, 19 (43) with children, 40 (7) with other relatives, and 48 (71) with non-family unit members. Among men, the corresponding minutes per day are 135 (56), 33 (38), 44 (5) and 56 (68). All these differences (p-values for the significance of such differences are shown in Tables A1 and A2 in the Appendix) may indicate that daily behaviors in terms of togetherness and the preference for joint activities are different in the US and the UK.

4 Empirical Strategy

We aim to explore whether the experienced utility of workers in their daily paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure activities is affected by whether such activities are done alone, or together with others (i.e., joint activities). To do so, we estimate the following linear equation using Ordinary Least Squares:
$${WB}_{ij}={\alpha }_{0}+{\alpha }_{1}{J}_{ij}+{\alpha }_{2}{X}_{i}+{\alpha }_{3}{E}_{ij}+{\alpha }_{4}{T}_{i}+{\epsilon }_{ij}$$
(1)
where \({WB}_{ij}\) represents the experienced utility of individual “i” while doing activity “j (paid work, unpaid work, childcare, leisure). Equation(1) is estimated separately by gender, and \({J}_{ij}\) is a vector of dummy variables that identifies whether episode “j” for individual “i” is done with the (married or unmarried) partner, with children, with other household members, or with other non-household members (solo activities are considered the reference category).
\({X}_{i}\) is a vector of variables controlling for the socio-demographic characteristics of individual “i” (e.g., age, education, living in couple, number of children, etc.). \({E}_{ij}\) is a vector of episode-level controls that includes the duration of the episode, where the activity took place (e.g., at home vs. other locations), if the day is a weekday (vs. weekend), and the start time of the activity. \({T}_{i}\) is the total time devoted by individual “i” to the reference activity during the diary day (paid work, unpaid work, childcare, or leisure) measured in log of minutes per day. Finally, \({\epsilon }_{ij}\) represents the error terms, and is clustered at the individual level, to take into account the heterogeneity in time-allocation decisions, as well as inter-personal differences in scales (Ferrer-i-Carbonell and Frijters, 2004).
We run several robustness checks. First, we estimate Eq.(1) using an ordered logit model. Second, we exclude from the analysis episodes between midnight and 8 am, to avoid “strange hours” (Hamermesh & Stancanelli, 2015). Third, we estimate Eq.(1) for the whole sample (i.e., men and women) and including gender scores, that is to say, a dummy variable that takes value 1 for men (0 for women), and the interactions of this dummy and the regressors of interest (the variables in \({J}_{ij}\)). Fourth, we estimate Eq.(1) but including standard errors not clustered at the individual level, which allows us to compute effect magnitudes to analyze the relative variance captured by the main explanatory variables, relative to the model’s explained variance. Results indicate that the main conclusions are robust to the model, sample selection, and choice of standard errors, since the ordered logit, reduced sample, alternative gender score model, and standard errors results shown in Tables A3-A6 in the Appendix are robust to the main analysis. Furthermore, effect magnitudes are shown in Table A7 in the Appendix, indicating that model effects are relatively small. This is standard when estimating regression models with time use data, where information is subject to unobserved heterogeneity and stochastic components (Gimenez-Nadal et al., 2021).12
Given the number of explanatory variables included in the estimated models, we have systematically studied potential multicollinearity among explanatory variables, and among interactions of explanatory variables. In doing so, we use Variance Inflation Factors (VIF), and find overall VIF measures of 2.76 in the UK, and 3.44 in the US, when no interactions between variables are considered. When we do consider interactions, VIF measures are estimated to be 3.12 in the UK, and 4.24 in the US, both below the problematic threshold of 10, and the concern threshold of 5 (James et al., 2013). Furthermore, the only individual variables with VIF above these thresholds are education dummies in the US, as expected, given that these dummies are collinear. Thus, the estimated VIF measures suggest no potential multicollinearity issues in the empirical analysis.

5 Results

Tables 3 and 4 show the main estimates of Eq.(1) in the UK and the US, respectively. Regarding paid work, we observe that in the UK, and in comparison to being alone, women report higher levels of enjoyment during paid work if it is done along with children or with non-family members, while men report lower levels of enjoyment when paid work is done with children. In the case of the US, we find no happiness differences for women when paid work is done with others, in comparison with being alone, while men report higher levels of happiness during paid work when it is done with other family members. When challenged by lockdowns, such as the one caused by COVID-19, implying an increase in teleworking for many workers, and thus more time in paid work in company with the spouse/partner and the children, and less time in the presence of others (both family and non-family members), men in both the UK and the US would reduce their experienced utility during paid work time, while women would increase theirs. It is also interesting that women in the UK report higher levels of experienced utility when paid work is done in the presence of children, which may be a consequence of the “double burden”, or second shift, that they face - especially for those women who work full-time and have children (Gimenez-Nadal & Sevilla, 2011). If those women are able to take care of their children while they are working - which would obviously be the case in a ‘lockdown’ situation - the difficulties in balancing work and household responsibilities would be reduced, which would clearly improve women’s well-being. Furthermore, we find that working at home is not correlated with worker experienced enjoyment in a significant way in the UK, nor among US men. Estimates show that episodes of paid work at home for women workers are correlated with lower happiness, relative to episodes of paid work not at home, which may indicate that women may face higher risks of work interruptions during lockdowns, given the presence of children at home. This last finding is consistent with prior evidence showing that women get more frequent interruptions while teleworking (Adams-Prassl, 2020).
Table 3
Estimates for the United Kingdom
VARIABLES
PAID WORK
UNPAID WORK
CHILDCARE
LEISURE
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
WITH:
        
With the spouse
-0.730
0.211
0.346**
0.102
-0.176
-0.048
0.291**
0.290***
 
(0.463)
(0.418)
(0.133)
(0.142)
(0.161)
(0.217)
(0.091)
(0.085)
 
[0.117]
[0.613]
[0.010]
[0.472]
[0.275]
[0.825]
[0.001]
[0.001]
With children
0.994**
-3.457***
0.152
0.111
-
-
0.107
0.214
 
(0.319)
(0.730)
(0.171)
(0.206)
  
(0.121)
(0.156)
 
[0.002]
[0.000]
[0.377]
[0.591]
  
[0.380]
[0.171]
With others (family unit)
-0.149
0.230
0.419**
-0.044
0.155
0.150
0.038
-0.010
 
(0.244)
(0.438)
(0.128)
(0.157)
(0.216)
(0.282)
(0.148)
(0.108)
 
[0.541]
[0.600]
[0.001]
[0.779]
[0.475]
[0.595]
[0.799]
[0.925]
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.358*
0.029
0.619***
0.389*
0.470*
0.490
0.217*
0.296**
 
(0.142)
(0.158)
(0.144)
(0.185)
(0.196)
(0.356)
(0.091)
(0.095)
 
[0.012]
[0.857]
[0.000]
[0.036]
[0.018]
[0.173]
[0.018]
[0.002]
EPISODE CONTROLS:
        
Where: at home
-0.182
0.066
-0.254
0.030
0.545
0.163
-0.268**
-0.158
 
(0.288)
(0.322)
(0.146)
(0.134)
(0.313)
(0.307)
(0.089)
(0.089)
 
[0.528]
[0.837]
[0.083]
[0.823]
[0.084]
[0.596]
[0.003]
[0.078]
Start time 0 = 4am
0.001*
0.000
0.000**
0.000*
0.001
0.001**
0.000***
0.000**
 
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
 
[0.018]
[0.499]
[0.002]
[0.022]
[0.064]
[0.005]
[0.001]
[0.001]
Episode duration
-0.002**
-0.002**
-0.001
-0.001
0.015**
0.006
0.002
0.003***
 
(0.001)
(0.001)
(0.002)
(0.002)
(0.005)
(0.003)
(0.001)
(0.001)
 
[0.006]
[0.006]
[0.488]
[0.518]
[0.002]
[0.068]
[0.084]
[0.000]
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
1,501
1,738
3,047
1,676
618
325
3,446
3,191
R-squared
0.099
0.117
0.090
0.056
0.207
0.288
0.065
0.081
Adjusted R-squared
0.087
0.106
0.084
0.044
0.182
0.241
0.060
0.075
F test p-value
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
AIC
4919.2
6037.9
11196.9
5865.5
2049.7
1019.8
11741.2
10423.6
BIC
5030.8
6158.0
11329.4
5984.8
2138.2
1099.3
11876.4
10557.1
Note: The sample (UKTUS 2014–2015) is restricted to paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. The dependent variable is the subjective enjoyment of episodes, which takes values from 1 (“not at all”) to 7 (“very much”). Robust standard errors, clustered at the individual level, in parentheses. T-test p-values in brackets. Additional coefficients shown in Table A8 in the Appendix. *** Significant at the 99.9% level; ** significant at the 99% level; * significant at the 95% level
Table 4
Estimates for the United States
VARIABLES
PAID WORK
UNPAID WORK
CHILDCARE
LEISURE
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
WITH:
        
With the spouse
0.195
-0.417
0.267*
0.422***
0.184
0.324**
0.113
0.378***
 
(0.251)
(0.447)
(0.108)
(0.123)
(0.112)
(0.099)
(0.114)
(0.083)
 
[0.436]
[0.351]
[0.013]
[0.001]
[0.101]
[0.001]
[0.322]
[0.000]
With children
0.360
-0.006
0.338**
0.048
-
-
0.373***
0.361***
 
(0.193)
(0.228)
(0.113)
(0.144)
  
(0.080)
(0.076)
 
[0.062]
[0.978]
[0.003]
[0.738]
  
[0.000]
[0.000]
With others (family unit)
0.615
1.748***
0.168
0.120
0.555**
-0.541
0.333
0.277
 
(0.454)
(0.319)
(0.218)
(0.352)
(0.181)
(0.451)
(0.179)
(0.163)
 
[0.176]
[0.000]
[0.441]
[0.733]
[0.002]
[0.231]
[0.063]
[0.088]
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.144
0.102
0.553***
0.511*
0.258
0.252
0.343***
0.170
 
(0.111)
(0.106)
(0.133)
(0.201)
(0.189)
(0.219)
(0.086)
(0.091)
 
[0.194]
[0.337]
[0.000]
[0.011]
[0.172]
[0.249]
[0.000]
[0.062]
EPISODE CONTROLS:
        
Where: at home
-0.464**
0.185
-0.005
0.127
-0.041
0.167
-0.051
-0.101
 
(0.157)
(0.148)
(0.287)
(0.201)
(0.140)
(0.185)
(0.076)
(0.086)
 
[0.003]
[0.211]
[0.986]
[0.528]
[0.769]
[0.368]
[0.498]
[0.243]
Start time 0 = 4am
-0.001***
-0.000
-0.000***
-0.000
0.000
-0.000
0.000
0.000
 
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.000)
 
[0.000]
[0.102]
[0.001]
[0.533]
[0.069]
[0.836]
[0.186]
[0.054]
Episode duration
-0.001
-0.001
0.001
-0.001
0.001**
0.000
0.000
0.000
 
(0.000)
(0.000)
(0.001)
(0.001)
(0.000)
(0.001)
(0.000)
(0.000)
 
[0.070]
[0.074]
[0.385]
[0.263]
[0.002]
[0.413]
[0.476]
[0.323]
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
2,432
3,112
4,187
2,271
1,778
1,140
5,347
5,950
R-squared
0.086
0.029
0.056
0.076
0.083
0.084
0.063
0.067
Adjusted R-squared
0.078
0.022
0.052
0.068
0.073
0.068
0.060
0.064
F test p-value
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
AIC
8785.3
11528.1
15643.2
8300.4
6049.7
3551.1
18355.4
20644.3
BIC
8912.8
11661.0
15782.7
8426.4
6164.8
3656.9
18500.3
20791.5
Note: The sample (ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) is restricted to paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. The dependent variable is the happiness level of episodes, which takes values from 0 (“not at all”) to 6 (“very much”). Robust standard errors, clustered at the individual level, in parentheses. T-test p-values in brackets. Additional coefficients shown in Table A9 in the Appendix. *** Significant at the 99.9% level; ** significant at the 99% level; * significant at the 95% level
Regarding the time devoted to unpaid work, women in the UK and the US report higher enjoyment/happiness during these activities if they are done with the spouse, or in the presence of children or non-family members, in comparison to being alone. The consequences of confinement on the experienced utility of women while doing unpaid work, with more time with the spouse and children and less time with non-family members, is a priori undetermined. Men in the UK report higher levels of enjoyment during unpaid work when the activity is done with non-family members, while in the US men report higher levels of happiness when the activity is done with the spouse/partner, or with non-family members, in comparison with doing the activity alone. Regarding the location of housework activities, we observe a non-statistically significant correlation between doing unpaid work at home and experienced enjoyment or happiness of workers. These results imply that during the confinement of lockdown, men in the UK obtain lower experienced utility than in normal circumstances, given that they spend less time with non-family members. In the case of men in the US, the final effect is undetermined.
In the case of childcare time, women in the UK report higher enjoyment when the activity is done with non-family members, while women in the US report higher levels of happiness when the activity is done with other family members, in comparison to being alone. Given the decrease in the time spent in childcare with other family members, and with non-family members, women have lower experienced utility during the lockdown. In the case of men in the UK, we find no differences between the time devoted to childcare with others or alone, while men in the US report higher levels of happiness when the childcare is done with the spouse. The latter finding indicates that men are better off in terms of experienced utility during childcare activities, given the increase in the presence of the spouse during childcare activities. Furthermore, there are no differences in the experienced enjoyment depending on whether childcare activities are done at home or elsewhere, as the associated coefficients are not statistically significant at standard levels.
Finally, regarding the time devoted to leisure, we find that both men and women in the UK report higher levels of enjoyment when the activity is done with the spouse, or in the presence of non-family members. Conversely, in the US men, but not women, report higher levels of happiness if leisure activities are done in the presence of the spouse, whereas women, but not men, reporter higher levels of happiness if the activity is done in the presence of non-family members. Both men and women in the US also report increased happiness if leisure activities are done with children. Given that leisure time with family members increases during the Covid-19 lockdown, but leisure time with non-family members decreases, the consequences for the experienced utility of individuals are undetermined. Furthermore, women in the UK report enjoying their leisure episodes less when they took place at home, therefore contributing to the undetermined result. In the US, the coefficients identifying episodes at home are not significant for women or for men.
It is important to note that the estimated models have low \({R}^{2}\) statistics, indicating that they explain relatively low rates of variance. However, this is standard when analyzing data from Time Use Surveys (Ferrer-i-Carbonell and Frijters, 2004) ; Gimenez-Nadal et al., 2021). Furthermore, we have analyzed which models should be preferred among the baseline estimates and the set of proposed robustness checks. Using the \({R}^{2}\), and the adjusted-\({R}^{2}\), estimates suggest that all models perform similarly, with slight changes in these statistics. We similarly estimated information criteria measures to study competition across models, and computed the AIC and BIC measures. These statistics suggest that ordered logit models slightly outperform OLS estimates, although we rely on the baseline OLS estimates, for the sake of simplicity of interpretation, and given that conclusions are robust. On the other hand, AIC and BIC measures also suggest that the estimates in which both men and women are pooled in the same sample are relatively worse than the baseline estimates, and that estimates on the reduced sample are slightly preferable. Nevertheless, as these results are based on different samples, the comparisons should be considered cautiously.

6 A Lockdown Simulation

Hamermesh (2020) simulates a lockdown and estimates its impact on life satisfaction, using data from the ATUS. The simulation is based on specific changes in time use of specific respondents, namely married individuals with no children, and singles aged 30 or older with no children. The author redefines all the “with whom” variables for married individuals to include the partner, and the same variable to time alone for singles. He also assumes that one third of the paid work time is lost, that such time is assigned to leisure activities, and that income is also cut by one third. He then computes OLS estimates under these changes, and predicts a simulated life satisfaction ladder (i.e., a subjective measure of cognitive well-being), which is predicted imposing the latter changes. Hamermesh (2020) then compares the predicted life satisfaction ladder under the simulated lockdown conditions, and the reported life satisfaction ladder. These assumptions translate into an increase in married couples’ general life satisfaction, but a decrease in that of singles.
In our analysis, we follow a comparable simulation. However, as Hamermesh (2020) carries out the analysis on a life satisfaction ladder, and not on affective measures of experienced utility, some differences emerge. First, we estimate Eq.(1) on real data, from where we get the effect of the presence of others, and/or being at home, on the experienced wellbeing during different activities (paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure). These are the results shown in Tables 3 and 4, for the UK and the US, respectively. We then simulate a strict lockdown, and in doing so we impose several changes on respondents’ diaries.
First, we assume that all the time categories remain unchanged. That is to say, all the paid work activity episodes remain as paid work episodes, all the unpaid work episodes remain as unpaid work, all the childcare episodes remain as childcare, and all the leisure episodes remain as leisure. However, we do make changes in the variables reflecting with whom the activities are done (alone, with partners, with children, with other relatives, with others), and in the location of episodes, for all the episodes of the samples. All the episodes are computed as if they took place at the respondent’s home, generating a simulated variable for episode location indicating that all episodes take place at homeFor married individuals without children, and not cohabiting with others, all the episodes are redefined as “with the partner”. For married individuals without children, but cohabiting with others, all the episodes are redefined as “with the partner” and “with others (family unit)”. For married individuals with children, and not cohabiting with others, all the episodes are redefined as “with the partner” and “with children”. For married individuals with children, and cohabiting with others, all the episodes are redefined as “with the partner”, “with children”, and “with others (family unit)”. For singles who live alone, all episodes are redefined as “alone”. For singles who live with children, and not cohabiting with others, all episodes are redefined as “with children”. For singles without children, but cohabiting with other relatives, all episodes are recoded as “with others (family unit)”. Finally, for single individuals with children and who cohabit with relatives other than the partner and children, all the episodes are redefined as “with children”, and “with other relatives”.
Once the synthetic sample of episodes is created, we predict the enjoyment/happiness arising from these episodes, but using the coefficients that were initially obtained from Eq.(1) regarding who else is present, being at home, and socio-demographic characteristics.13 Table 5 shows the average reported enjoyment/happiness of respondents in their daily activities (i.e., the real episodes), the average computed enjoyment/happiness under the simulated lockdown (i.e., the synthetic episodes), along with the differences in average enjoyment/happiness during the time devoted to paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure for the synthetic episodes, and for the real episodes. Positive values indicate increases in experienced enjoyment/happiness induced by the simulated lockdown, while negative values indicate decreases. We also report t-type test p-values for the statistical significance of the estimated differences.
Table 5
Lockdown simulation
  
PAID WORK
UNPAID WORK
CHILDCARE
LEISURE
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
UNITED KINGDOM
         
Married/cohabiting
         
With children
Reported
4.691
4.517
4.478
4.732
5.706
5.705
5.867
5.724
 
Simulated
4.621
1.546
4.738
4.874
5.551
5.695
6.003
5.857
 
Difference
-0.070
-2.971***
0.260***
0.142*
-0.151**
-0.011
0.136***
0.133***
 
p-value
(0.268)
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
(0.013)
(0.006)
(0.873)
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
Without children
Reported
5.140
4.401
4.942
4.912
-
-
5.914
5.718
 
Simulated
3.790
4.739
5.053
4.957
-
-
5.896
5.770
 
Difference
-1.350***
0.338***
0.111*
0.044
-
-
-0.019
0.052
 
p-value
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
(0.021)
(0.446)
  
(0.645)
(0.161)
Singles
         
With children
Reported
5.145
5.446
4.716
4.989
5.659
6.500
6.044
5.976
 
Simulated
5.514
1.887
4.876
5.003
5.754
6.178
6.101
5.900
 
Difference
0.370***
-3.559***
0.161
0.015
0.095
-0.321
0.057
-0.077
 
p-value
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
(0.053)
(0.925)
(0.459)
(0.519)
(0.289)
(0.345)
Without children
Reported
5.084
4.826
5.114
5.068
-
-
6.083
5.968
 
Simulated
4.599
4.998
5.125
5.059
-
-
6.023
5.846
 
Difference
-0.485***
0.172*
0.011
-0.014
-
-
-0.060
-0.122**
 
p-value
(< 0.001)
(0.042)
(0.875)
(0.856)
  
(0.141)
(0.006)
UNITED STATES
         
Married/cohabiting
         
With children
Reported
4.032
3.897
4.114
4.199
4.651
4.930
4.856
4.680
 
Simulated
4.290
3.956
4.492
4.461
4.897
5.228
5.076
5.121
 
Difference
0.258***
0.059
0.378***
0.262***
0.246***
0.297***
0.221***
0.441***
 
p-value
(< 0.001)
(0.205)
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
Without children
Reported
4.022
4.097
4.301
4.150
-
-
4.812
4.622
 
Simulated
3.837
4.192
4.363
4.402
-
-
4.819
4.862
 
Difference
-0.185*
0.095
0.061
0.252**
-
-
0.007
0.240***
 
p-value
(0.014)
(0.208)
(0.278)
(0.003)
  
(0.880)
(< 0.001)
Singles
         
With children
Reported
4.074
3.788
4.089
4.192
4.737
4.984
4.708
4.615
 
Simulated
4.194
5.297
4.351
4.219
5.017
4.801
5.095
4.932
 
Difference
0.120
1.509***
0.262***
0.027
0.281***
-0.183
0.387***
0.308***
 
p-value
(0.106)
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
(0.795)
(< 0.001)
(0.119)
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
Without children
Reported
3.802
3.890
3.949
3.918
-
-
4.595
4.359
 
Simulated
3.541
4.685
4.107
3.982
-
-
4.725
4.479
 
Difference
-0.262***
0.795***
0.158**
0.064
-
-
0.130***
0.120***
 
p-value
(< 0.001)
(< 0.001)
(0.004)
(0.268)
  
(< 0.001)
(0.001)
Note: The samples (UKTUS 2014–2015; ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) are restricted to paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. Differences computed between the experienced enjoyment/happiness predicted when simulating a lockdown, minus the experienced enjoyment/happiness reported by respondents. Positive values indicate estimated increases in enjoyment while in a simulated lockdown. Lockdown simulation includes: (1) For individuals cohabiting, all the time is assigned to: “with the spouse”. (2) For single individuals, all the time is assigned as: “alone”. (3) For individuals with children, all the time is assigned to: “with children”. (4) All the time is assigned to: “where: at home”. (5) For working individuals, we compute a cut in paid work of 1/3 of the time, which is assigned to leisure (Hamermesh, 2020). T-test p-values in brackets. *** Significant at the 99.9% level; ** significant at the 99% level; * significant at the 95% level
The results for the UK (Panel A) show that, for married individuals with children, the simulated lockdown has a negative impact on the instant enjoyment associated with paid work activities, though it is only statistically significant among men. On the other hand, the lockdown has a positive impact on the instant enjoyment associated with unpaid work, for both women and men. Childcare (leisure) episodes of married women, however, are less (more) enjoyable. For married individuals with children, the lockdown has an opposite effect on women and men, as the former seem to enjoy less their paid work episodes, while the latter enjoy them more. Among single workers with children, the simulation shows that a lockdown has a positive impact on women’s enjoyment during paid work episodes, but a negative impact on that of men. The results for single workers without children are the opposite. Finally, the leisure episodes of single men without children seem to be less enjoyable during the simulated lockdown.
Panel B shows the results for the US. The simulated lockdown has a statistically significant impact on married women’s experienced happiness while doing paid work, but not on married men. However, this impact depends on the presence of children, as it is positive for married women with children, but negative for married women without children. The simulated confinement also has a positive impact on the level of happiness while doing unpaid work and childcare activities of married women and men with children, and on unpaid work episodes of married men (but not women) without children. Leisure episodes also seem to be more enjoyable for couples with children, and for married men without children. For singles, the confinement has a positive impact on single men’s happiness while doing paid work, but a negative impact on single women without children. On the other hand, single women’s happiness while doing unpaid work increases during the simulated lockdown, while that of men seems to be unaffected. Single women with children also enjoy more their childcare episodes during the simulation, while the happiness while doing leisure increases for both single women and men, with and without children.
Table 5 shows that the simulated lockdown has a heterogeneous impact on individual experienced utility, which depends not only on the respondents’ gender and household composition, but also on the country analyzed. Overall, taking together all the episodes and activities, the reported enjoyment of women (men) in the UK was 5.33 (5.26) out of 7, and the reported happiness in the US was 4.38 (4.29), out of 6. The simulated lockdown leads to predicted enjoyment of 5.27 for women and 4.96 for men in the UK, and predicted happiness values of 4.56 for US women and 4.69 for US men. These figures indicate a decrease in experienced utility in the UK, and an increase in the experienced utility of US individuals (with the differences being statistically significant at standard levels).
These heterogeneous affects are interesting, across both genders and countries. We note that in the UK men with children would have lower levels of enjoyment in comparison to a normal situation, while those without children would report higher levels. This may indicate that fathers may have needed to increase their care responsibilities under lockdown and school closures (Sevilla & Smith, 2020) and thus their paid work activities may have been more intertwined with childcare responsibilities (more interruptions), leading to lower levels of well-being. But, in the absence of children, telework would have allowed them to be better off than in normal circumstances (Gimenez-Nadal et al., 2021). On the other hand, women in the UK would report lower levels of enjoyment during paid work if they did not have children, which may be a consequence of the higher proportion of women working in essential occupations and thus having to go to their workplaces (explaining the increased childcare time of fathers).
But in the case of the US, single men with and without children would report higher levels of enjoyment while working during lockdowns, which contrasts with the results in the UK. This may indicate that this group may prefer isolation to social contact in a normal situation. Furthermore, women without children would report lower levels of enjoyment, while married women with children would report higher levels of enjoyment while working, in comparison to a normal situation. This may indicate that women may be taking most responsibilities for childcare during lockdowns and school closures, and thus working from home may allow them to better balance their work and household responsibilities.
Such cross-country differences in the results of the simulation may be a reflection that both countries have different cultures or roles regarding who cares for the children. For instance, Fisher and Robinson (2011) compare the time devoted to different activities, by individuals between the ages of 18 and 64 in twenty three countries, including the UK and the US, and observe that the gender gap in childcare time (i.e., women devote more time than men) is one hour more for the US, which may indicate that gender roles regarding childcare time are more traditional in the US. Other factors explaining cross-country differences could be related to the occupational composition of the countries, since if one of the countries has a higher proportion of jobs that can be done via telework, the negative consequences of lockdown would be different. Other factors could be used to explain differences in cross-country in lockdowns (e.g., what occupations are considered essential, duration of lockdowns, restrictions applied to the population). Despite that we offer three factors that could be explored in order to explain cross-country differences in the results of simulations, there are many others that we omit. Future research should focus on exploring these different factors.

7 Conclusion

This paper analyzes the potential differences in the experienced wellbeing of women and men in their daily activities, with a focus on the presence of others and being at home, while doing paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure. We use time use diary data from the UKTUS for the years 2014–2015, and the ATUS SWB module for the years 2010, 2012, and 2013, which provide information on the experienced utility associated with a range of episodes. Our results reveal gender differences in the experienced utility of individuals during paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure activities. Furthermore, our simulation exercise shows that, in a lockdown situation, the experienced wellbeing of women may increase compared to that of men, given that more time is spent with the spouse/partner and children and less time with other family members and non-family members. This suggests that confinements, such as those generated by the COVID-19 pandemic, may have a differential impact on men and women, as men seem to be less sensitive to whether their daily activities are done alone, or not.
We report cross-country differences in these relationships, as estimated differences between joint and solo activities differ for men and women in the UK and in the US. Experienced utility in the UK seems to be more sensitive to ‘togetherness’ than in the US, suggesting the existence of heterogeneity between countries, as both men and women appear to enjoy the presence of their partner in certain ways, especially while engaged in leisure activities in the UK, while results in the US do not support this conclusion. Hence, the impact of confinement on daily behaviors may affect women and men differentially, depending on the context and the country analyzed. Our simulation exercise supports this argument, since it reveals asymmetric effects on individual experienced utility for the US and the UK, as the lockdown has a different impact in those countries, in addition to different effects for women and men. This suggests that the same measures aimed at improving individual welfare during a pandemic may have different effects in different economies, countries, and regions, and the measures policy-makers contemplate should be studied individually.
Our conclusions must be taken with caution, given that we are assuming that the time devoted to these four activities does not change with confinement. The relative gains in experienced utility of women, arising from spending more time with family members, may be countered by the fact that the COVID-19 outbreak has exacerbated the need for care work within the home, due to school closures and to the significant numbers of people contracting the virus and requiring care at home. The gender asymmetry, favoring women, in the gains in utility from more time spent with family members may compensate for the negative consequences of the extra workload for women in these difficult times. This limitation is closely related to the fact that we do not have reliable time use data on a confinement situation, including affective subjective well-being information. We need information not collected during a lockdown, and then results should be interpreted cautiously. It is important to note that the ATUS has recently launched the 2020 wave of the survey, including information on time use during the period between mid-May and the 31st December; however, this data does not contain information on subjective well-being, and given the impact of the COVID-19 on the sampling method, annual estimates and comparisons are not recommended.14
Other limitations include that the data is cross-sectional and, as a consequence, the results cannot be interpreted as causal; we can only report conditional correlations. Furthermore, the analysis focuses on “daily” experienced utility, which is a measure of instantaneous well-being. As such, the consequences are not applicable to the long run and will likely vanish as the lockdown is lifted (Hamermesh, 2020). However, it would be interesting to analyze whether these differences in experienced utility have any long-term consequences in, for example, an analysis of stress, depression, or mental health problems after the lockdown. Finally, the analysis is restricted to the UK and the US because of data availability, and thus results cannot be generalized to the general population in other economies, especially those not in the Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic (WEIRD) world.
.

Acknowledgements

This paper has benefitted from funding from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (Project “PID2019:108348RA-I00”), and the Santander Bank (Program “COVID19 Santander”).

Declarations

Declarations of Interest

None.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Appendix

Table A1
Additional summary statistics – United Kingdom
 
WOMEN
MEN
DIFF.
Mean
S. Dev.
Mean
S. Dev.
P-value
TIME USE VARIABLES
     
Paid work time
192.140
230.774
229.872
251.244
(0.024)
Paid work with:
     
Spouse
2.387
22.166
7.577
42.178
(0.024)
Children
0.676
13.770
2.066
26.347
(0.331)
Other family members
12.590
64.278
12.245
68.898
(0.940)
Non-family members
146.734
210.845
154.235
217.051
(0.613)
Unpaid work time
139.234
113.499
94.260
94.072
(< 0.001)
Unpaid work with:
     
Spouse
41.734
71.121
35.102
61.855
(0.153)
Children
15.315
47.839
12.602
41.498
(0.384)
Other family members
24.685
60.290
14.668
43.178
(0.007)
Non-family members
14.257
40.999
7.959
29.780
(0.012)
Childcare time
26.396
66.132
18.036
50.012
(0.042)
Childcare with:
     
Spouse
8.559
27.902
10.077
29.880
(0.448)
Children
17.950
55.620
15.128
46.685
(0.430)
Other family members
7.095
25.495
2.296
12.401
(0.000)
Non-family members
3.288
17.209
0.765
6.742
(0.007)
Leisure time
234.662
151.649
297.985
185.762
(< 0.001)
Leisure with:
     
Spouse
91.622
124.064
135.408
148.939
(< 0.001)
Children
19.414
55.984
32.730
88.271
(0.009)
Other family members
40.428
75.578
44.158
98.590
(0.537)
Non-family members
48.423
95.467
55.536
103.853
(0.303)
SOCIO-DEMOGRAPHICS
     
Age
39.777
12.239
41.916
12.503
(0.013)
Educ.: Primary
0.018
0.133
0.043
0.204
(0.032)
Educ.: Secondary
0.345
0.476
0.306
0.461
(0.237)
Educ.: University
0.637
0.481
0.651
0.477
(0.693)
Being native
0.919
0.273
0.921
0.270
(0.916)
Living in couple
0.628
0.484
0.747
0.435
(< 0.001)
Family size
2.899
1.272
3.013
1.288
(0.199)
Number of children
0.718
0.936
0.740
0.977
(0.748)
Full time worker
0.671
0.470
0.893
0.310
(< 0.001)
Self-employed worker
0.007
0.082
0.023
0.150
(0.049)
N. individuals
444
392
 
Note: The sample (UKTUS 2014–2015) is restricted to individuals with episodes of paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure, between 21 and 65 years old. Time uses are measured in minutes per day
Table A2
Additional summary statistics – United States
 
WOMEN
MEN
DIFF.
Mean
S. Dev.
Mean
S. Dev.
P-value
TIME USE VARIABLES
     
Paid work time
233.451
249.346
285.751
273.925
(< 0.001)
Paid work with:
     
Spouse
4.638
36.577
4.878
39.757
(0.662)
Children
4.579
38.140
2.919
30.943
(< 0.001)
Other family members
0.695
16.721
0.658
16.825
(0.878)
Non-family members
180.890
236.983
200.943
257.894
(< 0.001)
Unpaid work time
103.350
119.857
52.464
88.297
(< 0.001)
Unpaid work with:
     
Spouse
19.036
53.733
15.596
48.671
(< 0.001)
Children
25.802
64.845
10.080
36.835
(< 0.001)
Other family members
2.973
21.995
1.164
12.580
(< 0.001)
Non-family members
8.317
35.294
5.056
27.640
(< 0.001)
Childcare time
39.052
81.304
26.977
69.843
(< 0.001)
Childcare with:
     
Spouse
9.428
37.573
11.584
42.722
(0.002)
Children
35.631
78.359
25.027
67.166
(< 0.001)
Other family members
1.526
15.058
0.705
12.060
(< 0.001)
Non-family members
4.666
26.348
2.515
20.119
(< 0.001)
Leisure time
135.991
124.863
143.252
132.572
(< 0.001)
Leisure with:
     
Spouse
45.406
88.687
56.478
96.607
(< 0.001)
Children
43.193
83.772
38.344
80.154
(< 0.001)
Other family members
7.412
38.501
5.397
32.902
(< 0.001)
Non-family members
71.328
114.510
67.524
116.094
(0.022)
SOCIO-DEMOGRAPHICS
     
Age
41.811
12.031
41.862
11.770
(0.762)
Educ.: Primary
0.011
0.105
0.017
0.129
(< 0.001)
Educ.: Secondary
0.417
0.493
0.400
0.490
(0.098)
Educ.: University
0.572
0.495
0.584
0.493
(0.013)
Being Hispanic
0.848
0.359
0.819
0.385
(< 0.001)
Living in couple
0.508
0.500
0.590
0.492
(< 0.001)
Family size
2.936
1.470
3.027
1.557
(< 0.001)
Number of children
1.006
1.117
0.986
1.170
(0.246)
Full time worker
0.733
0.442
0.887
0.317
(< 0.001)
Self-employed worker
0.077
0.267
0.114
0.318
(< 0.001)
N. individuals
9,818
9,501
 
Note: The sample (ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) is restricted to individuals with episodes of paid work, unpaid work, childcare, and leisure, between 21 and 65 years old. Time uses are measured in minutes per day
Table A3
Ordered logit estimates
 
PAID WORK
UNPAID WORK
CHILDCARE
LEISURE
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
UNITED KINGDOM
        
With the spouse
-0.843
0.179
0.460**
0.174
-0.320
-0.271
0.466**
0.520***
 
(0.762)
(0.711)
(0.157)
(0.198)
(0.256)
(0.449)
(0.154)
(0.145)
 
[0.269]
[0.802]
[0.003]
[0.382]
[0.211]
[0.546]
[0.003]
[0.000]
With children
1.263*
-4.818***
0.143
0.161
-
-
0.141
0.352
 
(0.495)
(1.447)
(0.214)
(0.273)
  
(0.205)
(0.251)
 
[0.011]
[0.001]
[0.505]
[0.555]
  
[0.491]
[0.161]
With others (family unit)
-0.127
0.224
0.543***
-0.096
0.174
0.294
0.285
-0.002
 
(0.319)
(0.764)
(0.155)
(0.212)
(0.330)
(0.449)
(0.193)
(0.180)
 
[0.690]
[0.769]
[0.000]
[0.649]
[0.598]
[0.512]
[0.139]
[0.991]
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.518*
0.057
0.768***
0.614*
0.627
0.761
0.394*
0.557**
 
(0.210)
(0.221)
(0.184)
(0.260)
(0.356)
(0.720)
(0.184)
(0.175)
 
[0.013]
[0.797]
[0.000]
[0.018]
[0.078]
[0.291]
[0.033]
[0.001]
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
1,501
1,738
3,047
1,676
618
325
3,446
3,191
R-squared
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
Adjusted R-squared
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
F test p-value
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
AIC
4785.2
5908.7
10658.1
5670.7
1793.0
908.5
9228.6
9080.8
BIC
4923.3
6056.1
10826.7
5817.1
1903.6
1006.9
9400.7
9250.7
UNITED STATES
        
With the spouse
0.267
-0.461
0.302*
0.511***
0.281
0.512**
0.172
0.537***
 
(0.374)
(0.503)
(0.134)
(0.152)
(0.169)
(0.179)
(0.139)
(0.122)
 
[0.475]
[0.359]
[0.024]
[0.001]
[0.096]
[0.004]
[0.218]
[0.000]
With children
0.522
-0.117
0.404**
0.092
-
-
0.461***
0.527***
 
(0.277)
(0.270)
(0.137)
(0.175)
  
(0.115)
(0.123)
 
[0.060]
[0.665]
[0.003]
[0.599]
  
[0.000]
[0.000]
With others (family unit)
1.134
3.503**
0.155
0.342
0.483
-0.951
0.518*
0.423
 
(0.910)
(1.223)
(0.257)
(0.404)
(0.336)
(0.784)
(0.261)
(0.233)
 
[0.213]
[0.004]
[0.547]
[0.397]
[0.151]
[0.225]
[0.047]
[0.069]
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.183
0.068
0.561***
0.655*
0.484
0.510
0.540***
0.272*
 
(0.130)
(0.123)
(0.158)
(0.260)
(0.255)
(0.342)
(0.117)
(0.126)
 
[0.158]
[0.577]
[0.000]
[0.012]
[0.058]
[0.136]
[0.000]
[0.031]
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
2,432
3,112
4,187
2,271
1,778
1,140
5,347
5,950
R-squared
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
Adjusted R-squared
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
F test p-value
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
AIC
8266.3
10899.7
14193.7
7678.4
5099.7
2842.9
14895.5
17748.9
BIC
8422.8
11062.9
14364.9
7833.1
5242.2
2973.9
15073.3
17929.5
Note: The samples (UKTUS 2014–2015; ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) are restricted to market work, leisure, housework, and childcare episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. The dependent variable is the subjective enjoyment of episodes, which takes values from 1 (“not at all”) to 7 (“very much”), or the affective results of episodes, which take values from 0 (“not at all”) to 6 (“very much”). Robust standard errors, clustered at the individual level, in parentheses. T-test p-values in brackets. Additional coefficients are available upon request. *** Significant at the 99.9% level; ** significant at the 99% level; * significant at the 95% level
Table A4
Reduced sample estimates
 
PAID WORK
UNPAID WORK
CHILDCARE
LEISURE
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
UNITED KINGDOM
        
With the spouse
-0.251
0.241
0.355*
0.204
-0.115
-0.066
0.263**
0.308***
 
(0.442)
(0.457)
(0.145)
(0.150)
(0.132)
(0.224)
(0.094)
(0.087)
 
[0.572]
[0.598]
[0.015]
[0.176]
[0.385]
[0.768]
[0.005]
[0.000]
With children
1.003**
-3.538***
0.185
0.089
-
-
0.072
0.194
 
(0.330)
(0.761)
(0.176)
(0.212)
  
(0.125)
(0.158)
 
[0.003]
[0.000]
[0.295]
[0.674]
  
[0.566]
[0.222]
With others (family unit)
-0.050
0.220
0.459***
-0.040
0.292
0.107
0.032
-0.049
 
(0.210)
(0.431)
(0.129)
(0.156)
(0.201)
(0.312)
(0.151)
(0.104)
 
[0.812]
[0.611]
[0.000]
[0.798]
[0.150]
[0.732]
[0.832]
[0.637]
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.356*
0.015
0.649***
0.346
0.336
0.418
0.167
0.279**
 
(0.139)
(0.162)
(0.144)
(0.180)
(0.208)
(0.355)
(0.085)
(0.098)
 
[0.011]
[0.928]
[0.000]
[0.055]
[0.110]
[0.242]
[0.051]
[0.005]
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
1,342
1,521
2,831
1,533
527
295
3,253
3,002
R-squared
0.067
0.124
0.089
0.061
0.223
0.263
0.048
0.077
Adjusted R-squared
0.053
0.112
0.082
0.048
0.194
0.210
0.042
0.071
F test p-value
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
AIC
4359.6
5235.4
10407.3
5351.8
1652.7
909.3
11107.4
9766.9
BIC
4468.9
5347.3
10538.1
5469.2
1738.0
983.0
11241.4
9899.1
UNITED STATES
        
With the spouse
0.192
0.100
0.237*
0.408**
0.253*
0.345**
0.064
0.367***
 
(0.262)
(0.297)
(0.116)
(0.126)
(0.112)
(0.112)
(0.124)
(0.088)
 
[0.463]
[0.737]
[0.042]
[0.001]
[0.024]
[0.002]
[0.604]
[0.000]
With children
0.128
-0.370
0.377**
0.049
-
-
0.354***
0.349***
 
(0.230)
(0.237)
(0.119)
(0.157)
  
(0.086)
(0.079)
 
[0.579]
[0.118]
[0.002]
[0.755]
  
[0.000]
[0.000]
With others (family unit)
0.114
1.694***
0.111
0.038
0.433*
-0.575
0.354
0.284
 
(0.439)
(0.437)
(0.244)
(0.369)
(0.173)
(0.441)
(0.184)
(0.169)
 
[0.795]
[0.000]
[0.650]
[0.918]
[0.013]
[0.193]
[0.055]
[0.092]
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.091
-0.027
0.544***
0.452*
0.151
0.321
0.330***
0.193*
 
(0.118)
(0.124)
(0.144)
(0.210)
(0.182)
(0.248)
(0.089)
(0.089)
 
[0.442]
[0.827]
[0.000]
[0.031]
[0.408]
[0.197]
[0.000]
[0.030]
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
1,820
2,140
3,599
1,912
1,449
961
4,805
5,191
R-squared
0.073
0.037
0.063
0.075
0.085
0.096
0.063
0.062
Adjusted R-squared
0.062
0.028
0.057
0.064
0.072
0.077
0.059
0.059
F test p-value
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
AIC
6576.7
7962.6
13480.8
6926.4
4762.1
2982.3
16529.0
18001.0
BIC
6697.9
8087.3
13616.9
7048.6
4873.0
3084.5
16671.5
18145.2
Note: The samples (UKTUS 2014–2015; ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) are restricted to market work, leisure, housework, and childcare episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. The dependent variable is the subjective enjoyment of episodes, which takes values from 1 (“not at all”) to 7 (“very much”), or the affective results of episodes, which take values from 0 (“not at all”) to 6 (“very much”). Robust standard errors, clustered at the individual level, in parentheses. T-test p-values in brackets. Additional coefficients are available upon request. *** Significant at the 99.9% level; ** significant at the 99% level; * significant at the 95% level
Table A5
Pooled sample and gender score estimates
 
UNITED KINGDOM
UNITED STATES
Paid work
Unpaid work
Child- care
Leisure
Paid work
Unpaid work
Child- care
Leisure
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
UNITED KINGDOM
        
With the spouse
-0.870
0.348**
-0.089
0.250**
0.011
0.279**
0.205
0.109
 
(0.449)
(0.132)
(0.169)
(0.085)
(0.257)
(0.107)
(0.113)
(0.105)
 
[0.053]
[0.008]
[0.598]
[0.003]
[0.967]
[0.009]
[0.071]
[0.299]
With children
1.188***
0.161
-
0.188
0.510*
0.292**
-
0.378***
 
(0.300)
(0.167)
 
(0.126)
(0.198)
(0.107)
 
(0.075)
 
[0.000]
[0.335]
 
[0.136]
[0.010]
[0.006]
 
[0.000]
With others (family unit)
-0.199
0.361*
0.257
0.085
0.676
0.181
0.545***
0.238
 
(0.267)
(0.145)
(0.201)
(0.138)
(0.496)
(0.211)
(0.156)
(0.182)
 
[0.457]
[0.013]
[0.203]
[0.536]
[0.173]
[0.390]
[0.001]
[0.191]
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.398*
0.595***
0.383
0.261**
0.251*
0.578***
0.283
0.315***
 
(0.155)
(0.137)
(0.206)
(0.096)
(0.107)
(0.127)
(0.180)
(0.086)
 
[0.011]
[0.000]
[0.064]
[0.007]
[0.019]
[0.000]
[0.116]
[0.000]
Being male
-0.115
0.190
0.445
-0.233*
0.118
-0.120
0.277**
-0.203*
 
(0.201)
(0.122)
(0.260)
(0.095)
(0.116)
(0.102)
(0.105)
(0.095)
 
[0.570]
[0.118]
[0.088]
[0.015]
[0.310]
[0.240]
[0.008]
[0.032]
Being male X
        
With the spouse
1.104
-0.183
-0.156
0.089
-0.334
0.170
0.048
0.279*
 
(0.584)
(0.185)
(0.278)
(0.118)
(0.509)
(0.152)
(0.145)
(0.119)
 
[0.059]
[0.325]
[0.576]
[0.453]
[0.512]
[0.263]
[0.742]
[0.019]
With children
-4.460***
0.039
-
-0.068
-0.512
-0.150
-
-0.038
 
(0.794)
(0.256)
 
(0.200)
(0.290)
(0.168)
 
(0.100)
 
[0.000]
[0.878]
 
[0.733]
[0.078]
[0.374]
 
[0.706]
With others (family unit)
0.421
-0.304
-0.269
-0.136
1.121
0.123
-1.055*
0.069
 
(0.474)
(0.198)
(0.329)
(0.160)
(0.590)
(0.386)
(0.502)
(0.226)
 
[0.374]
[0.126]
[0.413]
[0.394]
[0.057]
[0.751]
[0.036]
[0.759]
With others (non-fam. unit)
-0.398
-0.263
0.057
-0.004
-0.220
-0.057
-0.129
-0.122
 
(0.213)
(0.230)
(0.402)
(0.137)
(0.136)
(0.205)
(0.258)
(0.115)
 
[0.062]
[0.252]
[0.888]
[0.979]
[0.106]
[0.781]
[0.616]
[0.287]
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
3,239
4,723
943
6,637
5,544
6,458
2,918
11,297
R-squared
0.093
0.064
0.175
0.068
0.040
0.055
0.079
0.066
Adjusted R-squared
0.086
0.058
0.153
0.065
0.035
0.051
0.071
0.064
F test p-value
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
AIC
11083.0
17127.8
3114.3
22200.4
20368.9
23982.0
9620.9
39023.4
BIC
11247.2
17302.3
3235.5
22384.0
20547.6
24164.9
9770.4
39221.3
Note: The samples (UKTUS 2014–2015; ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) are restricted to market work, leisure, housework, and childcare episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. The dependent variable is the subjective enjoyment of episodes, which takes values from 1 (“not at all”) to 7 (“very much”), or the affective results of episodes, which take values from 0 (“not at all”) to 6 (“very much”). Robust standard errors, clustered at the individual level, in parentheses. T-test p-values in brackets. Additional coefficients are available upon request. *** Significant at the 99.9% level; ** significant at the 99% level; * significant at the 95% level
Table A6
Estimates using standard errors
 
PAID WORK
UNPAID WORK
CHILDCARE
LEISURE
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
UNITED KINGDOM
        
With the spouse
-0.730**
0.211
0.346***
0.102
-0.176
-0.048
0.291***
0.290***
 
(0.282)
(0.246)
(0.068)
(0.084)
(0.116)
(0.147)
(0.057)
(0.053)
 
[0.010]
[0.391]
[0.000]
[0.225]
[0.130]
[0.744]
[0.000]
[0.000]
With children
0.994*
-3.457***
0.152
0.111
-
-
0.107
0.214*
 
(0.414)
(0.424)
(0.091)
(0.128)
  
(0.090)
(0.084)
 
[0.016]
[0.000]
[0.096]
[0.385]
  
[0.234]
[0.011]
With others (family unit)
-0.149
0.230
0.419***
-0.044
0.155
0.150
0.038
-0.010
 
(0.157)
(0.157)
(0.076)
(0.099)
(0.144)
(0.219)
(0.063)
(0.064)
 
[0.340]
[0.142]
[0.000]
[0.656]
[0.281]
[0.493]
[0.547]
[0.874]
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.358***
0.029
0.619***
0.389**
0.470*
0.490
0.217**
0.296***
 
(0.076)
(0.077)
(0.101)
(0.144)
(0.194)
(0.360)
(0.066)
(0.069)
 
[0.000]
[0.710]
[0.000]
[0.007]
[0.016]
[0.175]
[0.001]
[0.000]
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
1,501
1,738
3,047
1,676
618
325
3,446
3,191
R-squared
0.099
0.117
0.090
0.056
0.207
0.288
0.065
0.081
Adjusted R-squared
0.087
0.106
0.084
0.044
0.182
0.241
0.060
0.075
F test p-value
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
AIC
4919.2
6037.9
11196.9
5865.5
2049.7
1019.8
11741.2
10423.6
BIC
5030.8
6158.0
11329.4
5984.8
2138.2
1099.3
11876.4
10557.1
UNITED STATES
        
With the spouse
0.195
-0.417
0.267***
0.422***
0.184*
0.324***
0.113*
0.378***
 
(0.275)
(0.237)
(0.065)
(0.079)
(0.081)
(0.076)
(0.048)
(0.050)
 
[0.478]
[0.079]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.023]
[0.000]
[0.020]
[0.000]
With children
0.360
-0.006
0.338***
0.048
-
-
0.373***
0.361***
 
(0.267)
(0.355)
(0.068)
(0.104)
  
(0.050)
(0.060)
 
[0.178]
[0.986]
[0.000]
[0.643]
  
[0.000]
[0.000]
With others (family unit)
0.615
1.748
0.168
0.120
0.555***
-0.541**
0.333***
0.277**
 
(0.431)
(0.915)
(0.120)
(0.180)
(0.154)
(0.182)
(0.073)
(0.085)
 
[0.153]
[0.056]
[0.163]
[0.506]
[0.000]
[0.003]
[0.000]
[0.001]
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.144
0.102
0.553***
0.511***
0.258*
0.252
0.343***
0.170***
 
(0.084)
(0.072)
(0.097)
(0.130)
(0.104)
(0.154)
(0.047)
(0.045)
 
[0.086]
[0.157]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.013]
[0.102]
[0.000]
[0.000]
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
2,432
3,112
4,187
2,271
1,778
1,140
5,347
5,950
R-squared
0.086
0.029
0.056
0.076
0.083
0.084
0.063
0.067
Adjusted R-squared
0.078
0.022
0.052
0.068
0.073
0.068
0.060
0.064
F test p-value
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
< 0.001
AIC
8785.3
11528.1
15643.2
8300.4
6049.7
3551.1
18355.4
20644.3
BIC
8912.8
11661.0
15782.7
8426.4
6164.8
3657.0
18500.3
20791.5
Note: The samples (UKTUS 2014–2015; ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) are restricted to market work, leisure, housework, and childcare episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. The dependent variable is the subjective enjoyment of episodes, which takes values from 1 (“not at all”) to 7 (“very much”), or the affective results of episodes, which take values from 0 (“not at all”) to 6 (“very much”). Standard errors in parentheses. T-test p-values in brackets. Additional coefficients are available upon request. *** Significant at the 99.9% level; ** significant at the 99% level; * significant at the 95% level
Table A7
Size effects of main variables
 
PAID WORK
UNPAID WORK
CHILDCARE
LEISURE
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
UNITED KINGDOM
        
With the spouse
0.005
0.000
0.009
0.001
0.004
0.000
0.008
0.009
 
(0.045)
(0.003)
(0.096)
(0.016)
(0.018)
(0.001)
(0.115)
(0.116)
With children
0.004
0.037
0.001
0.001
-
-
0.004
0.002
 
(0.039)
(0.318)
(0.010)
(0.009)
-
-
(0.063)
(0.026)
With others (family unit)
0.001
0.001
0.010
0.000
0.002
0.002
0.000
0.000
 
(0.006)
(0.011)
(0.110)
(0.002)
(0.009)
(0.005)
(0.002)
(0.008)
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.015
0.000
0.012
0.004
0.010
0.006
0.003
0.006
 
(0.150)
(0.001)
(0.137)
(0.078)
(0.047)
(0.021)
(0.049)
(0.073)
Model size
0.099
0.117
0.090
0.056
0.207
0.288
0.065
0.081
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
1,501
1,738
3,047
1,676
618
325
3,446
3,191
UNITED STATES
        
With the spouse
0.000
0.001
0.004
0.013
0.003
0.016
0.001
0.010
 
(0.002)
(0.035)
(0.071)
(0.165)
(0.035)
(0.193)
(0.016)
(0.142)
With children
0.001
0.000
0.006
0.000
-
-
0.011
0.006
 
(0.009)
(0.009)
(0.107)
(0.001)
-
-
(0.166)
(0.093)
With others (family unit)
0.001
0.001
0.001
0.000
0.007
0.008
0.004
0.002
 
(0.009)
(0.042)
(0.009)
(0.003)
(0.089)
(0.093)
(0.062)
(0.027)
With others (non-fam. unit)
0.001
0.001
0.008
0.007
0.004
0.002
0.010
0.002
 
(0.014)
(0.021)
(0.137)
(0.089)
(0.042)
(0.029)
(0.155)
(0.036)
Model size
0.086
0.029
0.056
0.076
0.084
0.084
0.063
0.067
All other controls
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
2,432
3,112
4,187
2,271
1,778
1,140
5,347
5,950
Note: The samples (UKTUS 2014–2015; ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) are restricted to market work, leisure, housework, and childcare episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. The dependent variable is the subjective enjoyment of episodes, which takes values from 1 (“not at all”) to 7 (“very much”), or the affective results of episodes, which take values from 0 (“not at all”) to 6 (“very much”). Size effects (eta squared) computed from estimates in Table A8. Relative size effects (i.e., variable size effect, over model size) in parentheses
Table A8
Estimates of additional coefficients - United Kingdom
VARIABLES
PAID WORK
UNPAID WORK
CHILDCARE
LEISURE
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
Age
0.007
-0.011
-0.001
0.016*
-0.080***
-0.031
-0.003
0.001
 
(0.010)
(0.013)
(0.006)
(0.006)
(0.018)
(0.018)
(0.005)
(0.005)
 
[0.493]
[0.386]
[0.824]
[0.010]
[0.000]
[0.081]
[0.564]
[0.899]
Educ.: Secondary
0.232
0.134
0.528*
-0.291
-1.634**
-0.931
0.386
0.356
 
(0.334)
(0.662)
(0.267)
(0.459)
(0.528)
(0.837)
(0.304)
(0.282)
 
[0.487]
[0.840]
[0.049]
[0.526]
[0.002]
[0.269]
[0.206]
[0.208]
Educ.: University
0.255
0.182
0.503*
-0.263
-1.515***
-0.266
0.399
0.145
 
(0.285)
(0.658)
(0.251)
(0.455)
(0.431)
(0.794)
(0.286)
(0.283)
 
[0.371]
[0.782]
[0.046]
[0.563]
[0.001]
[0.739]
[0.164]
[0.609]
Being Hispanic
-0.057
-0.218
-0.300
0.101
0.904
-0.035
-0.204
0.189
 
(0.291)
(0.373)
(0.231)
(0.203)
(0.593)
(0.364)
(0.172)
(0.150)
 
[0.844]
[0.560]
[0.195]
[0.619]
[0.130]
[0.923]
[0.236]
[0.208]
Living in couple
-0.157
-0.320
-0.204
-0.414*
-0.638*
-2.172**
-0.330**
-0.268*
 
(0.209)
(0.271)
(0.157)
(0.177)
(0.247)
(0.678)
(0.112)
(0.126)
 
[0.453]
[0.240]
[0.194]
[0.020]
[0.011]
[0.002]
[0.003]
[0.033]
Family size
-0.154
0.152
-0.145*
0.092
0.360***
-0.286
0.016
-0.016
 
(0.091)
(0.095)
(0.067)
(0.074)
(0.105)
(0.206)
(0.052)
(0.050)
 
[0.092]
[0.110]
[0.032]
[0.214]
[0.001]
[0.167]
[0.761]
[0.754]
Number of children
0.111
0.066
-0.114
-0.137
-0.438**
-0.078
0.015
-0.093
 
(0.126)
(0.168)
(0.098)
(0.119)
(0.146)
(0.271)
(0.081)
(0.078)
 
[0.379]
[0.694]
[0.245]
[0.251]
[0.003]
[0.774]
[0.856]
[0.235]
Employment: self-employed
-
1.104
-0.672
-0.515
-
1.209
-0.629*
-0.415
  
(0.970)
(0.396)
(0.362)
 
(0.672)
(0.277)
(0.345)
  
[0.256]
[0.090]
[0.156]
 
[0.076]
[0.024]
[0.230]
Full time worker
-0.168
0.403
0.018
0.066
0.077
0.973*
0.132
0.202
 
(0.234)
(0.554)
(0.148)
(0.258)
(0.225)
(0.437)
(0.146)
(0.178)
 
[0.474]
[0.468]
[0.905]
[0.798]
[0.733]
[0.029]
[0.368]
[0.257]
Weekday
0.109
-0.100
0.061
0.203
-0.341
-0.136
-0.139
0.050
 
(0.201)
(0.251)
(0.138)
(0.163)
(0.217)
(0.236)
(0.148)
(0.140)
 
[0.589]
[0.690]
[0.656]
[0.214]
[0.119]
[0.566]
[0.351]
[0.720]
Log-Market work time
0.214
-0.085
-
-
-
-
-
-
 
(0.195)
(0.236)
      
 
[0.273]
[0.720]
      
Log-Housework time
-
-
-0.093
-0.047
-
-
-
-
   
(0.092)
(0.083)
    
   
[0.317]
[0.570]
    
Log-Childcare time
-
-
-
-
-0.055
0.359**
-
-
     
(0.125)
(0.126)
  
     
[0.657]
[0.006]
  
Log-Leisure time
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.201*
0.097
       
(0.079)
(0.106)
       
[0.011]
[0.360]
Constant
3.296*
4.930**
5.416***
4.209***
8.164***
7.350***
4.498***
4.407***
 
(1.300)
(1.654)
(0.589)
(0.688)
(1.466)
(1.197)
(0.556)
(0.870)
 
[0.012]
[0.003]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
Region F.E.
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
1,501
1,738
3,047
1,676
618
325
3,446
3,191
Note: The sample (UKTUS 2014–2015) is restricted to paid work, unpaid work, childcare and leisure episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. The dependent variable is the subjective enjoyment of episodes, which takes values from 1 (“not at all”) to 7 (“very much”). Robust standard errors, clustered at the individual level, in parentheses. T-test p-values in brackets. *** Significant at the 99.9% level; ** significant at the 99% level; * significant at the 95% level
Table A9
Estimates of additional coefficients - United States
VARIABLES
PAID WORK
UNPAID WORK
CHILDCARE
LEISURE
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
Women
Men
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
Age
0.014***
0.010*
0.001
0.007
-0.027***
-0.008
0.004
0.004
 
(0.004)
(0.004)
(0.005)
(0.005)
(0.007)
(0.007)
(0.003)
(0.003)
 
[0.000]
[0.013]
[0.840]
[0.136]
[0.000]
[0.257]
[0.207]
[0.269]
Educ.: Secondary
-0.314
-0.286
-0.643
-0.372
-0.742***
-0.390*
-0.058
-0.294
 
(0.446)
(0.319)
(0.441)
(0.263)
(0.207)
(0.163)
(0.281)
(0.202)
 
[0.482]
[0.370]
[0.145]
[0.157]
[0.000]
[0.017]
[0.837]
[0.146]
Educ.: University
-0.531
-0.472
-0.829
-0.484
-0.733***
-0.672***
-0.282
-0.379
 
(0.445)
(0.320)
(0.444)
(0.280)
(0.215)
(0.181)
(0.284)
(0.204)
 
[0.233]
[0.141]
[0.062]
[0.084]
[0.001]
[0.000]
[0.320]
[0.064]
Being Hispanic
0.171
-0.221*
-0.420***
-0.631***
-0.265
-0.276*
-0.034
-0.115
 
(0.129)
(0.111)
(0.123)
(0.157)
(0.141)
(0.138)
(0.111)
(0.100)
 
[0.188]
[0.047]
[0.001]
[0.000]
[0.061]
[0.046]
[0.757]
[0.249]
Living in couple
-0.052
0.006
0.000
-0.049
-0.078
0.040
0.056
0.049
 
(0.092)
(0.103)
(0.107)
(0.157)
(0.145)
(0.176)
(0.089)
(0.097)
 
[0.575]
[0.951]
[0.999]
[0.753]
[0.590]
[0.820]
[0.534]
[0.617]
Family size
0.091*
-0.026
-0.037
-0.015
-0.012
0.008
-0.102
-0.012
 
(0.046)
(0.053)
(0.061)
(0.091)
(0.072)
(0.142)
(0.065)
(0.056)
 
[0.047]
[0.623]
[0.545]
[0.867]
[0.864]
[0.953]
[0.119]
[0.831]
Number of children
0.053
0.040
-0.031
0.074
-0.055
0.015
0.094
-0.011
 
(0.063)
(0.071)
(0.083)
(0.106)
(0.084)
(0.157)
(0.080)
(0.068)
 
[0.398]
[0.576]
[0.705]
[0.485]
[0.512]
[0.924]
[0.236]
[0.871]
Employment: self-employed
0.387*
0.076
0.040
0.292
0.010
-0.178
0.257**
-0.155
 
(0.151)
(0.132)
(0.152)
(0.176)
(0.170)
(0.167)
(0.091)
(0.099)
 
[0.010]
[0.567]
[0.791]
[0.097]
[0.953]
[0.288]
[0.005]
[0.119]
Full time worker
-0.335**
0.027
0.165
-0.288
0.073
-0.139
0.124
-0.076
 
(0.119)
(0.154)
(0.110)
(0.183)
(0.116)
(0.231)
(0.089)
(0.122)
 
[0.005]
[0.860]
[0.132]
[0.116]
[0.528]
[0.548]
[0.166]
[0.531]
Weekday
-0.047
-0.046
-0.034
-0.102
0.024
0.081
-0.059
-0.106
 
(0.118)
(0.103)
(0.094)
(0.128)
(0.103)
(0.099)
(0.084)
(0.080)
 
[0.691]
[0.657]
[0.722]
[0.429]
[0.817]
[0.417]
[0.480]
[0.187]
Log-Market work time
-0.100
-0.099
-
-
-
-
-
-
 
(0.113)
(0.132)
      
 
[0.376]
[0.456]
      
Log-Housework time
-
-
-0.055
-0.005
-
-
-
-
   
(0.065)
(0.076)
    
   
[0.399]
[0.953]
    
Log-Childcare time
-
-
-
-
0.075
0.076
-
-
     
(0.073)
(0.064)
  
     
[0.305]
[0.233]
  
Log-Leisure time
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.136**
0.146**
       
(0.049)
(0.050)
       
[0.006]
[0.003]
Constant
4.514***
4.737***
5.350***
4.848***
5.877***
5.629***
3.876***
3.856***
 
(0.790)
(0.820)
(0.699)
(0.643)
(0.562)
(0.653)
(0.434)
(0.405)
 
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
[0.000]
Region F.E.
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Observations
2,432
3,112
4,187
2,271
1,778
1,140
5,347
5,950
Note: The sample (ATUS SWB module 2010-2012-2013) is restricted to paid work, unpaid work, childcare and leisure episodes of individuals between 21 and 65 years old. The dependent variable is the happiness scale of episodes, which takes values from 0 (“not at all”) to 6 (“very much”). Robust standard errors, clustered at the individual level, in parentheses. T-test p-values in brackets. *** Significant at the 99.9% level; ** significant at the 99% level; * significant at the 95% level
Fußnoten
1
Affective well-being refers to how individuals feel during an activity, and can be measured by specific positive and negative emotional experiences, such as enjoyment, happiness, joy, anxiety, sadness, stress, and anger (Kahneman & Deaton, 2010). A related concept, developed in the economic discipline, is the notion of experienced utility, which refers to the experience of feelings and emotions resulting from the outcome of a choice. When experienced utility is measured in real time it is known as instant utility (Kahneman et al., 1997). Kahneman et al., (2004) and Kahneman & Krueger (2006) provide a seminal contribution in which instant utility is derived from time use data.
 
2
Experienced utility, or instantaneous well-being, refers to the instant subjective feelings (enjoyment, or happiness) of individuals when doing a specific activity. Thus, these measures attempt to capture “the moment-to-moment flow of pleasure or pain” (Kahneman & Krueger, 2006). It is important to distinguish these measures from other measures of subjective well-being, such as cognitive measures based on overall, or average, life satisfaction or subjective well-being. It is important to note that positive instant feelings while doing certain activities do not necessarily mean higher general life satisfaction, or higher subjective well-being. See Fritjers (2022) for a recent review.
 
3
Our focus on the UK and the US is because, to the best of our knowledge, there are no datasets with information on time use and enjoyment/happiness for other countries in a similar period. Thus, we cannot generalize results to other economies, especially those from outside the Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic (WEIRD) world (Henrich et al., 2010).
 
4
Furthermore, some national surveys have updated their scope to cover behaviors linked to COVID-19 confinements, such as the ATUS and the Panel Study of Income Dynamics in the US, and the YouGov data in the UK.
 
5
The measurement of individual well-being dates back to authors such as Francis Ysidro Edgeworth and Alfred Marshall (Kahneman & Krueger, 2006), and refers to how a person believes their lives are going (Diener et al., 2018). Today, the concept of individual subjective well-being has evolved and there exist several measures and methods to measure it (see Fritjers (2022) for a recent review).
 
6
Affective well-being refers to how individuals feel during an activity, and can be measured by specific positive and negative emotional experiences, such as enjoyment, happiness, joy, anxiety, sadness, stress, and anger (Kahneman & Deaton, 2010). A related concept, developed in the economic discipline, is the notion of experienced utility, which refers to the experience of feelings and emotions resulting from the outcome of a choice. When experienced utility is measured in real time it is known as instant utility (Kahneman et al., 1997), Kahneman et al., (2004), and Kahneman & Krueger (2006) provide a seminal contribution in which instant utility is derived from time use data.
 
7
Both the UKTUS and the ATUS data are free access from the UK Data Service, and the Bureau of Labor Statistics, respectively. Data can also be freely downloaded from the Integrated Public Use Microdata Series (IPUMS) online system (https://​timeuse.​ipums.​org/​). The replication files for sample construction, final samples, and replication files to run the empirical analyses are available at https://​bit.​ly/​3sh8Air.
 
8
A detailed list of activities and classifications is available upon request.
 
9
See Sevilla et al., (2012) for a review of the different methods in the literature to assess feelings during time use activities.
 
10
All the statistics and results are computed using specific weights computed at the activity level.
 
11
It is beyond the scope of this manuscript to analyze gender differences in the average time devoted to the four time use categories, given that much prior research has documented gender differences in the uses of time. Results are available upon request.
 
12
We also run estimates including an interaction term between the vector of dummy variables \({{\rm{J}}_{{\rm{ij}}}}\), and the dummy that identifies episodes taking place at home, in order to capture whether there is an additional correlation between joint activities and the experienced satisfaction, if those activities take place at home, or elsewhere. These interactions were not statistically significant, indicating that such an additional effect is not relevant at standard levels. Interaction estimates are available upon request.
 
13
Among the socio-demographic characteristics included as explanatory variables, we include the (log) total time devoted to the activity of reference by the individual. Following Hamermesh (2020), the total time devoted to paid work is reduced by one third, which is spent in more leisure.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Adams-Prassl, A. (2020). The gender wage gap on an online labour market: The cost of interruptions. CEPR Discussion paper No. DP14294 Adams-Prassl, A. (2020). The gender wage gap on an online labour market: The cost of interruptions. CEPR Discussion paper No. DP14294
Zurück zum Zitat Boll, C., Müller, D., & Schüller, S. (2021). Neither backlash nor convergence: dynamics of intracouple childcare division after the first COVID-19 lockdown and subsequent reopening in Germany. CESifo Working Paper No. 9091 Boll, C., Müller, D., & Schüller, S. (2021). Neither backlash nor convergence: dynamics of intracouple childcare division after the first COVID-19 lockdown and subsequent reopening in Germany. CESifo Working Paper No. 9091
Zurück zum Zitat Brindal, E., Ryan, J. C., Kakoschke, N., Golley, S., Zajac, I. T., & Wiggins, B. (2021). Individual differences and changes in lifestyle behaviours predict decreased subjective well-being during COVID-19 restrictions in an Australian sample. Journal of Public Health, advanced online publication. https://doi.org/10.1093/pubmed/fdab040 Brindal, E., Ryan, J. C., Kakoschke, N., Golley, S., Zajac, I. T., & Wiggins, B. (2021). Individual differences and changes in lifestyle behaviours predict decreased subjective well-being during COVID-19 restrictions in an Australian sample. Journal of Public Health, advanced online publication. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1093/​pubmed/​fdab040
Zurück zum Zitat Cosaert, S., Theloudis, A., & Verheyden, B. (2022). Togetherness in the Household. American Economic Journal-Microeconomics, advanced online publication. https://www.aeaweb.org Cosaert, S., Theloudis, A., & Verheyden, B. (2022). Togetherness in the Household. American Economic Journal-Microeconomics, advanced online publication. https://​www.​aeaweb.​org
Zurück zum Zitat Farré, L., Fawaz, Y., González, L., & Graves, J. (2020). How the COVID-19 lockdown affected gender inequality in paid and unpaid work in Spain. IZA Discussion Paper No. 13434 Farré, L., Fawaz, Y., González, L., & Graves, J. (2020). How the COVID-19 lockdown affected gender inequality in paid and unpaid work in Spain. IZA Discussion Paper No. 13434
Zurück zum Zitat Fujiwara, D., Dolan, P., Lawton, R., Behzadnejad, F., Lagarde, A., Maxwell, C., & Peytrignet, S. (2020). Wellbeing costs of COVID-19 in the UK. Report to the World Health Organization Fujiwara, D., Dolan, P., Lawton, R., Behzadnejad, F., Lagarde, A., Maxwell, C., & Peytrignet, S. (2020). Wellbeing costs of COVID-19 in the UK. Report to the World Health Organization
Zurück zum Zitat Gershuny, J., & Halpin, B. (1996). Time use, quality of life and process benefits. In A. Offer (Ed.), Pursuit of the Quality of Life. Oxford: Clarendon Press Gershuny, J., & Halpin, B. (1996). Time use, quality of life and process benefits. In A. Offer (Ed.), Pursuit of the Quality of Life. Oxford: Clarendon Press
Zurück zum Zitat Helliwell, J. F., & Putnam, R. D. (2005). The social context of well-being. In F. Huppert, Beylis, N., & Keverne, B. (Eds), The Science of Well-Being, ch. 17. Oxford University Press Helliwell, J. F., & Putnam, R. D. (2005). The social context of well-being. In F. Huppert, Beylis, N., & Keverne, B. (Eds), The Science of Well-Being, ch. 17. Oxford University Press
Zurück zum Zitat James, G., Witten, D., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2013). An introduction to statistical learning. New York: springerCrossRef James, G., Witten, D., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2013). An introduction to statistical learning. New York: springerCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jenkins, S. P., & Osberg, L. (2004). Nobody to play with? The implications of leisure coordination. In D. Hamermesh, & G. Pfann (Eds.), The Economics of Time Use. Elsevier Jenkins, S. P., & Osberg, L. (2004). Nobody to play with? The implications of leisure coordination. In D. Hamermesh, & G. Pfann (Eds.), The Economics of Time Use. Elsevier
Zurück zum Zitat Juster, F. T., & Stafford, F. P. (1985). Time, Goods, and Well-Being. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research Juster, F. T., & Stafford, F. P. (1985). Time, Goods, and Well-Being. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research
Zurück zum Zitat Krueger, A. B. (2009). Measuring the Subjective Well-Being of Nations: National Accounts of Time Use and Well-Being. Chicago: University of Chicago Press/NBERCrossRef Krueger, A. B. (2009). Measuring the Subjective Well-Being of Nations: National Accounts of Time Use and Well-Being. Chicago: University of Chicago Press/NBERCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Szalai, A. (1972). The Use of Time. The Hague and Paris: Mouton Press Szalai, A. (1972). The Use of Time. The Hague and Paris: Mouton Press
Zurück zum Zitat Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070CrossRef Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yerkes, M. A., André, S. C., Besamusca, J. W., Kruyen, P. M., Remery, C. L., van der Zwan, R., & Geurts, S. A. (2020). ‘Intelligent’lockdown, intelligent effects? Results from a survey on gender (in) equality in paid work, the division of childcare and household work, and quality of life among parents in the Netherlands during the Covid-19 lockdown. PloS One, 15(11), e0242249. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242249CrossRef Yerkes, M. A., André, S. C., Besamusca, J. W., Kruyen, P. M., Remery, C. L., van der Zwan, R., & Geurts, S. A. (2020). ‘Intelligent’lockdown, intelligent effects? Results from a survey on gender (in) equality in paid work, the division of childcare and household work, and quality of life among parents in the Netherlands during the Covid-19 lockdown. PloS One, 15(11), e0242249. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0242249CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Should We Cheer Together? Gender Differences in Instantaneous Well-being: An Application to COVID-19 Lockdowns
verfasst von
José Ignacio Giménez-Nadal
José Alberto Molina
Jorge Velilla
Publikationsdatum
07.12.2022
Verlag
Springer Netherlands
Erschienen in
Journal of Happiness Studies / Ausgabe 2/2023
Print ISSN: 1389-4978
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-7780
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-022-00574-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2023

Journal of Happiness Studies 2/2023 Zur Ausgabe